Abstract

Based on its ability to increase brain complexity, a seemingly reliable index of conscious level, we propose testing the capacity of the classic psychedelic, psilocybin, to increase conscious awareness in patients with disorders of consciousness. We also confront the considerable ethical and practical challenges this proposal must address, if this hypothesis is to be directly assessed.

Introduction

Disorders of consciousness (DoC) are the most devastating form of impairment that may follow acquired brain injury. In contrast to comatose patients, those in the vegetative state (VS) and minimally conscious state (MCS) exhibit signs of wakefulness (eye opening). VS patients show no overt signs of awareness, whereas MCS patients show minimal but clearly discernible behavioural evidence of awareness. A range of therapies have been proposed for patients with DoC, including pharmacological (e.g. zolpidem, amantadine) (Gosseries et al. 2014), invasive- [e.g. deep brain stimulation (DBS) (Vanhoecke and Hariz 2017), vagal nerve stimulation (VNS) (Corazzol et al. 2017)] and non-invasive electrical stimulation [e.g. transcranial direct current stimulation (Thibaut et al. 2014)], and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) (Pistoia et al. 2013). However, no treatments have consistently shown beneficial effects on conscious awareness or functional recovery (Royal College of Physicians 2013; Giacino et al. 2014; Vanhoecke and Hariz 2017).

Classic psychedelics are currently undergoing significant investigation for the treatment of a range of psychiatric disorders (Carhart-Harris and Goodwin 2017). Here, we propose that the classic psychedelic, psilocybin, be explored as a treatment to increase conscious awareness in patients with DoC. A scientific rationale is proposed based on findings from research into the neurobiology of DoC and the effects of psychedelics. Developments in these hitherto separate fields of inquiry now suggest a potential therapeutic avenue, based on the twin discoveries that measures of brain complexity reliably index conscious level, and that brain complexity can be increased by psychedelics (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Brain complexity, consciousness and psychedelics. (A) Measures of brain complexity index conscious level. Empirical measures of brain complexity are high in the normal awake state and low whenever consciousness is lost. In the case of the perturbational complexity index (PCI Casali et al. 2013), a pulse of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) provides a cortical perturbation and the evoked electroencephalogram (EEG) responses are recorded. Alternatively, spontaneous EEG data alone are recorded. The Lempel-Ziv algorithm, a measure of compressibility, quantifies the complexity (LZC) in the thresholded EEG data (illustrated by black and white grids). LZC values robustly index conscious level. VS = vegetative state; MCS = minimally conscious state; EMCS = emergence from MCS; LIS = locked-in syndrome; LZC = Lempel-Ziv complexity; non-REM = non-rapid eye movement sleep; PCI = perturbational-complexity index. (B) Psychedelics increase brain complexity above normal levels. Classic psychedelics increase brain complexity measures like LZC above the levels in the normal awake state. This raises the possibility that psychedelics could increase conscious awareness in patients with disorders of consciousness, where brain complexity is low. LSD = lysergic acid diethylamide.

Brain Complexity and Consciousness

Complexity is a multifaceted concept that pervades many branches of the physical and life sciences. In the neurosciences, many theoretical accounts of consciousness have related the complexity of dynamics in a neural system to the manifestation of conscious experiences (Tononi et al. 1994; Edelman 2009; Ruffini 2017; Carhart-Harris 2018). One influential formulation has been that of neural complexity, proposed by Tononi and Edelman in 1994 (Tononi et al. 1994). This concept accounts for two fundamental features of consciousness, namely differentiation, the property that any particular experience is composed of many different components and is distinguishable from any other experience, and also integration, the property that any given conscious experience involves the integration of components into a unified whole. Importantly, neural complexity could, in principle, be calculated empirically, as the average mutual information—a measure of information sharing—between each subset and the rest of a system. Tononi and Edelman posited that during conscious awareness, ‘heterogeneous patterns of short-term correlations within the corticothalamic system will result in [high neural complexity]’ (Tononi et al. 1994).

Several theories of consciousness have since been advanced that emphasize a link between different formulations of complexity within brain activity and conscious level. Alongside these theoretical developments has been the introduction of a wide range of measures of dynamical complexity. These various measures reflect the diversity of definitions of complexity in use [for review, see Arsiwalla and Verschure (2018), Seth et al. (2006); see also Bassett and Gazzaniga (2011), Cocchi et al. (2017) for broader reviews in complex systems theory] and differ in the extent to which they directly capture the properties of differentiation versus integration, as well as temporal versus spatial complexity, and in their computational feasibility for large datasets.

Despite heterogenous definitions of complexity, a prediction shared by many theories of consciousness is that complexity should be high in the normal awake state and low whenever consciousness is lost, be it through anaesthesia, non-rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, or acquired brain injury. In the past two decades, a raft of empirical support for these predictions has emerged. Massimini and colleagues have provided striking evidence in favour of the principle via use of the so-called perturbational-complexity index (PCI). PCI quantifies the complexity of electroencephalogram (EEG) responses to pulses of TMS (Fig. 1A) (Casali et al. 2013). This perturbational approach has been likened to hitting a bell and measuring the complexity of the reverberations that follow. The PCI has been shown to robustly index conscious level across a range of states, including wakefulness (where the PCI is highest), sedation, non-REM sleep and anaesthesia. In patients with DoC, the PCI is lowest in VS patients, followed by patients in the MCS, then those emerged from MCS (denoted EMCS). In contrast, patients with locked-in syndrome, who have intact conscious awareness but cannot respond motorically, show PCI levels as high as healthy awake subjects (Casali et al. 2013).

At the heart of the PCI approach is quantification of the complexity of TMS-evoked EEG responses using an implementation of the Lempel-Ziv algorithm, a measure of compressibility which counts the number of unique patterns in a sequence, hence its everyday use in compressing large computer files (‘zipping’). Importantly, the Lempel-Ziv complexity (LZC) measure can also be applied to EEG recordings of spontaneous brain activity, i.e. without TMS perturbation. Whilst there are substantial differences between the spontaneous and perturbational approach, particularly that PCI evaluates only the complexity of deterministic responses of the cortex to TMS (Casali et al. 2013), the LZC of spontaneous EEG also effectively differentiates between conscious and unconscious states [including anaesthesia (Bai et al. 2015; Schartner et al. 2015) and sleep (Schartner et al. 2017b)]. In DoC, LZC-based values of spontaneous EEG reliably discriminate VS from MCS patients (Wu et al. 2011; Sitt et al. 2014) and values increase monotonically with patients’ conscious level (Sitt et al. 2014).

Our interpretation of these spontaneous EEG results is that LZC principally captures the variability or diversity of brain activity (i.e. differentiation rather than integration), and so behaves similarly to other measures of information entropy (i.e. capturing signal diversity over time). These related entropy-based metrics also appear to track conscious level [see Schartner et al. (2015), Carhart-Harris (2018) for further discussion]. Please see Schartner et al. (2015) and Mediano et al. (2019) for further discussion of these topics, and note that, for the sake of disambiguation, from here on, when we refer to ‘complexity’ we are referring to the ‘differentiation’ component in the original conception of ‘brain complexity’, i.e. the component that is measurable via LZC or a related entropy-based metric.

Psychedelics Increase Brain Complexity

Until recently, it was generally assumed that, in terms of states of consciousness, brain complexity would be maximal during normal wakefulness, since all other tested states of reduced consciousness (e.g. non-REM sleep, anaesthesia, DoC) feature correspondingly lower complexity values. It was therefore remarkable to discover that brain complexity values recorded during the psychedelic state exceed those found in normal waking consciousness (Fig. 1B). Specifically, in human subjects, increases in brain complexity (LZC) in excess of those seen in normal wakefulness were observed with psilocybin, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) and ketamine (at ‘psychedelic-like’ doses) (Schartner et al. 2017a). This finding has been replicated using a variety of complexity measures and measurement tools, including EEG, magnetoencephalography and functional MRI [see Carhart-Harris (2018) for review]. Furthermore, the magnitude of complexity increases correlated with the subjective intensity of the psychedelic experience (Schartner et al. 2017).

Increase Complexity, Increase Conscious Awareness?

Given that impairments in conscious awareness appear to closely relate to reductions in measures of brain complexity and psychedelics robustly increase brain complexity, could psychedelics elevate conscious awareness in patients with DoC?

Note that this hypothesis does not require that brain complexity be the cause of conscious awareness. Brain complexity per se may rather, in the terminology of Seth and Edelman (2009), be an explanatory correlate of the neural processes intimately related to conscious awareness. With this qualification in mind, a key question for our proposal is whether it is possible to increase measures of brain complexity without increasing conscious awareness. If it were possible, then this would negate our hypothesis and call into doubt the relationship between consciousness and brain complexity, at least as we define it here.

The classic psychedelic, psilocybin, is currently undergoing substantial clinical investment (Carhart-Harris and Goodwin 2017). Psilocybin is a prodrug of psilocin (4-hydroxy-dimethyltryptamine), whose principal psychoactive effects appear to be mediated by serotonin 2A (5-HT2A) receptor agonism. Psilocybin elevates measures of brain complexity in healthy humans (Schartner et al. 2017a; Carhart-Harris 2018) and many other lines of evidence support the idea that psilocybin could elevate conscious awareness in patients with DoC. The 5-HT2A receptors have their densest expression in the high-level cortical areas belonging to the default-mode network, which has been strongly implicated in conscious processing as well as the psychedelic state (Guldenmund et al. 2012; Beliveau et al. 2017; Carhart-Harris 2018). Most 5-HT2A receptors are expressed post-synaptically on layer 5 pyramidal neurons (Berthoux et al. 2018). These large, deep layer neurons are known to be key integration units in the cortex, and are the only cell type with dendrites spanning all cortical layers (Shai et al. 2015). In addition, presynaptic 5-HT2A receptors located at thalamo-cortical synapses have been shown to play an important role in the control of thalamo-frontal connectivity, also known to be important for consciousness (Giacino et al. 2014; Barre et al. 2016). 5-HT2A receptor agonism in animals is associated with enhanced cognitive flexibility as well as cortical neural plasticity (Frankel and Cunningham 2002; Boulougouris et al. 2008; Furr et al. 2012; Zhang and Stackman 2015; Ly et al. 2018; Olson 2018) whereas 5-HT2A receptor antagonism is associated with reduced cognitive flexibility and increased slow-wave sleep (Carhart-Harris and Nutt 2017). We recognize that one should be cautious when extrapolating from findings in animals to humans. However, there is some tentative evidence that cognitive flexibility is also enhanced in humans under psychedelics (Kuypers et al. 2016), although we would be hesitant to infer from this that psychedelics can enhance cognitive performance [see also Bayne and Carter (2018) and Carhart-Harris and Nutt (2017)].

Complexity, Conscious Content and Arousal

The standard conception of consciousness is that it encompasses two inter-related dimensions (Laureys et al. 2009; Boly et al. 2013): (i) the ‘content’ of consciousness, thought to be primarily related to cortical mechanisms, and (ii) wakefulness, or arousal, which subserves (i) and is controlled by the ascending activation systems of the brainstem and basal forebrain (i.e. the reticular activating system) (Boly et al. 2013). A key question is: how do these dimensions relate to measures of brain complexity like LZC?

Studies of impaired consciousness suggest that LZC and related measures of complexity chiefly index conscious content rather than arousal, e.g. as shown by the reductions in LZC that differentiate VS from MCS patients (Casali et al. 2013; Sitt et al. 2014) and non-REM from REM sleep (Abásolo et al. 2015). To our knowledge, there is no evidence that stimulant drugs, such as D-amphetamine or methylphenidate, which primarily increase arousal, increase brain complexity measures. In the case of psychedelics, our own experience is that arousal provides minimal explanatory value for describing the quality of the psychedelic experience. Moreover, we have argued that the evidence overwhelmingly suggests that psychedelic-related elevations in LZC or information entropy (to which LZC is closely and formally related) reflect an increased richness of conscious experience (Carhart-Harris 2018). Together, these observations suggest that targeting increases in conscious content, rather than (or perhaps in addition to) arousal, may be key to increasing conscious awareness in DoC patients.

Experiments comparing psychedelics with stimulant medications may help address the question of whether drugs presupposed (here) to increase conscious content (e.g. psilocybin) have more significant effects on brain complexity, and conscious content, than drugs that primarily promote arousal. Neurotransmitter systems implicated in the regulation and maintenance of arousal include noradrenaline, dopamine, acetylcholine, orexin, adenosine, histamine and 5-HT (Boutrel and Koob 2004; Ciurleo et al. 2013; Mura et al. 2014). Most classic stimulants act on catecholamines, and drugs such as D-amphetamine (Zhang et al. 2009), levodopa (Krimchansky et al. 2004) and modafinil (Dhamapurkar et al. 2017) have been used in DoC patients, with evidence of modest and variable clinical effects [see Ciurleo et al. (2013) and Mura et al. (2014) for review]. Our working hypothesis is that psilocybin is able to enhance conscious awareness to a greater extent than these stimulant-based alternatives.

The Relevance of a Multidimensional Conception of Consciousness

The current classification of DoC uses a taxonomy of states of consciousness ordered along a single scale, i.e. with EMCS patients having a higher level of consciousness than MCS patients who, in turn, have a higher level of consciousness than VS patients. However, recent challenges to this standard unidimensional construct of ‘levels of consciousness’ have been proposed (Bayne et al. 2016, 2017; Bayne and Carter 2018). These commentaries argue that the full range of consciousness-related capacities would be better classified using a graded, multidimensional space that captures, e.g. cognitive, sensory, affective and behavioural characteristics (Bayne et al. 2017). The same criticism has been levelled to applying the ‘levels of consciousness’ construct to all global states of consciousness—e.g. alert wakefulness, REM sleep, general anaesthesia, absence seizures and the psychedelic state—in that it fails to do justice to the evidently multifaceted nature of these states (Bayne et al. 2016; Bayne and Carter 2018). We are sympathetic to this view but also mindful of the pragmatic value of simple guiding principles in science. Thus, it remains to be seen how such a multidimensional framework, the details of which remain somewhat underspecified (Bayne et al. 2016), will align with the unidimensional complexity measures such as PCI and LZC that dominate empirical studies of states of consciousness and indeed current theories of consciousness (Baars 2005,;Tononi et al. 2016; Carhart-Harris 2018). As we acknowledged earlier, we see our proposal (to explore psychedelics as a treatment in DoC) as a challenge to the unidimensional conception of conscious level as indexed by brain complexity, in that to find a dissociation between complexity increases and conscious awareness would suggest important limitations to this simplistic framework.

An Ethical Hypothesis?

We believe that the evidence presented here suggests a strong scientific case for research exploring the hypothesis that psychedelics can increase conscious awareness in patients with DoC. However, stern ethical objections could supervene to prevent it from being tested. Ethical consideration of any interventional research in patients with DoC must grapple with dual opposing imperatives: on the one hand are concerns about risks of harm to patients lacking the capacity to consent; and on the other hand, is the principle that research must be done if we are ever to progress in our ability to improve the health of these patients. We agree with others who have argued that the inability of patients to consent doesn’t make research ethically illegitimate so long as it is ethically proportionate, a judgement that hinges on the accurate assessment of risks and benefits (Giacino et al. 2014; Fins et al. 2008).

Contrary to the alarmist campaigning that so negatively affected perceptions of psychedelics during and after the 1960s, plant-based psychedelics have been used for centuries for therapeutic purposes, and a recent resumption of clinical research with them has established conditions for their safe administration. Psilocybin has a particularly favourable safety profile, with a low toxicity and addiction potential (Passie et al. 2002; Carhart-Harris and Goodwin 2017). Evidence clearly indicates that, contrary to a popular misconception, psychedelics, when used with the relevant safeguards in place, are associated with positive rather than negative long-term mental health outcomes (Carhart-Harris and Goodwin 2017). There is now converging support for the safety and tolerability of psilocybin in a variety of psychiatric disorders [e.g. see Carhart-Harris and Goodwin (2017) for review]. The psychedelic effects of psilocybin are detectable 30–60 min after oral dosing (10–25 mg), peaking at 2–3h, and subsiding to negligible levels at least 6h post-dose (Carhart-Harris et al. 2016). Intravenous administration accelerates the onset into the domain of seconds and shortens the duration of the experience considerably (Turton et al. 2014).

Several experimental interventions in DoC patients have been invasive by comparison with what propose here. For example, the surgical implantation of DBS electrodes has been carried out for 50years, despite a lack of consistent evidence of benefits for improving conscious awareness (Vanhoecke and Hariz 2017). Recently, VNS implantation has been reported in a single case of a VS patient. Only modest behavioural improvements were observed when stimulation levels were titrated over a 6-month period (Corazzol et al. 2017).

A special ethical concern for neuromodulatory treatments such as DBS and VNS has been the possibility of a ‘self-awareness paradox’, whereby through an increase in conscious awareness, the patient experiences a concomitant increase in awareness of his/her clinical predicament and disability (Schiff et al. 2009). For psilocybin, treatment could conceivably also induce a transient unpleasant state of awareness, sometimes referred to colloquially (although not always accurately or helpfully) as a ‘bad trip’. It is difficult to gauge the likelihood or nature of either scenario in DoC patients given a psychedelic. A low baseline level of awareness might intuitively imply that unpleasant psychological phenomena will be both less likely and less severe than in fully aware subjects. Our experience is that such phenomena are rare in the investigational context, but more likely at higher doses and in settings lacking in psychological support.

Based on experience and accumulating evidence, there appear to be ways to mitigate the risks of difficult psychological experiences (Carhart-Harris et al. 2018). In our Phase 2 study in treatment-resistant depression, psychologically supported administration of oral psilocybin (10–25 mg) was well-tolerated by all patients, with the most common adverse events being mild transient anxiety just prior to as well as during drug onset (Carhart-Harris et al. 2016). In the case of DoC patients, one might expect anticipatory anxiety to be lower than in neurotypical individuals and the risk of anxiety could be further reduced by careful attention to the environment and ensuring familiar carers are at hand during dosing.

Future Horizons

The modest outcomes from previous interventional studies in patients with DoC should temper optimism that psilocybin could bring clinically meaningful benefits, particularly in cases of extensive neuronal loss, e.g. a sufficient degree of functional neuronal architecture may need to be in place for psychedelics to elicit a functionally meaningful effect. However, we believe that pragmatics and need, supported by sound theory and evidence, as well as proper consideration of ethics and care, should dictate how to proceed.

One potential starting point might be to test the idea using animal models, but we are doubtful of its translational value to our hypothesis. While animal studies have informed our understanding of the neural circuitry involved in information processing in general (Boly et al. 2013), it is less evident that any existing animal model of acquired brain injury (i.e. either severe traumatic brain injury or adult hypoxic ischaemic injury) holds relevance for the goal of understanding the recovery of conscious awareness in human individuals with DoC. An alternative to injury models would be to test in sedated animals whether psilocybin increases measures of brain complexity from a baseline of sedative-induced reduced complexity. Intriguingly, evidence of this kind can be found in the literature from the 1950s, with a report that LSD reverses the sedating effects of anaesthetic doses of barbiturates in cats (Apter 1958).

Given the difficulties in assessing consciousness in non-human animals, an advance on this would be to carry out the experiment in sedated healthy human volunteers, measuring complexity with scalp EEG and either LZC on spontaneous EEG signals or using PCI, accompanied by repeated behavioural measurements of consciousness, before and after psilocybin administration. Moreover, by combining spontaneous EEG/LZC and PCI measures within the same sample, one could potentially gain insight into their inter-relatedness or indeed separation and differential relevance for conscious awareness.

Whilst there are fundamental difficulties in extrapolating findings from sedated volunteers to patients with DoC, positive findings would support the case for a study of psilocybin in DoC patients. A related experiment in humans could be carried out in sleep, testing the hypothesis that psychedelics increase complexity and conscious level in non-REM sleep, perhaps by promoting REM sleep, evidence for which can also be found in the historical literature (Muzio et al. 1966; Torda 1968). Experiments comparing psilocybin with stimulant medications would help answer the question of whether drugs presupposed to increase conscious content (e.g. psilocybin) have more significant effects on brain complexity and conscious awareness than drugs that more specifically promote arousal.

Experimental Considerations

Assuming the scientific, ethical and regulatory case can be won for the testing of our hypothesis, we suggest some principles for the design of preliminary studies of psilocybin in patients with DoC, based on our experience of psychedelic experimental research.

We would advocate an incremental and adaptive approach, where the first steps would be to establish safety and tolerability and examine the signal changes of interest (i.e. changes in LZC in spontaneous EEG activity). This step-wise procedure would then be followed by a focus on optimizing the dosage parameters, and measuring and searching for the desired behavioural effects, i.e. an observable increase in conscious awareness, while maintaining good tolerability.

We expect that the patient inclusion criteria and recruitment protocol would be similar to previous early-phase pharmacology studies in this population. Exclusion criteria should include a history of psychotic disorder, as is typical with psychedelic research, and an abnormal resting electrocardiogram. Serotonergic antidepressants have been found to downregulate the 5-HT2A receptor, and attenuated responses to psychedelics have previously been reported in individuals chronically medicated with serotonergic antidepressants (Bonson et al. 1996). We would therefore exclude any patients receiving these drugs or request controlled washout from these medications, for which we have a working protocol.

As outlined above, we would first aim to test the hypothesis that psilocybin increases measures of brain complexity. To do this safely, we would use a dose-escalation design, using low doses to assess tolerability before moving to a higher, potentially therapeutic dose range. Doses in the range of 25–40 mg are used clinically and have been found to induce profound, existentially ‘transformative’ experiences in both healthy and clinical populations. The physiological safety of 30mg/70kg psilocybin has been well-demonstrated in healthy volunteers (Griffiths et al. 2006). We would therefore aim to reach such doses, particularly as it is possible DoC patients have reduced sensitivity to the effects of psilocybin. Although it would add complication to procedures, 5-HT2A receptor positron emission tomography could allow this assumption to be tested empirically (Kumar and Mann 2014).

We would, as a minimum, record continuous scalp EEG before, during and after dosing, and calculate LZC measures of brain complexity offline. If practicable, it would be beneficial to additionally use the PCI (i.e. EEG combined with TMS) (Casali et al. 2013). Alongside neurophysiological measures, we would also record repeated cardiorespiratory observations (heart rate, blood pressure, respiratory rate) and carefully observe the participant for signs of psychological distress and increased sympathetic nervous system activity. For an early-phase study, although behavioural endpoints would not be of primary interest, standardized assessments would be incorporated where this is feasible (i.e. the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised and/or Wessex Head Injury Matrix).

In subsequent analysis, it would be of interest to explore the relationship between pre- and post-dose EEG complexity and behavioural measures. Given the evidence that measures derived from EEG connectivity in patients with DoC could not only prognosticate recovery (Sitt et al. 2014; Chennu et al. 2017), but also predict response to intervention (Thibaut et al. 2018), it is possible that pre-dosage EEG measures could be used in a similar way in relation to psychedelic treatment.

We recommend detailed consideration of the choice and setup of the environment and the support provided to the patient during study sessions, although we acknowledge it will be difficult to know how a DoC patient could be prepared for the expected effects of the drug. The sessions themselves would be carried out in a familiar environment for the patient, with their normal carers present. In our study of treatment-resistant depression, psychological support was provided before, during and after each session. There is evidence to suggest that control of context is important for positive therapeutic responses (Carhart-Harris et al. 2018). The timing of dosing in relation to markers of the circadian rhythm, which is likely to influence arousal, should also be factored in (Blume et al. 2017).

In later studies of psilocybin as a treatment for patients with DoC, behavioural indices like the Coma Recovery Scale - Revised (CRS-R) must ultimately be used as the primary outcome measure. In this sense, we would wish to treat psilocybin no differently than any other experimental intervention for DoC. At present, we view brain complexity as an informative index of conscious content, but for this to be functionally relevant in the context of psychedelic interventions in DoC, it would be vital to establish that a psychedelic-induced increase in complexity ultimately translates into behavioural improvements, with the important caveat that it may be necessary in fact to test for covert awareness, i.e. identifying consciousness in the absence of any behavioural response, e.g. by using covert command-following paradigms (Owen et al. 2006).

The growing evidence for pro-plasticity effects via 5-HT2A receptor agonism suggests that later placebo-controlled studies could be designed to test whether a given dose regimen may enhance standard rehabilitative care, with the aim of augmenting its effectiveness. Here, a ‘micro-dosing’ strategy could be considered, similar to that adopted by many current studies, whereby threshold perceptible doses of psychedelics are given two to three times per week, and acute psychological ‘side effects’, such as anxiety, may be minimal. Thus, a micro-dosing protocol would put more focus on drug-assisted rehabilitative care rather than neuropharmacology alone, which would be consistent with the therapeutic model currently being employed in psychiatric contexts.

Overall, we propose an explorative, adaptive approach (within specified boundaries, and with an emphasis on caution) for this research, as so much is uncertain, including of course, positive eventualities.

Conclusion

Disorders of consciousness present unique prospects for fundamental scientific discovery and major clinical breakthroughs but also significant ethical and pragmatic challenges. The nascent renaissance in psychedelic research has shined a light on the study of consciousness, revealing anomalous positive effects on the complexity of brain activity, the low values of which have come to define states of impaired consciousness. The modern era of responsible scientific experimentation with psychedelics is yielding significant support for their safety across a range of conditions. Taken together, we call for an open-minded attitude about the possibility of exploring the potential of psychedelics to elevate conscious awareness in patients with DoC.

Authors’ Contributions

G.S.: conceptualization, drafting of and revision of the manuscript; R.L.C.-H.: conceptualization and revision of the manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We thank Prof Diane Playford, Dr Sophie Duport and the staff at the Royal Hospital for Neurodisability (RHN), who provided valuable feedback on our proposal in June 2018. We also thank Prof Rob Leech, Dr Fernando Rosas, Dr Leor Roseman and Dr Edward Presswood for their comments on a draft of the manuscript. We are very grateful to the anonymous referees for their comments. G.S. is funded by a National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Clinical Lectureship. R.L.C.-H. is funded by the Alex Mosley Trust, Tim Ferriss, Anton Bilton, the Singhal Health Foundation and the Ad Astria Chandaria Foundation.

Conflict of interest statement. G.S. has nothing to report. R.L.C.-H. is currently advising Compass Pathways, a commercial initiative to develop psilocybin as a medicine.

References

Abásolo
D
,
Simons
S
,
da Silva
R
, et al. 
Lempel-Ziv complexity of cortical activity during sleep and waking in rats
.
J Neurophysiol
2015
;
113
:
2742
52
.

Apter
JT.
Analeplic action of lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD-25) against pentobarbital
.
Arch Neurol Psychiatry
1958
;
79
:
711
5
.

Arsiwalla
XD
,
Verschure
P.
Measuring the Complexity of Consciousness
.
Front Neurosci
2018
;
12
:
424

Baars
BJ.
Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience
.
Prog Brain Res
2005
;
150
:
45
53
.

Bai
Y
,
Liang
Z
,
Li
X.
A permutation Lempel-Ziv complexity measure for EEG analysis [Internet]
.
Biomed Signal Process Control
2015
;
19
:
102
14
.

Barre
A
,
Berthoux
C
,
De Bundel
D
, et al. 
Presynaptic serotonin 2A receptors modulate thalamocortical plasticity and associative learning
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
2016
;
113
:
E1382
91
.

Bassett
DS
,
Gazzaniga
MS.
Understanding complexity in the human brain
.
Trends Cogn Sci
2011
;
15
:
200
9
.

Bayne
T
,
Carter
O.
Dimensions of consciousness and the psychedelic state
.
Neurosci Conscious
2018
;
2018
:
niy008.

Bayne
T
,
Hohwy
J
,
Owen
AM.
Are there levels of consciousness? [Internet]
.
Trends Cogn Sci
2016
;
20
:
405
13
.

Bayne
T
,
Hohwy
J
,
Owen
AM.
Reforming the taxonomy in disorders of consciousness
.
Ann Neurol
2017
;
82
:
866
72
.

Beliveau
V
,
Ganz
M
,
Feng
L
, et al. 
A high-resolution in vivo atlas of the human brain’s serotonin system
.
J Neurosci
2017
;
37
:
120
8
.

Berthoux
C
,
Barre
A
,
Bockaert
J
, et al.  Sustained activation of postsynaptic 5-HT2A receptors gates plasticity at prefrontal cortex synapses. Cereb Cortex
2018
;
29
:
1659
69
.

Blume
C
,
Lechinger
J
,
Santhi
N
, et al. 
Significance of circadian rhythms in severely brain-injured patients
.
Neurology
2017
;
88
:
1933
41
.

Boly
M
,
Seth
A
,
Wilke
M
, et al. 
Consciousness in humans and non-human animals: recent advances and future directions
.
Front Psychol
2013
;
4
:
625.

Bonson
KR
,
Buckholtz
JW
,
Murphy
DL.
Chronic administration of serotonergic antidepressants attenuates the subjective effects of LSD in humans
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
1996
;
14
:
425
36
.

Boulougouris
V
,
Glennon
JC
,
Robbins
TW.
Dissociable effects of selective 5-HT 2A and 5-HT 2C receptor antagonists on serial spatial reversal learning in rats
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
2008
;
33
:
2007.

Boutrel
B
,
Koob
GF.
What keeps us awake: the neuropharmacology of stimulants and wakefulness promoting medications
.
Sleep
2004
;
27
:
1181
94
.

Carhart-Harris
RL.
The entropic brain—revisited
.
Neuropharmacology
2018
;
142
:
167
178
.

Carhart-Harris
RL
,
Bolstridge
M
,
Rucker
J
, et al. 
Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: an open-label feasibility study
.
Lancet Psychiatry
2016
;
3
:
619
27
.

Carhart-Harris
RL
,
Goodwin
GM.
The therapeutic potential of psychedelic drugs: past, present, and future
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
2017
;
42
:
2105.

Carhart-Harris
RL
,
Nutt
DJ.
Serotonin and brain function: a tale of two receptors
.
J. Psychopharmacol
2017
;
31
:
1091
120
.

Carhart-Harris
RL
,
Roseman
L
,
Haijen
E
, et al. 
Psychedelics and the essential importance of context
.
J Psychopharmacol
2018
;
32
:
725
731
.

Casali
AG
,
Gosseries
O
,
Rosanova
M
, et al. 
A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior
.
Sci Transl Med
2013
;
5
:198ra105.

Chennu
S
,
Annen
J
,
Wannez
S
, et al. 
Brain networks predict metabolism, diagnosis and prognosis at the bedside in disorders of consciousness
.
Brain
2017
;
140
:
2120
32
.

Ciurleo
R
,
Bramanti
P
,
Calabrò
RS.
Pharmacotherapy for disorders of consciousness: are awakening’drugs really a possibility?
Drugs
2013
;
73
:
1849
62
.

Cocchi
L
,
Gollo
LL
,
Zalesky
A
, et al. 
Criticality in the brain: a synthesis of neurobiology, models and cognition
.
Prog Neurobiol
2017
;
158
:
132
52
.

Corazzol
M
,
Lio
G
,
Lefevre
A
, et al. 
Restoring consciousness with vagus nerve stimulation
.
Curr Biol
2017
;
27
:
R994
6
.

Dhamapurkar
SK
,
Wilson
BA
,
Rose
A
, et al. 
Does Modafinil improve the level of consciousness for people with a prolonged disorder of consciousness? A retrospective pilot study
.
Disabil Rehabil
2017
;
39
:
2633
9
.

Fins
JJ
,
Illes
J
,
Bernat
JL
, et al. 
Neuroimaging and disorders of consciousness: envisioning an ethical research agenda
.
Am J Bioethics
2008
;
8
:
3
12
.

Frankel
PS
,
Cunningham
KA.
The hallucinogen d-lysergic acid diethylamide (d-LSD) induces the immediate-early gene c-Fos in rat forebrain
.
Brain Res
2002
;
958
:
251
60
.

Furr
A
,
Lapiz-Bluhm
MD
,
Morilak
DA.
5-HT2A receptors in the orbitofrontal cortex facilitate reversal learning and contribute to the beneficial cognitive effects of chronic citalopram treatment in rats
.
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol
2012
;
15
:
1295
305
.

Giacino
JT
,
Fins
JJ
,
Laureys
S
, et al. 
Disorders of consciousness after acquired brain injury: the state of the science
.
Nat Rev Neurol
2014
;
10
:
99
114
.

Gosseries
O
,
Charland-Verville
V
,
Thonnard
M
, et al. 
Amantadine, apomorphine and zolpidem in the treatment of disorders of consciousness
.
Curr Pharm Des
2014
;
20
:
4167
84
.

Griffiths
RR
,
Richards
WA
,
McCann
U
, et al. 
Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
2006
;
187
:
268
83
.

Guldenmund
JP
,
Vanhaudenhuyse
A
,
Boly
M
, et al. 
A default mode of brain function in altered states of consciousness
.
Arch Ital Biol
2012
;
150
:
107
21
.

Krimchansky
B-Z
,
Keren
O
,
Sazbon
L
, et al. 
Differential time and related appearance of signs, indicating improvement in the state of consciousness in vegetative state traumatic brain injury (VS-TBI) patients after initiation of dopamine treatment
.
Brain Inj
2004
;
18
:
1099
105
.

Kumar
JSD
,
Mann
JJ.
PET tracers for serotonin receptors and their applications
.
Cent Nerv Syst Agents Med Chem
2014
;
14
:
96
112
.

Kuypers
KPC
,
Riba
J
,
de la Fuente Revenga
M
, et al. 
Ayahuasca enhances creative divergent thinking while decreasing conventional convergent thinking
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
2016
;
233
:
3395
403
.

Laureys
S
,
Boly
M
,
Moonen
G
, et al.  Two dimensions of consciousness: arousal and awareness.
Encyclopedia of Neuroscience
2009
;
2
:
1133
42
.

Ly
C
,
Greb
AC
,
Cameron
LP
, et al. 
Psychedelics promote structural and functional neural plasticity
.
Cell Rep
2018
;
23
:
3170
82
.

Mediano
AP
,
Seth
KA
,
Barrett
BA.
Measuring integrated information: comparison of candidate measures in theory and simulation
.
Entropy
2019
;
21
:
17
.

Mura
E
,
Pistoia
F
,
Sara
M
, et al. 
Pharmacological modulation of the state of awareness in patients with disorders of consciousness: an overview
.
Curr Pharm Des
2014
;
20
:
4121
39
.

Muzio
JN
,
Roffwarg
HP
,
Kaufman
E.
Alterations in the nocturnal sleep cycle resulting from LSD
.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
1966
;
21
:
313
24
.

Olson
DE.
Psychoplastogens: a promising class of plasticity-promoting neurotherapeutics
.
J Exp Neurosci
2018
;
12
:1179069518800508.

Owen
AM
,
Coleman
MR
,
Boly
M
, et al. 
Detecting awareness in the vegetative state
.
Science
2006
;
313
:
1402.

Passie
T
,
Seifert
J
,
Schneider
U
, et al. 
The pharmacology of psilocybin
.
Addict Biol
2002
;
7
:
357
64
.

Pistoia
F
,
Sacco
S
,
Carolei
A
, et al. 
Corticomotor facilitation in vegetative state: results of a pilot study
.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
2013
;
94
:
1599
606
.

Royal College of Physicians.

Prolonged disorders of consciousness: National clinical guidelines
.
London
:
RCP
,
2013
.

Ruffini
G.
An algorithmic information theory of consciousness
.
Neurosci Conscious
2017
;
2017
:nix019.

Schartner
M
,
Seth
A
,
Noirhomme
Q
, et al. 
Complexity of multi-dimensional spontaneous EEG decreases during propofol induced general anaesthesia
.
PLoS One
2015
;
10
:
1
21
.

Schartner
MM
,
Carhart-Harris
RL
,
Barrett
AB
, et al. 
Increased spontaneous MEG signal diversity for psychoactive doses of ketamine, LSD and psilocybin
.
Sci Rep
2017a
;
7
:46421.

Schartner
MM
,
Pigorini
A
,
Gibbs
SA
, et al.  Global and local complexity of intracranial EEG decreases during NREM sleep [Internet]. Neurosci. Conscious
2017b
;2017:niw022.

Schiff
N
,
Giacino
J
,
Fins
J.
Deep brain stimulation, neuroethics, and the minimally conscious state: moving beyond proof of principle
.
Arch Neurol
2009
;
66
:
697
702
.

Seth
AK
,
Edelman
GM
, Consciousness and complexity. In:
Meyers
RA
, editor.
Encyclopedia of Complexity and Systems Science
.
New York, NY
:
Springer-Verlag
,
2009
.

Seth
AK
,
Izhikevich
E
,
Reeke
GN
, et al. 
Theories and measures of consciousness: an extended framework
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
2006
;
103
:
10799
804
.

Shai
AS
,
Anastassiou
CA
,
Larkum
ME
, et al. 
Physiology of layer 5 pyramidal neurons in mouse primary visual cortex: coincidence detection through bursting
.
PLoS Comput Biol
2015
;
11
:
e1004090.

Sitt
JD
,
King
JR
,
El Karoui
I
, et al. 
Large scale screening of neural signatures of consciousness in patients in a vegetative or minimally conscious state
.
Brain
2014
;
137
:
2258
70
.

Thibaut
A
,
Bruno
M-A
,
Ledoux
D
, et al. 
tDCS in patients with disorders of consciousness: sham-controlled randomized double-blind study
.
Neurology
2014
;
82
:
1112
8
.

Thibaut
A
,
Chennu
S
,
Chatelle
C
, et al. 
Theta network centrality correlates with tDCS response in disorders of consciousness
.
Brain Stimul
2018
;
11
:
1407
9
.

Tononi
G
,
Boly
M
,
Massimini
M
, et al. 
Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
2016
;
17
:
450.

Tononi
G
,
Sporns
O
,
Edelman
GM.
A measure for brain complexity: relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci
1994
;
91
:
5033
7
.

Torda
C.
Contribution to serotonin theory of dreaming (LSD infusion)
.
N Y State J Med
1968
;
68
:
1135
8
.

Turton
S
,
Nutt
DJ
,
Carhart-Harris
RL.
A qualitative report on the subjective experience of intravenous psilocybin administered in an FMRI environment
.
Curr Drug Abuse Rev
2014
;
7
:
117
27
.

Vanhoecke
J
,
Hariz
M.
Deep brain stimulation for disorders of consciousness: systematic review of cases and ethics
.
Brain Stimul
2017
;
10
:
1013
23
.

Wu
D-Y
,
Cai
G
,
Yuan
Y
, et al. 
Application of nonlinear dynamics analysis in assessing unconsciousness: a preliminary study
.
Clin Neurophysiol
2011
;
122
:
490
8
.

Zhang
G
,
Stackman
RW.
The role of serotonin 5-HT(2A) receptors in memory and cognition
.
Front Pharmacol
2015
;
6
:
225.

Zhang
W
,
Doherty
M
,
Leeb
BF
, et al. 
EULAR evidence-based recommendations for the diagnosis of hand osteoarthritis: report of a task force of ESCISIT [Internet]
.
Ann Rheum Dis
2009
;
68
:
8
17
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.