Abstract

Background

most older individuals who experience mobility decline, also show cognitive decline, but whether cognitive decline precedes or follows mobility limitation is not well understood.

Objective

examine the temporal sequence of mobility and cognition among initially unimpaired older adults.

Methods

mobility and cognition were assessed every 2 years for 6 years in 412 participants aged ≥60 with initially unimpaired cognition and gait speed. Using autoregressive models, accounting for the dependent variable from the prior assessment, baseline age, sex, body mass index and education, we examine the temporal sequence of change in mobility (6 m usual gait speed, 400 m fast walk time) and executive function (visuoperceptual speed: Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST); cognitive flexibility: Trail Making Test part B (TMT-B)) or memory (California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) immediate, short-delay, long-delay).

Results

there was a bidirectional relationship over time between slower usual gait speed and both poorer DSST and TMT-B scores (Bonferroni-corrected P < 0.005). In contrast, slower 400 m fast walk time predicted subsequent poorer DSST, TMT-B, CVLT immediate recall and CVLT short-delay scores (P < 0.005), while these measures did not predict subsequent 400 m fast walk time (P > 0.005).

Conclusions

among initially unimpaired older adults, the temporal relationship between usual gait speed and executive function is bidirectional, with each predicting change in the other, while poor fast walking performance predicts future executive function and memory changes but not vice versa. Challenging tasks like the 400 m walk appear superior to usual gait speed for predicting executive function and memory change in unimpaired older adults.

Introduction

Cognitive and mobility deficits are major contributors to late life disability, and are often related. Based on neuroimaging studies, age-related alterations in gait and cognitive function are associated with shared underlying structural changes in the prefrontal and medial temporal lobes, as well as increased burden of cerebrovascular lesions [1]. However, the relative temporal sequence of age-related changes in mobility and cognition remains controversial. If we were able to determine which measures of mobility or cognition change first during ageing, we might better focus our screening and intervention efforts. Knowledge about the sequence of events might also provide new insights into underlying causal pathways.

Cross-sectionally, slow gait is consistently associated with poor cognition, especially with executive function [2]. Unidirectional longitudinal studies suggest that poorer mobility is associated with greater declines in global cognitive function and executive function, as well as risk of cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease and dementia, and that poorer cognitive function, particularly global cognitive function and executive function, predicts a greater decline in mobility [3, 4].

Studies examining co-occurring longitudinal relationships between mobility and cognition have shown conflicting results. Three examined rates of change; two reported co-occurring changes [5, 6] while one found no relationship [7]. Among those that examined bidirectional relationships, some reported cognitive performance predicts mobility decline while mobility function does not predict cognitive decline [8, 9], while others suggested that mobility decline predicts cognitive decline but not vice versa [9, 10] and two reported bidirectional longitudinal relationships [11, 12]. These mixed results may be due to factors such as characteristics of participants, duration of follow-up and/or types of measures of mobility and cognition.

Here, we attempt to determine the sequence of cognitive and mobility changes over time by starting with an older adult sample without evidence of cognitive or mobility impairment. We measure change over multiple repeated visits, evaluate measures of mobility at usual and fast walking speed, and include multiple measures of cognition within two domains; (executive function and memory).

Methods

Study population

Participants were drawn from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA), an ongoing prospective study of adults aged 20 and older [13]. Participants undergo testing at the National Institute on Aging's clinical research unit.

Because we wanted to focus on temporal sequences, we sought to create an initially unimpaired sample. Eligibility included aged 60 or older, initially unimpaired mobility and cognition, and repeated assessments of mobility and cognition at subsequent follow-up (details in Supplementary data, Appendix 1, available in Age and Ageing online). We defined unimpaired mobility as a usual gait speed of >0.8 meters/second and unimpaired cognitive function as score of >24 on the 30-point Mini-Mental State Exam [14], with no adjudicated cognitive impairment or dementia. Of 901 participants aged 60 and older who had concurrent assessments of the measures of interest at Time 1, 812 had unimpaired mobility and cognition, and 412 had repeated assessments 2 years later at Time 2. This study included these 412, of which 220 had data at 4 years (Time 3) and 140 had data at 6 years (Time 4). Reasons for failure to return are reported in Supplementary data, Appendix 2, available in Age and Ageing online.

The study protocol was approved by the institutional review board. All participants provided written informed consent.

Mobility assessment

Usual gait speed to the nearest 0.1 second was measured on a 6-meter course. The average speed in m/s over two trials was used for analyses.

The 400 m walk test provides a 400 m fast walk time. Participants complete ten 40-meter laps walking as quickly as possible and are not allowed to run. Lap time was recorded using a stop watch; the sum of 10 lap times yields the 400 m time. The use of walking aids during the test increased from 0.2% to 2.1% from Time 1 to Time 4.

Cognition assessment

Executive function and verbal memory were assessed with a neuropsychological battery. Measures of executive function included the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) [15] and the Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B) [16]. DSST assesses complex visuoperceptual speed and mental tracking. TMT-B assesses cognitive flexibility, visuomotor speed and tracking. Verbal episodic memory was measured using the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) [17], including immediate recall, short-delay free recall and long-delay free recall.

Cognitive status

At every visit, all participants were screened using the Blessed Information Memory Concentration (BIMC) Test. Participants were evaluated with the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR) or the dementia questionnaire if the BIMC score was ≥4. In addition, participants in some sub-studies acquire a CDR at each study visit. A participant was reviewed at a consensus diagnostic conference if the BIMC score was ≥4, the CDR score ≥0.5 or the dementia questionnaire was abnormal. Mild cognitive impairment was determined by Petersen criteria [18]. Diagnoses of dementia and Alzheimer's disease, respectively, were determined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM)-III-R and the National Institute of Neurological and Communication Disorders and Stroke—Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria [19], respectively. The prevalence of cognitive impairment or dementia changed from 0 at Time 1 to 3.7% at Time 4.

Covariates

Age, sex, self-reported education in years, height and weight were collected at each visit. Body mass index was calculated as weight in kg divided by square of height in meters.

Statistical analysis

We first examined the cross-sectional association between mobility and cognition at each assessment using multivariable linear regression models, adjusted for age, sex, body mass index and education. We then examined the temporal sequence between mobility and cognition using bivariate autoregressive cross-lagged models from structural equation models [20]. In this model, both cross-lagged and autoregressive associations were evaluated simultaneously. Specifically, the autoregressive analyses examined the association between cognition at time t and cognition at time t + 1 as well as the association between mobility at time t and mobility at t + 1. The cross-lagged analyses examined the association between cognition at time t and mobility at time t + 1 while controlling for mobility at time t, and vice versa (i.e. the association between mobility at time t and cognition at time t + 1, while controlling for cognition at time t). Associations between each pair of sequential time periods (i.e. Time 1–2, 2–3, 3–4) were set to be the same in both cross-lagged and autoregressive analyses. Both autoregressive and cross-lagged associations were adjusted for age, sex, body mass index and education at Time 1. Due to multiple testing between two mobility measures and five cognitive measures, significance was defined as P < 0.005 using Bonferroni correction (0.05/10 = 0.005). A significant association between cognition at time t and mobility at time t + 1 would be over and beyond the variance accounted for by the autoregressive association between mobility at time t and mobility at t + 1, and covariates. Values of TMT-B were log transformed due to a skewed distribution. For comparison purposes, values of mobility and cognitive measures were standardised in Z scores based on the mean and standard deviation at Time 1. The regression coefficient of an association is equivalent to the effect size. We also examined the temporal sequence of two mobility measures.

We conducted a sensitivity analysis to evaluate whether the associations would change due to selection bias attributable to the increased difficulty and risk of missing follow-up data for the 400 m walk compared to the short usual pace walk. Thus, we repeated the analyses in a larger sample who had data on usual gait speed whether or not they had data on the 400 m walk (N = 484 at Time 1).

Statistical analyses were performed using SAS statistical package (v9.3, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) and Mplus7.11 (Muthen & Muthen).

Results

Table 1 provides sample characteristics. Compared to those who were initially unimpaired and returned at Time 2, those who did not meet these criteria had poorer scores on DSST, CVLT immediate recall, short-delay free recall, long-delay free recall, and had longer 400 m time (see Supplementary data, Appendix 3, available in Age and Ageing online). The two groups did not differ in other characteristics.

Table 1.

Sample characteristics

Time 1 (n = 412)Time 2 (n = 412)Time 3 (n = 222)Time 4 (n = 140)
Characteristics
 Age, years71.4 (7.7)73.6 (7.7)75.3 (7.2)76.9 (7.2)
 Female, N (%)209 (50.7)209 (50.7)108 (48.7)71 (50.7)
 Body mass index, kg/m226.9 (4.6)27.3 (4.7)27.2 (4.6)27.0 (4.4)
 Education, yearsa17 (2)17 (2)17 (2)17 (2)
Cognitive impairment or dementia, N (%)0 (0)3 (0.7)3 (1.4)4 (2.9)
Global mental state
 MMSE (range 0–30)28.8 (1.2)28.7 (1.4)28.6 (1.4)28.7 (1.3)
Executive function
 DSST (range 0–90)47.0 (10.9)44.9 (11.4)41.8 (11.4)41.7 (11.2)
 TMT-B, s, median (IQR)69.5 (33)71.0 (36)69.5 (36)71.0 (37)
Verbal memory
 CVLT immediate recall52.5 (12.1)52.5 (12.7)54.2 (13.1)53.1 (12.6)
 CVLT short-delay free recall (range 0–16)10.7 (3.3)10.6 (3.5)10.9 (3.4)10.6 (3.4)
 CVLT long-delay free recall (range 0–16)11.2 (3.3)11.2 (3.3)11.4 (3.4)11.1 (3.5)
Mobility measures
 Usual gait speed, m/s1.14 (0.18)1.12 (0.19)1.13 (0.20)1.10 (0.19)
 400 m time, s267.4 (41.3)279.2 (52.4)290.2 (65. 3)297.1 (66.0)
 Use of a walking aid, N (%)1 (0.2)2 (0.5)3 (1.4)3 (2.1)
Time 1 (n = 412)Time 2 (n = 412)Time 3 (n = 222)Time 4 (n = 140)
Characteristics
 Age, years71.4 (7.7)73.6 (7.7)75.3 (7.2)76.9 (7.2)
 Female, N (%)209 (50.7)209 (50.7)108 (48.7)71 (50.7)
 Body mass index, kg/m226.9 (4.6)27.3 (4.7)27.2 (4.6)27.0 (4.4)
 Education, yearsa17 (2)17 (2)17 (2)17 (2)
Cognitive impairment or dementia, N (%)0 (0)3 (0.7)3 (1.4)4 (2.9)
Global mental state
 MMSE (range 0–30)28.8 (1.2)28.7 (1.4)28.6 (1.4)28.7 (1.3)
Executive function
 DSST (range 0–90)47.0 (10.9)44.9 (11.4)41.8 (11.4)41.7 (11.2)
 TMT-B, s, median (IQR)69.5 (33)71.0 (36)69.5 (36)71.0 (37)
Verbal memory
 CVLT immediate recall52.5 (12.1)52.5 (12.7)54.2 (13.1)53.1 (12.6)
 CVLT short-delay free recall (range 0–16)10.7 (3.3)10.6 (3.5)10.9 (3.4)10.6 (3.4)
 CVLT long-delay free recall (range 0–16)11.2 (3.3)11.2 (3.3)11.4 (3.4)11.1 (3.5)
Mobility measures
 Usual gait speed, m/s1.14 (0.18)1.12 (0.19)1.13 (0.20)1.10 (0.19)
 400 m time, s267.4 (41.3)279.2 (52.4)290.2 (65. 3)297.1 (66.0)
 Use of a walking aid, N (%)1 (0.2)2 (0.5)3 (1.4)3 (2.1)

aPresented in years of full time education.

Note: MMSE, Mini-mental State Exam; DSST, Digit symbol substitution test; TMT-B, Trail Making Test Part-B; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test. Values are mean (SD) unless otherwise noted.

Table 1.

Sample characteristics

Time 1 (n = 412)Time 2 (n = 412)Time 3 (n = 222)Time 4 (n = 140)
Characteristics
 Age, years71.4 (7.7)73.6 (7.7)75.3 (7.2)76.9 (7.2)
 Female, N (%)209 (50.7)209 (50.7)108 (48.7)71 (50.7)
 Body mass index, kg/m226.9 (4.6)27.3 (4.7)27.2 (4.6)27.0 (4.4)
 Education, yearsa17 (2)17 (2)17 (2)17 (2)
Cognitive impairment or dementia, N (%)0 (0)3 (0.7)3 (1.4)4 (2.9)
Global mental state
 MMSE (range 0–30)28.8 (1.2)28.7 (1.4)28.6 (1.4)28.7 (1.3)
Executive function
 DSST (range 0–90)47.0 (10.9)44.9 (11.4)41.8 (11.4)41.7 (11.2)
 TMT-B, s, median (IQR)69.5 (33)71.0 (36)69.5 (36)71.0 (37)
Verbal memory
 CVLT immediate recall52.5 (12.1)52.5 (12.7)54.2 (13.1)53.1 (12.6)
 CVLT short-delay free recall (range 0–16)10.7 (3.3)10.6 (3.5)10.9 (3.4)10.6 (3.4)
 CVLT long-delay free recall (range 0–16)11.2 (3.3)11.2 (3.3)11.4 (3.4)11.1 (3.5)
Mobility measures
 Usual gait speed, m/s1.14 (0.18)1.12 (0.19)1.13 (0.20)1.10 (0.19)
 400 m time, s267.4 (41.3)279.2 (52.4)290.2 (65. 3)297.1 (66.0)
 Use of a walking aid, N (%)1 (0.2)2 (0.5)3 (1.4)3 (2.1)
Time 1 (n = 412)Time 2 (n = 412)Time 3 (n = 222)Time 4 (n = 140)
Characteristics
 Age, years71.4 (7.7)73.6 (7.7)75.3 (7.2)76.9 (7.2)
 Female, N (%)209 (50.7)209 (50.7)108 (48.7)71 (50.7)
 Body mass index, kg/m226.9 (4.6)27.3 (4.7)27.2 (4.6)27.0 (4.4)
 Education, yearsa17 (2)17 (2)17 (2)17 (2)
Cognitive impairment or dementia, N (%)0 (0)3 (0.7)3 (1.4)4 (2.9)
Global mental state
 MMSE (range 0–30)28.8 (1.2)28.7 (1.4)28.6 (1.4)28.7 (1.3)
Executive function
 DSST (range 0–90)47.0 (10.9)44.9 (11.4)41.8 (11.4)41.7 (11.2)
 TMT-B, s, median (IQR)69.5 (33)71.0 (36)69.5 (36)71.0 (37)
Verbal memory
 CVLT immediate recall52.5 (12.1)52.5 (12.7)54.2 (13.1)53.1 (12.6)
 CVLT short-delay free recall (range 0–16)10.7 (3.3)10.6 (3.5)10.9 (3.4)10.6 (3.4)
 CVLT long-delay free recall (range 0–16)11.2 (3.3)11.2 (3.3)11.4 (3.4)11.1 (3.5)
Mobility measures
 Usual gait speed, m/s1.14 (0.18)1.12 (0.19)1.13 (0.20)1.10 (0.19)
 400 m time, s267.4 (41.3)279.2 (52.4)290.2 (65. 3)297.1 (66.0)
 Use of a walking aid, N (%)1 (0.2)2 (0.5)3 (1.4)3 (2.1)

aPresented in years of full time education.

Note: MMSE, Mini-mental State Exam; DSST, Digit symbol substitution test; TMT-B, Trail Making Test Part-B; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test. Values are mean (SD) unless otherwise noted.

The cross-sectional association between cognition and mobility

Cross-sectionally at Time 1, there were no significant associations between usual gait speed and any of the cognitive measures after adjustment, but there were significant associations of 400 m time with DSST, TMT-B and CVLT immediate recall. There were associations and trends toward a variety of associations among usual gait, 400 m time, and cognition at the follow-up periods in the expected direction (see Supplementary data, Appendix 4, available in Age and Ageing online).

The temporal sequences of usual gait speed with executive function and memory

After adjustment, the autoregressive associations (e.g. the association of a measure with itself at follow-up) of usual gait speed, DSST, TMT-B and CVLT measures across all time points were significant at P < 0.001 (data not shown) and there were significant bidirectional cross-lagged associations of usual gait speed with DSST and TMT-B. Slower gait speed was associated with subsequent poorer DSST and TMT-B while accounting for prior DSST and TMT-B, respectively; poorer DSST and TMT-B were also associated with subsequent slower gait speed while accounting for prior gait speed (Table 2, Figure 1a). There were one-way trends toward associations of slower usual gait speed with subsequent poorer CVLT immediate and short-delay free recall that did not hold after correcting for multiple testing. There were no cross-lagged associations between usual gait speed and CVLT long-delay free recall (Table 2).
Figure 1.

Significant cross-lagged and autoregressive associations between cognition and usual gait speed (a) and fast 400 m time (b) (c). Solid line: significant associations. Dashed line: non-significant associations.

Table 2.

Cross-lagged associations of mobility with cognition

β (SE), P-valueβ (SE), P-value
Cross-lagged associations between cognitive measures and gait speed (higher = better)
DSST→gait speed0.092 (0.028), 0.001gait speed→DSST0.069 (0.018), <0.001
TMT-B→gait speed−0.100 (0.029), 0.001gait speed→TMT-B−0.137 (0.025), <0.001
CVLT immediate→gait speed0.049 (0.028), 0.081gait speed→CVLT immediate0.059 (0.022), 0.008
CVLT short-delay→gait speed0.051 (0.029), 0.074gait speed→CVLT short-delay0.063 (0.024), 0.009
CVLT long-delay→gait speed0.046 (0.030), 0.119gait speed→CVLT long-delay0.031 (0.023), 0.178
Cross-lagged associations between cognitive measures and 400 m time (lower = better)
DSST→400 m time−0.031 (0.023), 0.183400 m time→DSST−0.048 (0.016), 0.003
TMT-B→400 m time0.012 (0.023), 0.620400 m time→TMT-B0.115 (0.023), <0.001
CVLT immediate→400 m time−0.019 (0.023), 0.394400 m time→CVLT immediate−0.078 (0.020), <0.001
CVLT short-delay→400 m time−0.029 (0.023), 0.203400 m time→CVLT short-delay−0.087 (0.022), <0.001
CVLT long-delay→400 m time−0.038 (0.023), 0.108400 m time→CVLT long-delay−0.043 (0.021), 0.043
Cross-lagged associations between usual gait speed and 400 m time
Gait speed→400 m time−0.060 (0.026), 0.023400 m time→usual gait speed−0.311 (0.028), <0.001
β (SE), P-valueβ (SE), P-value
Cross-lagged associations between cognitive measures and gait speed (higher = better)
DSST→gait speed0.092 (0.028), 0.001gait speed→DSST0.069 (0.018), <0.001
TMT-B→gait speed−0.100 (0.029), 0.001gait speed→TMT-B−0.137 (0.025), <0.001
CVLT immediate→gait speed0.049 (0.028), 0.081gait speed→CVLT immediate0.059 (0.022), 0.008
CVLT short-delay→gait speed0.051 (0.029), 0.074gait speed→CVLT short-delay0.063 (0.024), 0.009
CVLT long-delay→gait speed0.046 (0.030), 0.119gait speed→CVLT long-delay0.031 (0.023), 0.178
Cross-lagged associations between cognitive measures and 400 m time (lower = better)
DSST→400 m time−0.031 (0.023), 0.183400 m time→DSST−0.048 (0.016), 0.003
TMT-B→400 m time0.012 (0.023), 0.620400 m time→TMT-B0.115 (0.023), <0.001
CVLT immediate→400 m time−0.019 (0.023), 0.394400 m time→CVLT immediate−0.078 (0.020), <0.001
CVLT short-delay→400 m time−0.029 (0.023), 0.203400 m time→CVLT short-delay−0.087 (0.022), <0.001
CVLT long-delay→400 m time−0.038 (0.023), 0.108400 m time→CVLT long-delay−0.043 (0.021), 0.043
Cross-lagged associations between usual gait speed and 400 m time
Gait speed→400 m time−0.060 (0.026), 0.023400 m time→usual gait speed−0.311 (0.028), <0.001

Note: N = 412 at Time 1, N = 412 at Time 2, N = 222 at Time 3, N = 140 at Time 4. Models adjusted for the dependent variable measure at the previous assessment, baseline age, sex, body mass index and years of education. Bold number reflects significant associations with Bonferroni correction.

Table 2.

Cross-lagged associations of mobility with cognition

β (SE), P-valueβ (SE), P-value
Cross-lagged associations between cognitive measures and gait speed (higher = better)
DSST→gait speed0.092 (0.028), 0.001gait speed→DSST0.069 (0.018), <0.001
TMT-B→gait speed−0.100 (0.029), 0.001gait speed→TMT-B−0.137 (0.025), <0.001
CVLT immediate→gait speed0.049 (0.028), 0.081gait speed→CVLT immediate0.059 (0.022), 0.008
CVLT short-delay→gait speed0.051 (0.029), 0.074gait speed→CVLT short-delay0.063 (0.024), 0.009
CVLT long-delay→gait speed0.046 (0.030), 0.119gait speed→CVLT long-delay0.031 (0.023), 0.178
Cross-lagged associations between cognitive measures and 400 m time (lower = better)
DSST→400 m time−0.031 (0.023), 0.183400 m time→DSST−0.048 (0.016), 0.003
TMT-B→400 m time0.012 (0.023), 0.620400 m time→TMT-B0.115 (0.023), <0.001
CVLT immediate→400 m time−0.019 (0.023), 0.394400 m time→CVLT immediate−0.078 (0.020), <0.001
CVLT short-delay→400 m time−0.029 (0.023), 0.203400 m time→CVLT short-delay−0.087 (0.022), <0.001
CVLT long-delay→400 m time−0.038 (0.023), 0.108400 m time→CVLT long-delay−0.043 (0.021), 0.043
Cross-lagged associations between usual gait speed and 400 m time
Gait speed→400 m time−0.060 (0.026), 0.023400 m time→usual gait speed−0.311 (0.028), <0.001
β (SE), P-valueβ (SE), P-value
Cross-lagged associations between cognitive measures and gait speed (higher = better)
DSST→gait speed0.092 (0.028), 0.001gait speed→DSST0.069 (0.018), <0.001
TMT-B→gait speed−0.100 (0.029), 0.001gait speed→TMT-B−0.137 (0.025), <0.001
CVLT immediate→gait speed0.049 (0.028), 0.081gait speed→CVLT immediate0.059 (0.022), 0.008
CVLT short-delay→gait speed0.051 (0.029), 0.074gait speed→CVLT short-delay0.063 (0.024), 0.009
CVLT long-delay→gait speed0.046 (0.030), 0.119gait speed→CVLT long-delay0.031 (0.023), 0.178
Cross-lagged associations between cognitive measures and 400 m time (lower = better)
DSST→400 m time−0.031 (0.023), 0.183400 m time→DSST−0.048 (0.016), 0.003
TMT-B→400 m time0.012 (0.023), 0.620400 m time→TMT-B0.115 (0.023), <0.001
CVLT immediate→400 m time−0.019 (0.023), 0.394400 m time→CVLT immediate−0.078 (0.020), <0.001
CVLT short-delay→400 m time−0.029 (0.023), 0.203400 m time→CVLT short-delay−0.087 (0.022), <0.001
CVLT long-delay→400 m time−0.038 (0.023), 0.108400 m time→CVLT long-delay−0.043 (0.021), 0.043
Cross-lagged associations between usual gait speed and 400 m time
Gait speed→400 m time−0.060 (0.026), 0.023400 m time→usual gait speed−0.311 (0.028), <0.001

Note: N = 412 at Time 1, N = 412 at Time 2, N = 222 at Time 3, N = 140 at Time 4. Models adjusted for the dependent variable measure at the previous assessment, baseline age, sex, body mass index and years of education. Bold number reflects significant associations with Bonferroni correction.

The temporal sequences of 400 m time with executive function and memory

After adjustment, the autoregressive associations of 400 m time, DSST, TMT-B and CVLT measures across all time points were all significant at P < 0.001 (data not shown). There were significant one-way cross-lagged associations of slower 400 m time with poorer DSST, TMT-B, CVLT immediate recall and short-delay free recall scores. Slower 400 m time was associated with subsequent poorer DSST, TMT-B, CVLT immediate recall and short-delay free recall, while accounting for prior scores on these measures. In contrast, DSST, TMT-B, CVLT immediate recall and short-delay free recall were not associated with subsequent 400 m time (Table 2, Figure 1b–c). There was a one-way trend toward the association of slower 400 m time and subsequent poorer CVLT long-delay free recall that did not hold after correcting for multiple testing.

The temporal sequences of usual gait speed and fast 400 m time

There was a one-way cross-lagged association of longer 400 m time with subsequent slower usual gait speed, while accounting for prior usual gait speed. In contrast, usual gait speed did not predict subsequent 400 m time after accounting for prior 400 m time.

Sensitivity analyses

Among all who met entry criteria, with or without complete follow-up 400 m test data, results of the bidirectional relationships of slower usual gait speed with poorer DSST and TMT-B remained significant after Bonferroni correction (see Supplementary data, Appendix 5, available in Age and Ageing online).

Discussion

In a sample of older adults with initially unimpaired gait speed and cognition, we found that usual gait speed and executive function measures of DSST and TMT-B have a bidirectional change relationship. In contrast, poor fast-paced endurance walk performance may be a strong and early predictor of subsequent deficits in executive function and memory, while the reverse was not found.

The strong bidirectional association between usual gait speed and executive function is consistent with prior findings [11, 12]. Several potential mechanisms for this relationship have been proposed. First, gait performance and executive function may share common neuropathologies. Based on neuroimaging evidence, poorer gait and executive function are both associated with greater brain atrophy and white matter lesions [1]. Second, some have proposed that the increasing number of senescent cells during ageing, accompanied by the secretion of pro-inflammatory cytokines, growth factors and proteases, may explain the bidirectional relationship [21, 22]. Third, multiple health-related conditions of age, including multi-morbidity and lifestyle factors may affect systems that contribute to worsening of both gait and executive function [23].

We did not find significant longitudinal associations between usual gait and memory function, while some prior studies reported a bidirectional relationship [12, 24]. In contrast, we found that the longer 400 m time at a fast pace unidirectionally predicted subsequent executive function and memory, whereas tasks assessing these cognitive domains did not predict subsequent 400 m time. This is consistent with a report suggesting that fast but not usual gait speed predicts global cognitive function [25], but that study did not test specific cognitive domains. There is evidence that one path to brain health appears to be fitness training [26]. Perhaps the more demanding longer fast-paced walk may be more able to detect cardiopulmonary fitness, and physiological reserve compared to usual gait [27]. In this cohort with unimpaired cognition, the 400 m time predicted CVLT immediate and short-delay free recall, but not long-delay free recall. Longitudinal data from the BLSA suggest that CVLT immediate recall is the first to decline during the early preclinical stages of disease, while decline in delayed recall is enhanced in the later stage [28]. Perhaps, the ability of 400 m to predict CVLT immediate recall and short-delay free recall is capturing the earliest changes in this initially unimpaired cohort.

The inconsistency among studies may be attributable to methodological differences, including the sample, the duration of follow-up and the types of measures used [5–9, 11, 12]. Three studies only examined rate of change, so were unable to assess the sequence of events [6, 7, 12]. Of the remaining five studies, two required normal cognition [10, 11], one that participants be free of dementia [8], one excluded diagnosed neurological disease [9] and one did not specify cognitive eligibility [12]. In terms of mobility at entry, one required no report of difficulty walking ¼ mile, climbing 10 steps, or performing activities of daily living at entry [11], one that participants be able to walk 3 meters [9], but others do not provide an explicit criterion. Among the five studies, measures of cognition vary from global indicators such as MMSE to executive function tests only, to one report with an extensive cognitive battery [10]. All five measured usual gait, while some also included chair stands and grip strength. None included a long fast walk. Follow-up ranged from 1 to 9 years.

Our study has several strengths. We attempted to create an initially unimpaired cohort where the sequence of decline could be detected. We tested two key measures of gait and two domains of cognition, to allow assessment of differential associations within types of mobility and cognition. In addition, we performed a sensitivity analysis to assess selection bias during follow-up. Our study also has limitations. Those who return are in better health than those who do not (Appendix 2, available at Age and Ageing online). If those who failed to return had greater decline, it is likely that our estimates of the magnitude of change over time are underestimates. In the overall ageing population, the true rate of decline may be greater. We cannot predict the effect of loss of follow-up on the direction or sequence of change. While our follow-up lasted 6 years, an even longer period might reveal stronger effects because over longer periods of time people are likely to decline more.

In this initially unimpaired cohort of older adults, change in usual gait speed and executive function predict each other over time, whereas poor fast-paced endurance walk performance appears to precede decline in executive function and memory. Endurance walking may be a sensitive early predictor of cognitive change.

Key points

  • Age-related declines in mobility and cognition are strongly correlated, but the temporal sequence of change over time is not well understood.

  • Changes in usual gait speed and executive function predict each other over time, whereas poor fast-paced endurance walk performance appears to precede declines in executive function and memory.

  • Endurance walking performance may be a sensitive early predictor of cognitive change.

Supplementary data

Supplementary data are available at Age and Ageing online.

Conflicts of interest

None declared.

Funding

This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging.

References

1

Holtzer
R
,
Epstein
N
,
Mahoney
JR
,
Izzetoglu
M
,
Blumen
HM
.
Neuroimaging of mobility in aging: a targeted review
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2014
;
69
:
1375
–
88
.

2

Montero-Odasso
M
,
Verghese
J
,
Beauchet
O
,
Hausdorff
JM
.
Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
2012
;
60
:
2127
–
36
.

3

Albers
MW
,
Gilmore
GC
,
Kaye
J
et al. .
At the interface of sensory and motor dysfunctions and Alzheimer's disease
.
Alzheimers Dement
2015
;
11
:
70
–
98
.

4

Beauchet
O
,
Annweiler
C
,
Callisaya
ML
et al. .
Poor gait performance and prediction of dementia: results from a meta-analysis
.
J Am Med Dir Assoc
2016
;
17
:
482
–
90
.

5

Callisaya
ML
,
Blizzard
CL
,
Wood
AG
et al. .
Longitudinal relationships between cognitive decline and gait slowing: the Tasmanian Study of Cognition and Gait
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2015
;
70
:
1226
–
32
.

6

Tabbarah
M
,
Crimmins
EM
,
Seeman
TE
.
The relationship between cognitive and physical performance: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2002
;
57
:
M228
–
35
.

7

Payette
H
,
Gueye
NR
,
Gaudreau
P
et al. .
Trajectories of physical function decline and psychological functioning: the Quebec longitudinal study on nutrition and successful aging (NuAge)
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
2011
;
66
:
i82
–
90
.

8

Atkinson
HH
,
Rapp
SR
,
Williamson
JD
et al. .
The relationship between cognitive function and physical performance in older women: results from the women's health initiative memory study
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2010
;
65
:
300
–
6
.

9

Best
JR
,
Davis
JC
,
Liu-Ambrose
T
.
Longitudinal analysis of physical performance, functional status, physical activity, and mood in relation to executive function in older adults who fall
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
2015
;
63
:
1112
–
20
.

10

Mielke
MM
,
Roberts
RO
,
Savica
R
et al. .
Assessing the temporal relationship between cognition and gait: slow gait predicts cognitive decline in the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2013
;
68
:
929
–
37
.

11

Best
JR
,
Liu-Ambrose
T
,
Boudreau
RM
et al. .
An evaluation of the longitudinal, bidirectional associations between gait speed and cognition in older women and men
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2016
;
63
:
1112
–
20
.

12

Gale
CR
,
Allerhand
M
,
Sayer
AA
,
Cooper
C
,
Deary
IJ
.
The dynamic relationship between cognitive function and walking speed: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing
.
Age (Dordr)
2014
;
36
:
9682
.

13

Shock
NW
,
Greulich
RC
,
Andres
R
et al. .
Normal Human Aging: The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging
.
Washington, DC
:
US Government Printing Office
,
1984
.

14

Folstein
MF
,
Robins
LN
,
Helzer
JE
.
The mini-mental state examination
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
1983
;
40
:
812
.

15

Wechsler
D
WAIS-R Manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-revised
.
New York, NY
:
Psychological Corporation
,
1981
.

16

Lezak
MD
,
Howieson
DB
,
Loring
DW
,
Hannay
HJ
,
Fischer
JS
.
Neuropsychological Assessment
, 4th ed.
New York, NY, US
:
Oxford University Press
,
2004
.

17

Delis
DC
et al.  .
Profiles of demented and amnesic patients on the California Verbal Learning Test: Implications for the assessment of memory disorders
.
Psychol Assessment
1991
;
3
:
19
–
26
.

18

Zonderman
AB
,
Giambra
LM
,
Arenberg
D
et al. .
Changes in immediate visual memory predict cognitive impairment
.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol
1995
;
10
:
111
–
23
.

19

McKhann
G
,
Drachman
D
,
Folstein
M
et al. .
Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease
.
Neurology
1984
;
34
:
939
–
44
.

20

Bollen
KA
,
Curran
PJ
Latent Curve Models: A Structural Equation Perspective
.
Hoboken, NJ
:
John Wiley & Sons
,
2006
.

21

Campisi
J
,
Andersen
JK
,
Kapahi
P
,
Melov
S
.
Cellular senescence: a link between cancer and age-related degenerative disease
.
Semin Cancer Biol
2011
;
21
:
354
–
9
.

22

Campisi
J
,
d'Adda di Fagagna
F
.
Cellular senescence: when bad things happen to good cells
.
Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol
2007
;
8
:
729
–
40
.

23

Fabbri
E
,
An
Y
,
Zoli
M
et al. .
Association between accelerated multimorbidity and age-related cognitive decline in older baltimore longitudinal study of aging participants without dementia
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
2016
;
64
:
965
–
72
.

24

Watson
NL
,
Rosano
C
,
Boudreau
RM
et al. .
Executive function, memory, and gait speed decline in well-functioning older adults
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2010
;
65
:
1093
–
100
.

25

Deshpande
N
,
Metter
EJ
,
Bandinelli
S
,
Guralnik
J
,
Ferrucci
L
.
Gait speed under varied challenges and cognitive decline in older persons: a prospective study
.
Age Ageing
2009
;
38
:
509
–
14
.

26

Kramer
AF
,
Colcombe
SJ
,
McAuley
E
et al. .
Enhancing brain and cognitive function of older adults through fitness training
.
J Mol Neurosci
2003
;
20
:
213
–
21
.

27

Fitzpatrick
AL
,
Buchanan
CK
,
Nahin
RL
et al. .
Associations of gait speed and other measures of physical function with cognition in a healthy cohort of elderly persons
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2007
;
62
:
1244
–
51
.

28

Bilgel
M
,
An
Y
,
Lang
A
et al. .
Trajectories of Alzheimer disease-related cognitive measures in a longitudinal sample
.
Alzheimers Dement
2014
;
10
:
735
–
742
e4.

Supplementary data

Comments

0 Comments
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.