Abstract

Cognitive dysfunction is a well-established feature of schizophrenia, and there is evidence suggesting that cognitive deficits are secondary to abnormal neurodevelopment leading to problems in acquiring such abilities. However, it is not clear whether there is also a decline in cognitive performance over, or after, the onset of psychosis. Our objective was to quantitatively examine the longitudinal changes in cognitive function in patients who presented with first-episode psychosis (FEP), ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis, and controls. Electronic databases were searched for the studies published between January 1987 and February 2013. All studies reporting longitudinal cognitive data in FEP and UHR subjects were retrieved. We conducted meta-analyses of 25 studies including 905 patients with FEP, 560 patients at UHR, and 405 healthy controls. The cognitive performances of FEP, UHR, and healthy controls all significantly improved over time. There was no publication bias, and distributions of effect sizes were very homogenous. In FEP, the degree of improvement in verbal working memory and executive functions was significantly associated with reduction in negative symptoms. There was no evidence of cognitive decline in patients with UHR and FEP. In contrast, the cognitive performances of both groups improved at follow-up. These findings suggest that cognitive deficits are already established before the prodromal phases of psychosis. These data support the neurodevelopmental model rather than neurodegenerative and related staging models of schizophrenia.

Introduction

It is well established that cognitive dysfunction is one of the characteristics of schizophrenia.1–3 However, the development and course of the cognitive deficits continues to be a topic of controversy. The main debate regarding the trajectory of cognitive deficits is about whether schizophrenia follows a neurodevelopmental or neurodegenerative course (or some combination of them). Neurodevelopmental theories suggest that core cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are the outcome of abnormal development of the brain leading to problems in acquiring cognitive abilities.4–6 A number of studies have provided evidence indicating that cognitive and intellectual deficits are evident early in neurodevelopment much before the onset of psychosis.7–9

Although most researchers accept that such developmental deficits are evident in schizophrenia, some suggest that there is also evidence for subsequent deterioration of previously acquired cognitive abilities immediately before, and after, the illness onset.10,11 Although there is no pathological evidence for gliosis or neurodegeneration in schizophrenia,12 other neuroregressive processes such as excessive pruning or inflammation in late adolescence have been proposed as possible neurobiological underpinnings of cognitive decline.13–17 The idea of cognitive decline in schizophrenia dates back to early description of the illness as “dementia praecox” by Kraepelin. More recently, clinical observations suggesting functional decline after the onset of illness in subgroups of patients, especially in the late prodrome, and earlier years after the onset of psychosis, have been put forward to support the idea of cognitive deterioration.18,19 In accordance with neurodegenerative views, some authors have proposed applying a clinical staging model to schizophrenia in which there is cognitive decline between stages such that the later stages of illness are associated with increasingly more severe cognitive dysfunction.20

Most evidence supporting cognitive deterioration in schizophrenia is based on indirect comparison of cross-sectional studies of schizophrenia patients in different phases of the illness (chronic, first episode, ultrahigh risk [UHR] to psychosis) with healthy controls. The findings indicate that cognitive deficits in UHR psychosis are substantially less severe than in first-episode psychosis (FEP),21–23 suggesting potential cognitive decline over the transition into psychosis, which might be secondary to the neurobiological changes that lead to the emergence of psychotic symptoms. Although some cross-sectional data suggest that cognitive deficits in first-episode and chronic patients are more similar,23 there is other evidence suggesting progression in selected cognitive domains after the onset of psychosis.11 Overall, findings of cross-sectional cognitive studies can be interpreted as indicating substantial cognitive decline over the onset of psychosis followed by a further more modest and selective deterioration after the onset of the illness.

However, there are many problems in using indirect comparisons of cross-sectional studies as an evidence of progression of cognitive deficits. First, there are methodological problems related to sample differences such as symptom levels, medication, demographical characteristics such as education and gender. Typically, first-episode patients are more symptomatic and take more medications than UHR subjects. Then, UHR is a more heterogeneous concept than either established schizophrenia or FEP: it is likely to include a mixture of true prodromal schizophrenia, affective psychosis and other psychotic disorders, subjects who are in psychotic disorder spectrum but have a favorable outcome, and a majority of subjects who will never develop psychosis. Therefore, it is expected that a lesser percentage of UHR subjects would have cognitive deficits leading to modest effect sizes. In a similar fashion, many individuals who suffered an FEP will recover completely or sufficiently to be cared for by their family doctor leaving psychiatrists to treat those with the most severe and recurrent illnesses. For all these reasons, only the most severe UHR or FEP cases will eventually end up in those samples of chronic schizophrenic patients who are examined for cognitive deficits.6

Longitudinal studies are likely to give a clearer picture. Two meta-analyses of longitudinal cognitive studies in established schizophrenia and also in older patients with this illness did not find evidence of cognitive decline.24,25 In these patient samples, cognitive deficits are stable or even slightly improved at follow-up (possibly due to practice effects and/or clinical stabilization). However, authors of a recent meta-analysis of 8 studies suggested that there is IQ decline in schizophrenia.26 Also McIntosh et al27 who investigated a large birth cohort suggested that schizophrenia susceptibility genes were associated with a greater relative cognitive decline between age 11 and 70.

Follow-up studies of the course of cognitive deficits in FEP and UHR might be more able to detect potential cognitive decline over, and after, the onset of psychosis, especially as it has been argued that most functional decline occurs just before or within few years after the onset of psychosis. A number of studies have investigated the course of cognition in UHR and FEP, and many of these have failed to provide evidence for cognitive decline;28 however, there are contradictory findings suggesting cognitive decline in early psychosis, and a number of authors’ conclusions were indecisive or endorsed cognitive deterioration.26,27,29–32 Overall, the idea of cognitive decline early in psychosis remains the dominant view among clinicians, and the idea of early intervention to prevent cognitive decline remains popular.

Most individual studies have small sample sizes, and reviews are based on vote counting the results of individual studies; thus, they ignore sample size differences and its implications on statistical threshold. Therefore, findings of such reviews might be biased, and they are likely to fail to detect possible modest cognitive decline in selected measures in early psychosis. Meta-analysis go beyond vote counting, and they combine sample size–weighted effect sizes without a statistical threshold. Also confounders such as clinical stabilization/acute presentation, practice effects, and outcome of UHR can be examined in a meta-analysis. Therefore, a formal meta-analysis of the longitudinal course of cognition in UHR and FEP and analysis of the effect of confounding factors would be important to show cognitive changes before and after the onset of psychosis.

Methods

Study Selection

We followed the guidelines of the meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology in this study.33 A literature search was conducted (by E.B.) using the databases Pubmed, ScienceDirect, PsycINFO, and Scopus to identify the relevant studies (January 1987 to February 2013). We used the combination of following keywords in this search: schizophrenia, FEP, longitudinal, follow-up, clinical high risk, UHR, prodrome, at risk mental state, psychosis, schizophrenia, cog*, neurops*, memory, attention, and executive function. We also reviewed the reference lists of published studies. The corresponding authors were asked to provide additional data not included in the original report. Inclusion criteria were studies that (1) published in an English language peer-reviewed journal (study quality criterion), (2) reported longitudinal neurocognitive data, and (3) included FEP and/or UHR subjects. Inclusion criteria for follow-up duration for the FEP studies were 1–5 years as we aimed to study possible cognitive changes before or within 5 years after onset of psychosis. Few available early onset schizophrenia studies were not included (3 studies, see online Supplementary Data). For sensitivity analysis of study quality (in addition to inclusion criterion 1), studies were coded as to whether they excluded substance abuse/dependence and whether they used a structured clinical interview. UHR was defined as having 1 or more of 3 psychosis risk syndromes at help seeking youth or young adults: (1) recent onset or worsening of attenuated positive symptoms; (2) recent onset of psychotic symptoms that were significant but not sufficiently sustained to meet the criteria for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, psychotic disorder (brief intermittent psychotic syndrome); (3) genetic/familial risk to psychosis plus deterioration (recent onset or worsening functional decline) syndrome. To qualify as FEP, studies including recent onset psychosis needed to conduct the first cognitive assessment after the psychotic episode (unlike UHR-P). As in UHR sample, FEP included not only schizophrenia cases but also other schizophreniform psychoses and some patients with affective disorders (table 1).

Table 1.

Characteristics of Studies Included in the Meta-analysis of Longitudinal Studies of UHR and FEP

StudySample (Male)Follow-upAgeCharacteristicsMedicationCognitive Tasks
Addingtonet al48124 (83) FEP; 66 (38) HC1, 2 yAge: FEP = 24.6; HC = 22.7Substantial improvement in positive symptoms; PANSS; schizophrenia spectrum1 y = 82%, 2y = 68%; baseline = 98% on SGAWCST, TMT A, B, RAVLT, letter and category fluency, Stroop, CPT, logical memory, Rey figure
Albuset al5071 (36) FES; 71 (36) HC5 yAge: FES = 29.7; HC = 29.9Baseline stable, after admission; PANSS, SANS56/71 on AP; 16/71 FGAWMS, CVLT, TMT, symbol, Stroop, WCST, letter fluency
Albus et al3450 (26) FES; 50 (23) HC2 yAge: FES = 29; HC = 31.6Baseline stable, after admission; PANSS23/50 FGA; 18/50 clozapineWMS, CVLT, TMT, symbol, Stroop, WCST
de Mello Ayreset al5730 FES; 67 HC1–3 yMost taking APDigits span, verbal fluency
Chanet al4534 FEP1 and 3 yAge: FEP = 28.1; HC = 27.622/34 Schizophrenia, 2/34 schizoaffective; 10/34 schizophreniform; PANSSBaseline medication naiveWCST, HSCT, monotone counting task
Goldet al5454 (41) FEP5 yAge: FEP = 24.0FE or recent onset, schizophrenia spectrum; SANS/SAPS40/54 AP; 29/54 FGATMT B, cancellation task, WCST, RCFT, letter fluency, LM, IQ
Noupoulos et al3535 (29) FES1–2 yAge: FES = 23.7Baseline at index admission
FE or recent onset
29/35 on AP; 24/35 FGA; baseline all on FGARAVLT, PAL, LM, Stroop, letter fluency, TMT B,
Hill et al5145 (28) FES; 33 (23) HC1, 2 yAge: FES = 26.1; FES=23.5Baseline at index admissionBaseline AP naive; 31/45 on AP at 1 y; 21/45 on AP at 2 yWCST, Stroop, TMT A, B, Stroop, Symbol coding, digit span, CVLT, WMS-R visual memory, cancellation test
Keefeet al4458 FEP1, 2 y (26/58 in 2 y)Olanzapine or haloperidone; at baseline < 4 mo history of APTotal score based factor; analysis of a cognitive battery
Kopalaet al5220 FEP1, 2 yAge around 23Drawn from sample of 39 consisting of vast majority schizophrenia and 3 schizoaffectiveAll on quetiapineRAVLT, Vis memory, letter fluency, design fluency, WCST, CPT, TMT A and B
Penaet al4771 (49) FEP; 34 HC2 yAge: FEP = 28.5Mixture of schizophreniform and affective psychosis; PANSSTreated with SGABrief test of attention, Digits backwards, LNS, TMT A, Stroop, WCST, letter fluency, categorical fluency, WMS memory
Purdonet al5665 (45) FES1 yAge: FES = 28.8PANSSHaloperidol, olanzapine, risperidoneWCST, letter and category fluency, symbol coding, digit span, visual span, trail making, TMT B, Rey figure, story learning, list learning, visual reproduction
Rodriguez-Sanchezet al49112 (73) FEP; 22 (9) HC1 y; active illness to stabilizationAge: FEP = 2.5SAPSMostly on haloperidol, olanzapine, risperidone; Baseline AP naiveRAVLT, RCFT, CPT, brief test of attention
Rundet al46111 (64) FEP1–2 yAge: FEP = 28.1; HC = 24.1PANSS78% AP; 73% SGACVLT, WCST, TMT A and B, letter fluency, digit span, CPT
Scottish Schizophrenia Research Group5527 FES 281 yAll pimozide or flupenthixolDigit span, block design, IQ
Towsendet al5383 (62) FEP1 y; baseline after stableAge: FEP = 24.7SAPS/SANS: 67/83 schizophrenia; 14/83 schizoaffective; 2/83 schizophreniform40 risperidone, 17 olanzapine, 11 quetiapine, 8 clozapine, 7 FGAPASAT, WCST, TMT A, B, letter fluency, CPT, Stroop, IQ, symbol coding, WMS
UHR
Beckeret al3941(29) UHR; 17 (9) HC1–2 yAge: UHR = 19.8; HC = 19.424 UHR-NP; 17 UHR-PUHR-P tested when stabilized on medicationCVLT, letter and category fluency, CPT, SWM, RCFT
Bowieet al3853 (40) UHR (3 groups); 17 (9) HC6 moAge: UHR = 15.8; HC = 16.4SIPS; SOPS11 UHR with AP; 15 UHR with AD; 27 UHR-no medicationCPT, TMT A and B, LNS, letter fluency
Keefeet al3728 UHR1 y11UHR-P; 17 UHR-NPNo medication; olanzapine using patients excludedGlobal cognition
Linet al42230 (99) UHR7.2 yAge: UHR = 18.7PACE: 189 UHR good and 41 UHR poor prognosisVMI, IQ
Wood et al3616 (10) UHR; 17 (14) HC1.4 yAge: UHR = 17.3 and 21; HC = 19.7PACE: 7 UHR-NP; 9 UHR-PAP = 10/16 (9/10 UHR-P)Digit span, symbol coding, logical memory, visual reproduction, RAVLT, TMT A, B, letter fluency, verbal pair associate
Niendamet al4135 (21) UHR0.63 yAge = 17.3SIPS; SOPSHalf on APTMT A, B, symbol coding letter fluency, matrix reasoning, digit span, visual reproduction, CVLT, logical memory
Jahsanet al4048 (28) UHR; 29 (14) HC1.1 yAge: UHR = 18.7; UHR = 19.0SIPS/CARE10/48 on APWCST, Stroop, HVLT, LNS, spatial span
Barbatoet al4372 UHR6 moAll UHR-NPRAVLT, LNS, WCST, Stroop, TMT A and B, CPT, category fluency, SWM, n-back
Woodberryet al5853 (26) UHR; 32 (16) HC1 yAge: UHR = 16; HC = 16.343 UHR-NP; 10 UHR-PAP = 59%; mood stabilizer = 40%CVLT, WMS, WCST, LNS, CPT, verbal fluency, trail making
StudySample (Male)Follow-upAgeCharacteristicsMedicationCognitive Tasks
Addingtonet al48124 (83) FEP; 66 (38) HC1, 2 yAge: FEP = 24.6; HC = 22.7Substantial improvement in positive symptoms; PANSS; schizophrenia spectrum1 y = 82%, 2y = 68%; baseline = 98% on SGAWCST, TMT A, B, RAVLT, letter and category fluency, Stroop, CPT, logical memory, Rey figure
Albuset al5071 (36) FES; 71 (36) HC5 yAge: FES = 29.7; HC = 29.9Baseline stable, after admission; PANSS, SANS56/71 on AP; 16/71 FGAWMS, CVLT, TMT, symbol, Stroop, WCST, letter fluency
Albus et al3450 (26) FES; 50 (23) HC2 yAge: FES = 29; HC = 31.6Baseline stable, after admission; PANSS23/50 FGA; 18/50 clozapineWMS, CVLT, TMT, symbol, Stroop, WCST
de Mello Ayreset al5730 FES; 67 HC1–3 yMost taking APDigits span, verbal fluency
Chanet al4534 FEP1 and 3 yAge: FEP = 28.1; HC = 27.622/34 Schizophrenia, 2/34 schizoaffective; 10/34 schizophreniform; PANSSBaseline medication naiveWCST, HSCT, monotone counting task
Goldet al5454 (41) FEP5 yAge: FEP = 24.0FE or recent onset, schizophrenia spectrum; SANS/SAPS40/54 AP; 29/54 FGATMT B, cancellation task, WCST, RCFT, letter fluency, LM, IQ
Noupoulos et al3535 (29) FES1–2 yAge: FES = 23.7Baseline at index admission
FE or recent onset
29/35 on AP; 24/35 FGA; baseline all on FGARAVLT, PAL, LM, Stroop, letter fluency, TMT B,
Hill et al5145 (28) FES; 33 (23) HC1, 2 yAge: FES = 26.1; FES=23.5Baseline at index admissionBaseline AP naive; 31/45 on AP at 1 y; 21/45 on AP at 2 yWCST, Stroop, TMT A, B, Stroop, Symbol coding, digit span, CVLT, WMS-R visual memory, cancellation test
Keefeet al4458 FEP1, 2 y (26/58 in 2 y)Olanzapine or haloperidone; at baseline < 4 mo history of APTotal score based factor; analysis of a cognitive battery
Kopalaet al5220 FEP1, 2 yAge around 23Drawn from sample of 39 consisting of vast majority schizophrenia and 3 schizoaffectiveAll on quetiapineRAVLT, Vis memory, letter fluency, design fluency, WCST, CPT, TMT A and B
Penaet al4771 (49) FEP; 34 HC2 yAge: FEP = 28.5Mixture of schizophreniform and affective psychosis; PANSSTreated with SGABrief test of attention, Digits backwards, LNS, TMT A, Stroop, WCST, letter fluency, categorical fluency, WMS memory
Purdonet al5665 (45) FES1 yAge: FES = 28.8PANSSHaloperidol, olanzapine, risperidoneWCST, letter and category fluency, symbol coding, digit span, visual span, trail making, TMT B, Rey figure, story learning, list learning, visual reproduction
Rodriguez-Sanchezet al49112 (73) FEP; 22 (9) HC1 y; active illness to stabilizationAge: FEP = 2.5SAPSMostly on haloperidol, olanzapine, risperidone; Baseline AP naiveRAVLT, RCFT, CPT, brief test of attention
Rundet al46111 (64) FEP1–2 yAge: FEP = 28.1; HC = 24.1PANSS78% AP; 73% SGACVLT, WCST, TMT A and B, letter fluency, digit span, CPT
Scottish Schizophrenia Research Group5527 FES 281 yAll pimozide or flupenthixolDigit span, block design, IQ
Towsendet al5383 (62) FEP1 y; baseline after stableAge: FEP = 24.7SAPS/SANS: 67/83 schizophrenia; 14/83 schizoaffective; 2/83 schizophreniform40 risperidone, 17 olanzapine, 11 quetiapine, 8 clozapine, 7 FGAPASAT, WCST, TMT A, B, letter fluency, CPT, Stroop, IQ, symbol coding, WMS
UHR
Beckeret al3941(29) UHR; 17 (9) HC1–2 yAge: UHR = 19.8; HC = 19.424 UHR-NP; 17 UHR-PUHR-P tested when stabilized on medicationCVLT, letter and category fluency, CPT, SWM, RCFT
Bowieet al3853 (40) UHR (3 groups); 17 (9) HC6 moAge: UHR = 15.8; HC = 16.4SIPS; SOPS11 UHR with AP; 15 UHR with AD; 27 UHR-no medicationCPT, TMT A and B, LNS, letter fluency
Keefeet al3728 UHR1 y11UHR-P; 17 UHR-NPNo medication; olanzapine using patients excludedGlobal cognition
Linet al42230 (99) UHR7.2 yAge: UHR = 18.7PACE: 189 UHR good and 41 UHR poor prognosisVMI, IQ
Wood et al3616 (10) UHR; 17 (14) HC1.4 yAge: UHR = 17.3 and 21; HC = 19.7PACE: 7 UHR-NP; 9 UHR-PAP = 10/16 (9/10 UHR-P)Digit span, symbol coding, logical memory, visual reproduction, RAVLT, TMT A, B, letter fluency, verbal pair associate
Niendamet al4135 (21) UHR0.63 yAge = 17.3SIPS; SOPSHalf on APTMT A, B, symbol coding letter fluency, matrix reasoning, digit span, visual reproduction, CVLT, logical memory
Jahsanet al4048 (28) UHR; 29 (14) HC1.1 yAge: UHR = 18.7; UHR = 19.0SIPS/CARE10/48 on APWCST, Stroop, HVLT, LNS, spatial span
Barbatoet al4372 UHR6 moAll UHR-NPRAVLT, LNS, WCST, Stroop, TMT A and B, CPT, category fluency, SWM, n-back
Woodberryet al5853 (26) UHR; 32 (16) HC1 yAge: UHR = 16; HC = 16.343 UHR-NP; 10 UHR-PAP = 59%; mood stabilizer = 40%CVLT, WMS, WCST, LNS, CPT, verbal fluency, trail making

Note: UHR, ultra-high risk; FEP, first-episode psychosis; HC, healthy controls; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; SGA, second-generation antipsychotics; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; TMT, trail making test; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; CPT, continuous performance test; FES, first-episode schizophrenia; SANS, Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms; AP, antipsychotics; FGA, first-generation antipsychotics; CVLT, California verbal learning test; HSCT, Hayling sentence completion test; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; SAPS, Schedule for Assessment of Positive Symptoms; SWM, spatial working memory; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; LM, logical memory; PAL, paired associate learning; LNS, letter number sequencing; PASAT, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test; UHR-P, transition to psychosis in follow-up; UHR-NP, no transition to psychosis in follow-up; SIPS, Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes; SOPS, Scale of Prodromal Symptoms; AD, antidepressants; PACE, Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation; VMI, Verbal Memory Index; HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test. Studies with bold characters indicate main samples included in the meta-analysis.

Table 1.

Characteristics of Studies Included in the Meta-analysis of Longitudinal Studies of UHR and FEP

StudySample (Male)Follow-upAgeCharacteristicsMedicationCognitive Tasks
Addingtonet al48124 (83) FEP; 66 (38) HC1, 2 yAge: FEP = 24.6; HC = 22.7Substantial improvement in positive symptoms; PANSS; schizophrenia spectrum1 y = 82%, 2y = 68%; baseline = 98% on SGAWCST, TMT A, B, RAVLT, letter and category fluency, Stroop, CPT, logical memory, Rey figure
Albuset al5071 (36) FES; 71 (36) HC5 yAge: FES = 29.7; HC = 29.9Baseline stable, after admission; PANSS, SANS56/71 on AP; 16/71 FGAWMS, CVLT, TMT, symbol, Stroop, WCST, letter fluency
Albus et al3450 (26) FES; 50 (23) HC2 yAge: FES = 29; HC = 31.6Baseline stable, after admission; PANSS23/50 FGA; 18/50 clozapineWMS, CVLT, TMT, symbol, Stroop, WCST
de Mello Ayreset al5730 FES; 67 HC1–3 yMost taking APDigits span, verbal fluency
Chanet al4534 FEP1 and 3 yAge: FEP = 28.1; HC = 27.622/34 Schizophrenia, 2/34 schizoaffective; 10/34 schizophreniform; PANSSBaseline medication naiveWCST, HSCT, monotone counting task
Goldet al5454 (41) FEP5 yAge: FEP = 24.0FE or recent onset, schizophrenia spectrum; SANS/SAPS40/54 AP; 29/54 FGATMT B, cancellation task, WCST, RCFT, letter fluency, LM, IQ
Noupoulos et al3535 (29) FES1–2 yAge: FES = 23.7Baseline at index admission
FE or recent onset
29/35 on AP; 24/35 FGA; baseline all on FGARAVLT, PAL, LM, Stroop, letter fluency, TMT B,
Hill et al5145 (28) FES; 33 (23) HC1, 2 yAge: FES = 26.1; FES=23.5Baseline at index admissionBaseline AP naive; 31/45 on AP at 1 y; 21/45 on AP at 2 yWCST, Stroop, TMT A, B, Stroop, Symbol coding, digit span, CVLT, WMS-R visual memory, cancellation test
Keefeet al4458 FEP1, 2 y (26/58 in 2 y)Olanzapine or haloperidone; at baseline < 4 mo history of APTotal score based factor; analysis of a cognitive battery
Kopalaet al5220 FEP1, 2 yAge around 23Drawn from sample of 39 consisting of vast majority schizophrenia and 3 schizoaffectiveAll on quetiapineRAVLT, Vis memory, letter fluency, design fluency, WCST, CPT, TMT A and B
Penaet al4771 (49) FEP; 34 HC2 yAge: FEP = 28.5Mixture of schizophreniform and affective psychosis; PANSSTreated with SGABrief test of attention, Digits backwards, LNS, TMT A, Stroop, WCST, letter fluency, categorical fluency, WMS memory
Purdonet al5665 (45) FES1 yAge: FES = 28.8PANSSHaloperidol, olanzapine, risperidoneWCST, letter and category fluency, symbol coding, digit span, visual span, trail making, TMT B, Rey figure, story learning, list learning, visual reproduction
Rodriguez-Sanchezet al49112 (73) FEP; 22 (9) HC1 y; active illness to stabilizationAge: FEP = 2.5SAPSMostly on haloperidol, olanzapine, risperidone; Baseline AP naiveRAVLT, RCFT, CPT, brief test of attention
Rundet al46111 (64) FEP1–2 yAge: FEP = 28.1; HC = 24.1PANSS78% AP; 73% SGACVLT, WCST, TMT A and B, letter fluency, digit span, CPT
Scottish Schizophrenia Research Group5527 FES 281 yAll pimozide or flupenthixolDigit span, block design, IQ
Towsendet al5383 (62) FEP1 y; baseline after stableAge: FEP = 24.7SAPS/SANS: 67/83 schizophrenia; 14/83 schizoaffective; 2/83 schizophreniform40 risperidone, 17 olanzapine, 11 quetiapine, 8 clozapine, 7 FGAPASAT, WCST, TMT A, B, letter fluency, CPT, Stroop, IQ, symbol coding, WMS
UHR
Beckeret al3941(29) UHR; 17 (9) HC1–2 yAge: UHR = 19.8; HC = 19.424 UHR-NP; 17 UHR-PUHR-P tested when stabilized on medicationCVLT, letter and category fluency, CPT, SWM, RCFT
Bowieet al3853 (40) UHR (3 groups); 17 (9) HC6 moAge: UHR = 15.8; HC = 16.4SIPS; SOPS11 UHR with AP; 15 UHR with AD; 27 UHR-no medicationCPT, TMT A and B, LNS, letter fluency
Keefeet al3728 UHR1 y11UHR-P; 17 UHR-NPNo medication; olanzapine using patients excludedGlobal cognition
Linet al42230 (99) UHR7.2 yAge: UHR = 18.7PACE: 189 UHR good and 41 UHR poor prognosisVMI, IQ
Wood et al3616 (10) UHR; 17 (14) HC1.4 yAge: UHR = 17.3 and 21; HC = 19.7PACE: 7 UHR-NP; 9 UHR-PAP = 10/16 (9/10 UHR-P)Digit span, symbol coding, logical memory, visual reproduction, RAVLT, TMT A, B, letter fluency, verbal pair associate
Niendamet al4135 (21) UHR0.63 yAge = 17.3SIPS; SOPSHalf on APTMT A, B, symbol coding letter fluency, matrix reasoning, digit span, visual reproduction, CVLT, logical memory
Jahsanet al4048 (28) UHR; 29 (14) HC1.1 yAge: UHR = 18.7; UHR = 19.0SIPS/CARE10/48 on APWCST, Stroop, HVLT, LNS, spatial span
Barbatoet al4372 UHR6 moAll UHR-NPRAVLT, LNS, WCST, Stroop, TMT A and B, CPT, category fluency, SWM, n-back
Woodberryet al5853 (26) UHR; 32 (16) HC1 yAge: UHR = 16; HC = 16.343 UHR-NP; 10 UHR-PAP = 59%; mood stabilizer = 40%CVLT, WMS, WCST, LNS, CPT, verbal fluency, trail making
StudySample (Male)Follow-upAgeCharacteristicsMedicationCognitive Tasks
Addingtonet al48124 (83) FEP; 66 (38) HC1, 2 yAge: FEP = 24.6; HC = 22.7Substantial improvement in positive symptoms; PANSS; schizophrenia spectrum1 y = 82%, 2y = 68%; baseline = 98% on SGAWCST, TMT A, B, RAVLT, letter and category fluency, Stroop, CPT, logical memory, Rey figure
Albuset al5071 (36) FES; 71 (36) HC5 yAge: FES = 29.7; HC = 29.9Baseline stable, after admission; PANSS, SANS56/71 on AP; 16/71 FGAWMS, CVLT, TMT, symbol, Stroop, WCST, letter fluency
Albus et al3450 (26) FES; 50 (23) HC2 yAge: FES = 29; HC = 31.6Baseline stable, after admission; PANSS23/50 FGA; 18/50 clozapineWMS, CVLT, TMT, symbol, Stroop, WCST
de Mello Ayreset al5730 FES; 67 HC1–3 yMost taking APDigits span, verbal fluency
Chanet al4534 FEP1 and 3 yAge: FEP = 28.1; HC = 27.622/34 Schizophrenia, 2/34 schizoaffective; 10/34 schizophreniform; PANSSBaseline medication naiveWCST, HSCT, monotone counting task
Goldet al5454 (41) FEP5 yAge: FEP = 24.0FE or recent onset, schizophrenia spectrum; SANS/SAPS40/54 AP; 29/54 FGATMT B, cancellation task, WCST, RCFT, letter fluency, LM, IQ
Noupoulos et al3535 (29) FES1–2 yAge: FES = 23.7Baseline at index admission
FE or recent onset
29/35 on AP; 24/35 FGA; baseline all on FGARAVLT, PAL, LM, Stroop, letter fluency, TMT B,
Hill et al5145 (28) FES; 33 (23) HC1, 2 yAge: FES = 26.1; FES=23.5Baseline at index admissionBaseline AP naive; 31/45 on AP at 1 y; 21/45 on AP at 2 yWCST, Stroop, TMT A, B, Stroop, Symbol coding, digit span, CVLT, WMS-R visual memory, cancellation test
Keefeet al4458 FEP1, 2 y (26/58 in 2 y)Olanzapine or haloperidone; at baseline < 4 mo history of APTotal score based factor; analysis of a cognitive battery
Kopalaet al5220 FEP1, 2 yAge around 23Drawn from sample of 39 consisting of vast majority schizophrenia and 3 schizoaffectiveAll on quetiapineRAVLT, Vis memory, letter fluency, design fluency, WCST, CPT, TMT A and B
Penaet al4771 (49) FEP; 34 HC2 yAge: FEP = 28.5Mixture of schizophreniform and affective psychosis; PANSSTreated with SGABrief test of attention, Digits backwards, LNS, TMT A, Stroop, WCST, letter fluency, categorical fluency, WMS memory
Purdonet al5665 (45) FES1 yAge: FES = 28.8PANSSHaloperidol, olanzapine, risperidoneWCST, letter and category fluency, symbol coding, digit span, visual span, trail making, TMT B, Rey figure, story learning, list learning, visual reproduction
Rodriguez-Sanchezet al49112 (73) FEP; 22 (9) HC1 y; active illness to stabilizationAge: FEP = 2.5SAPSMostly on haloperidol, olanzapine, risperidone; Baseline AP naiveRAVLT, RCFT, CPT, brief test of attention
Rundet al46111 (64) FEP1–2 yAge: FEP = 28.1; HC = 24.1PANSS78% AP; 73% SGACVLT, WCST, TMT A and B, letter fluency, digit span, CPT
Scottish Schizophrenia Research Group5527 FES 281 yAll pimozide or flupenthixolDigit span, block design, IQ
Towsendet al5383 (62) FEP1 y; baseline after stableAge: FEP = 24.7SAPS/SANS: 67/83 schizophrenia; 14/83 schizoaffective; 2/83 schizophreniform40 risperidone, 17 olanzapine, 11 quetiapine, 8 clozapine, 7 FGAPASAT, WCST, TMT A, B, letter fluency, CPT, Stroop, IQ, symbol coding, WMS
UHR
Beckeret al3941(29) UHR; 17 (9) HC1–2 yAge: UHR = 19.8; HC = 19.424 UHR-NP; 17 UHR-PUHR-P tested when stabilized on medicationCVLT, letter and category fluency, CPT, SWM, RCFT
Bowieet al3853 (40) UHR (3 groups); 17 (9) HC6 moAge: UHR = 15.8; HC = 16.4SIPS; SOPS11 UHR with AP; 15 UHR with AD; 27 UHR-no medicationCPT, TMT A and B, LNS, letter fluency
Keefeet al3728 UHR1 y11UHR-P; 17 UHR-NPNo medication; olanzapine using patients excludedGlobal cognition
Linet al42230 (99) UHR7.2 yAge: UHR = 18.7PACE: 189 UHR good and 41 UHR poor prognosisVMI, IQ
Wood et al3616 (10) UHR; 17 (14) HC1.4 yAge: UHR = 17.3 and 21; HC = 19.7PACE: 7 UHR-NP; 9 UHR-PAP = 10/16 (9/10 UHR-P)Digit span, symbol coding, logical memory, visual reproduction, RAVLT, TMT A, B, letter fluency, verbal pair associate
Niendamet al4135 (21) UHR0.63 yAge = 17.3SIPS; SOPSHalf on APTMT A, B, symbol coding letter fluency, matrix reasoning, digit span, visual reproduction, CVLT, logical memory
Jahsanet al4048 (28) UHR; 29 (14) HC1.1 yAge: UHR = 18.7; UHR = 19.0SIPS/CARE10/48 on APWCST, Stroop, HVLT, LNS, spatial span
Barbatoet al4372 UHR6 moAll UHR-NPRAVLT, LNS, WCST, Stroop, TMT A and B, CPT, category fluency, SWM, n-back
Woodberryet al5853 (26) UHR; 32 (16) HC1 yAge: UHR = 16; HC = 16.343 UHR-NP; 10 UHR-PAP = 59%; mood stabilizer = 40%CVLT, WMS, WCST, LNS, CPT, verbal fluency, trail making

Note: UHR, ultra-high risk; FEP, first-episode psychosis; HC, healthy controls; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; SGA, second-generation antipsychotics; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; TMT, trail making test; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; CPT, continuous performance test; FES, first-episode schizophrenia; SANS, Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms; AP, antipsychotics; FGA, first-generation antipsychotics; CVLT, California verbal learning test; HSCT, Hayling sentence completion test; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; SAPS, Schedule for Assessment of Positive Symptoms; SWM, spatial working memory; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; LM, logical memory; PAL, paired associate learning; LNS, letter number sequencing; PASAT, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test; UHR-P, transition to psychosis in follow-up; UHR-NP, no transition to psychosis in follow-up; SIPS, Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes; SOPS, Scale of Prodromal Symptoms; AD, antidepressants; PACE, Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation; VMI, Verbal Memory Index; HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test. Studies with bold characters indicate main samples included in the meta-analysis.

In the case of multiple publications from overlapping samples, the study with the largest sample size was included. Data from 3 other studies were used for duration of follow-up34,35 or UHR-P vs UHR-NP36 analyses or to examine cognitive tasks36 that were not examined by main study. A total of 47 studies met the inclusion criteria. Of these, 22 studies were excluded due to (a) sample overlap, (b) not reporting sufficient data to calculate effect sizes, (c) reassessing FEP sample more than 5 years after, and (d) including early-onset schizophrenia. A complete list of excluded studies and reasons for exclusion for each study and a low diagram are reported in the online Supplementary Data. Finally, 25 studies were included in our meta-analysis (table 1).34–58

Participants

A total of 17 FEP (905 subjects; 14 studies, 65.8% males, age 24.5), 14 UHR (560 subjects; 9 studies, 53.6% males, age 18.0), and 11 control (405 subjects, 11 studies, 52.7% males, age 23.6) main samples were included. UHR subjects were younger, and FEP had higher ratio of males. Data from additional 3 studies with overlapping samples were used for subgroup analyses.41,45,48 A preliminary meta-analysis to examine the effect of outcome on UHR was also conducted based on only 4 studies.

Cognitive Measures

We combined individual tasks into the broader cognitive domains of verbal memory, visual memory, executive functions, fluency, attention, and verbal working memory. This step was undertaken because there were not sufficient studies to perform meta-analyses for all individual tasks (see online Supplementary Data). Visual working memory was not included as there were not sufficient studies in FEP and healthy control groups. In addition to cognitive domain analyses, task-specific analyses were conducted when at least 3 independent studies had employed a given task (e.g., trail making task). Individual tasks that were analyzed separately included list learning, Wisconsin Cart Sorting Test (WCST) perseveration errors, trail making A and B, digit span, symbol coding, Stroop interference, continuous performance test (CPT) d sensitivity score, letter number sequencing, and letter fluency.

Statistical Analyses

Meta-analyses were performed using MIX software version 1.7 on a Windows platform.59 For each cognitive task, an effect size and standard error were estimated. Effect sizes were weighted using the inverse variance method, and a random effects model (DerSimonian–Laird estimate) was used because the distributions of effect sizes were heterogeneous for number of variables. For studies that reported more than 1 cognitive task for each domain, a pooled effect size was calculated. The Q test was used to measure the heterogeneity of the distribution of effect sizes. When the Q test was significant, “I2” (a measure of the degree of inconsistency in the results of the studies) was used to quantify heterogeneity.60I2 estimates the percentage of total variation across studies, which is due to heterogeneity rather than chance. I2 values between 0 and 0.25 suggest small magnitudes of heterogeneity, whereas I2 values in the range 0.25–0.50 suggest medium magnitudes and those > 0.50 indicate large magnitudes. Publication bias was assessed by Egger’s test. We also calculated homogeneity statistics using Qbet to test the differences between cognitive changes in diagnostic groups (FEP, UHR, and controls) and the effect of follow-up duration in FEP (1, 2, and 3–5 y) and the effect of quality measures.

Meta-regression analyses were conducted for age, gender (male ratio), duration of follow-up, education (years), transition rate to psychosis at follow-up (UHR), change in positive and negative symptoms (effect size of change from baseline to reassessment), baseline positive and negative symptoms based on Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. Meta-regression analyses (weighted generalized least squares regressions) were conducted using SPSS version 11.0 (SPSS Inc). Meta-regression analyses performed with a random effects model were conducted using the restricted-information maximum-likelihood method with a significance level set at P < .05.

Results

Global Cognition

Meta-analysis of global cognition scores showed that performances of all 3 groups (FEP, UHR, and healthy controls) significantly improved over time (table 2). Distribution of effect sizes was very homogenous for each of the 3 groups (I2 = 0). There was no evidence of publication bias either for global cognition or for other cognitive domains (table 2).

Table 2.

Mean Weighted Effect Sizes for Cognitive Changes in FEP, UHR, and HCs

TestSamplenD95% CIZPQ Test, PI2Bias
Global
 FEP179050.300.20–0.396.11<.001.5400.15
 UHR145600.230.11–0.353.86<.001.9500.67
 Con114050.380.24–0.525.23<.001.9400.81
Processing speed
 FEP126270.190.08–0.303.48<.001.8400.93
 UHR92420.180.0–0.361.95.05.6400.42
 Con82990.380.21–0.544.48<.001.8500.98
Trail A
 FEP64470.250.11–0.393.44<.001.350
 UHR61410.300.06–0.542.46.01.740
 Con51670.470.25–0.694.13<.001.580
Symbol coding
 FEP62540.05−0.34–0.430.23.82<.0010.17
 Con3840.21−0.10–0.511.33.18.950
Stroop
 FEP32520.12−0.06–0.291.26.21.770
 Con31280.18−0.14–0.501.11.27.210.03
Trail B
 FEP95020.190.07–0.322.98.003.750
 UHR51410.08−0.38–0.530.33.74.010.20
 Con41330.450.12–0.772.69.007.210.04
Verbal memory
 FEP117020.330.19–0.474.67<.001.140.020.54
 UHR125320.310.12–0.513.11.002.020.060.60
 Con103380.350.17–0.533.86<.001.260.020.97
Learning
 FEP73650.260.11–0.403.36<.001.420
 UHR92300.340.15–0.523.52<.001.520
 Con61790.300.09–0.522.81.005.850
 Delayed
Visual memory
 FEP105740.270.06–0.482.54.01.0010.070.91
 UHR5920.34−0.02–0.701.86.06.250.040.79
 Con62280.450.16–0.733.03.002.060.060.71
Executive function
 FEP126780.380.20–0.564.15<.001.0060.050.09
 UHR52080.370.17–0.563.68<.001.9900.31
 Con62650.390.13–0.652.97.003.060.050.07
WCST per
 FEP105530.430.18–0.683.32<.001<.0010.11
 UHR31010.400.12–0.692.76.006.970
 Con51940.600.39–0.805.72<.001.780
Verbal WM
 FEP105030.13−0.03–0.281.63.10.200.020.27
 UHR82240.200.01–0.392.10.04.9700.97
 Con72680.340.16–0.513.80<.001.7900.62
Digit span
 FEP72770.220.05–0.392.51.01.680.01
 Con31170.24−0.02–0.51.78.07.820
LNS
 UHR51730.21−0.01–0.421.87.06.830
 HC3800.410.09–0.732.53.01.550
Attention
 FEP86200.270.12–0.423.58<.001.140.020.27
 UHR82190.330.14–0.522.80<.001.8700.48
 Con71550.270.08–0.462.77.006.5700.11
CPT d
 FEP43380.230.0–0.491.95.05.110.03
 UHR82190.330.14–0.523.39<.001.870
 Con41320.340.09–0.582.70.007.630
Fluency
 FEP125750.140.01–0.271.99.04.150.020.05
 UHR102350.03−0.15–0.200.30.76.9700.99
 Con93640.310.14–0.493.53<.001.230.020.61
Letter fluency
 FEP115450.08−0.05–0.211.26.21.250.01
 UHR71100.07−0.19–0.340.54.59.940
 Con72650.260.08–0.442.88.004.600
TestSamplenD95% CIZPQ Test, PI2Bias
Global
 FEP179050.300.20–0.396.11<.001.5400.15
 UHR145600.230.11–0.353.86<.001.9500.67
 Con114050.380.24–0.525.23<.001.9400.81
Processing speed
 FEP126270.190.08–0.303.48<.001.8400.93
 UHR92420.180.0–0.361.95.05.6400.42
 Con82990.380.21–0.544.48<.001.8500.98
Trail A
 FEP64470.250.11–0.393.44<.001.350
 UHR61410.300.06–0.542.46.01.740
 Con51670.470.25–0.694.13<.001.580
Symbol coding
 FEP62540.05−0.34–0.430.23.82<.0010.17
 Con3840.21−0.10–0.511.33.18.950
Stroop
 FEP32520.12−0.06–0.291.26.21.770
 Con31280.18−0.14–0.501.11.27.210.03
Trail B
 FEP95020.190.07–0.322.98.003.750
 UHR51410.08−0.38–0.530.33.74.010.20
 Con41330.450.12–0.772.69.007.210.04
Verbal memory
 FEP117020.330.19–0.474.67<.001.140.020.54
 UHR125320.310.12–0.513.11.002.020.060.60
 Con103380.350.17–0.533.86<.001.260.020.97
Learning
 FEP73650.260.11–0.403.36<.001.420
 UHR92300.340.15–0.523.52<.001.520
 Con61790.300.09–0.522.81.005.850
 Delayed
Visual memory
 FEP105740.270.06–0.482.54.01.0010.070.91
 UHR5920.34−0.02–0.701.86.06.250.040.79
 Con62280.450.16–0.733.03.002.060.060.71
Executive function
 FEP126780.380.20–0.564.15<.001.0060.050.09
 UHR52080.370.17–0.563.68<.001.9900.31
 Con62650.390.13–0.652.97.003.060.050.07
WCST per
 FEP105530.430.18–0.683.32<.001<.0010.11
 UHR31010.400.12–0.692.76.006.970
 Con51940.600.39–0.805.72<.001.780
Verbal WM
 FEP105030.13−0.03–0.281.63.10.200.020.27
 UHR82240.200.01–0.392.10.04.9700.97
 Con72680.340.16–0.513.80<.001.7900.62
Digit span
 FEP72770.220.05–0.392.51.01.680.01
 Con31170.24−0.02–0.51.78.07.820
LNS
 UHR51730.21−0.01–0.421.87.06.830
 HC3800.410.09–0.732.53.01.550
Attention
 FEP86200.270.12–0.423.58<.001.140.020.27
 UHR82190.330.14–0.522.80<.001.8700.48
 Con71550.270.08–0.462.77.006.5700.11
CPT d
 FEP43380.230.0–0.491.95.05.110.03
 UHR82190.330.14–0.523.39<.001.870
 Con41320.340.09–0.582.70.007.630
Fluency
 FEP125750.140.01–0.271.99.04.150.020.05
 UHR102350.03−0.15–0.200.30.76.9700.99
 Con93640.310.14–0.493.53<.001.230.020.61
Letter fluency
 FEP115450.08−0.05–0.211.26.21.250.01
 UHR71100.07−0.19–0.340.54.59.940
 Con72650.260.08–0.442.88.004.600

Note: Abbreviations are explained in the first footnote to table 1. Con, healthy controls; D, Cohen D; Bias, P value of Egger’s test; WM, working memory. Domain names are represented in bold.

Table 2.

Mean Weighted Effect Sizes for Cognitive Changes in FEP, UHR, and HCs

TestSamplenD95% CIZPQ Test, PI2Bias
Global
 FEP179050.300.20–0.396.11<.001.5400.15
 UHR145600.230.11–0.353.86<.001.9500.67
 Con114050.380.24–0.525.23<.001.9400.81
Processing speed
 FEP126270.190.08–0.303.48<.001.8400.93
 UHR92420.180.0–0.361.95.05.6400.42
 Con82990.380.21–0.544.48<.001.8500.98
Trail A
 FEP64470.250.11–0.393.44<.001.350
 UHR61410.300.06–0.542.46.01.740
 Con51670.470.25–0.694.13<.001.580
Symbol coding
 FEP62540.05−0.34–0.430.23.82<.0010.17
 Con3840.21−0.10–0.511.33.18.950
Stroop
 FEP32520.12−0.06–0.291.26.21.770
 Con31280.18−0.14–0.501.11.27.210.03
Trail B
 FEP95020.190.07–0.322.98.003.750
 UHR51410.08−0.38–0.530.33.74.010.20
 Con41330.450.12–0.772.69.007.210.04
Verbal memory
 FEP117020.330.19–0.474.67<.001.140.020.54
 UHR125320.310.12–0.513.11.002.020.060.60
 Con103380.350.17–0.533.86<.001.260.020.97
Learning
 FEP73650.260.11–0.403.36<.001.420
 UHR92300.340.15–0.523.52<.001.520
 Con61790.300.09–0.522.81.005.850
 Delayed
Visual memory
 FEP105740.270.06–0.482.54.01.0010.070.91
 UHR5920.34−0.02–0.701.86.06.250.040.79
 Con62280.450.16–0.733.03.002.060.060.71
Executive function
 FEP126780.380.20–0.564.15<.001.0060.050.09
 UHR52080.370.17–0.563.68<.001.9900.31
 Con62650.390.13–0.652.97.003.060.050.07
WCST per
 FEP105530.430.18–0.683.32<.001<.0010.11
 UHR31010.400.12–0.692.76.006.970
 Con51940.600.39–0.805.72<.001.780
Verbal WM
 FEP105030.13−0.03–0.281.63.10.200.020.27
 UHR82240.200.01–0.392.10.04.9700.97
 Con72680.340.16–0.513.80<.001.7900.62
Digit span
 FEP72770.220.05–0.392.51.01.680.01
 Con31170.24−0.02–0.51.78.07.820
LNS
 UHR51730.21−0.01–0.421.87.06.830
 HC3800.410.09–0.732.53.01.550
Attention
 FEP86200.270.12–0.423.58<.001.140.020.27
 UHR82190.330.14–0.522.80<.001.8700.48
 Con71550.270.08–0.462.77.006.5700.11
CPT d
 FEP43380.230.0–0.491.95.05.110.03
 UHR82190.330.14–0.523.39<.001.870
 Con41320.340.09–0.582.70.007.630
Fluency
 FEP125750.140.01–0.271.99.04.150.020.05
 UHR102350.03−0.15–0.200.30.76.9700.99
 Con93640.310.14–0.493.53<.001.230.020.61
Letter fluency
 FEP115450.08−0.05–0.211.26.21.250.01
 UHR71100.07−0.19–0.340.54.59.940
 Con72650.260.08–0.442.88.004.600
TestSamplenD95% CIZPQ Test, PI2Bias
Global
 FEP179050.300.20–0.396.11<.001.5400.15
 UHR145600.230.11–0.353.86<.001.9500.67
 Con114050.380.24–0.525.23<.001.9400.81
Processing speed
 FEP126270.190.08–0.303.48<.001.8400.93
 UHR92420.180.0–0.361.95.05.6400.42
 Con82990.380.21–0.544.48<.001.8500.98
Trail A
 FEP64470.250.11–0.393.44<.001.350
 UHR61410.300.06–0.542.46.01.740
 Con51670.470.25–0.694.13<.001.580
Symbol coding
 FEP62540.05−0.34–0.430.23.82<.0010.17
 Con3840.21−0.10–0.511.33.18.950
Stroop
 FEP32520.12−0.06–0.291.26.21.770
 Con31280.18−0.14–0.501.11.27.210.03
Trail B
 FEP95020.190.07–0.322.98.003.750
 UHR51410.08−0.38–0.530.33.74.010.20
 Con41330.450.12–0.772.69.007.210.04
Verbal memory
 FEP117020.330.19–0.474.67<.001.140.020.54
 UHR125320.310.12–0.513.11.002.020.060.60
 Con103380.350.17–0.533.86<.001.260.020.97
Learning
 FEP73650.260.11–0.403.36<.001.420
 UHR92300.340.15–0.523.52<.001.520
 Con61790.300.09–0.522.81.005.850
 Delayed
Visual memory
 FEP105740.270.06–0.482.54.01.0010.070.91
 UHR5920.34−0.02–0.701.86.06.250.040.79
 Con62280.450.16–0.733.03.002.060.060.71
Executive function
 FEP126780.380.20–0.564.15<.001.0060.050.09
 UHR52080.370.17–0.563.68<.001.9900.31
 Con62650.390.13–0.652.97.003.060.050.07
WCST per
 FEP105530.430.18–0.683.32<.001<.0010.11
 UHR31010.400.12–0.692.76.006.970
 Con51940.600.39–0.805.72<.001.780
Verbal WM
 FEP105030.13−0.03–0.281.63.10.200.020.27
 UHR82240.200.01–0.392.10.04.9700.97
 Con72680.340.16–0.513.80<.001.7900.62
Digit span
 FEP72770.220.05–0.392.51.01.680.01
 Con31170.24−0.02–0.51.78.07.820
LNS
 UHR51730.21−0.01–0.421.87.06.830
 HC3800.410.09–0.732.53.01.550
Attention
 FEP86200.270.12–0.423.58<.001.140.020.27
 UHR82190.330.14–0.522.80<.001.8700.48
 Con71550.270.08–0.462.77.006.5700.11
CPT d
 FEP43380.230.0–0.491.95.05.110.03
 UHR82190.330.14–0.523.39<.001.870
 Con41320.340.09–0.582.70.007.630
Fluency
 FEP125750.140.01–0.271.99.04.150.020.05
 UHR102350.03−0.15–0.200.30.76.9700.99
 Con93640.310.14–0.493.53<.001.230.020.61
Letter fluency
 FEP115450.08−0.05–0.211.26.21.250.01
 UHR71100.07−0.19–0.340.54.59.940
 Con72650.260.08–0.442.88.004.600

Note: Abbreviations are explained in the first footnote to table 1. Con, healthy controls; D, Cohen D; Bias, P value of Egger’s test; WM, working memory. Domain names are represented in bold.

Meta-analysis of FEP gave a very similar outcome when this analysis was restricted to samples that include only schizophrenia subjects (d = 0.24, CI = 0.04–0.43, Z = 2.36, P = .02). The magnitude of cognitive improvement observed at 1-year follow-up (d = 0.34, CI = 0.23–0.45, P < .001) was not statistically different from that at 2 years follow-up (d = 0.33, CI = 0.15–0.51, P < .001) (Qbet = 0.04, P = .84) and 3–5 (d = 0.27, CI = 0.07–0.47, P = .001) (Qbet = 0.25, P = .62). Sensitivity analyses did not suggest significant effects of quality measures (substance abuse exclusion criteria, structured clinical interview for diagnosis) on results (see online Supplementary Data).

In meta-regression analyses, there were no effects of the male/female ratio, education, transition rate, or the duration of follow-up on longitudinal cognitive changes (see online Supplementary Data).

Cognitive Domains

First-Episode Psychosis.

There were significant improvements in performance of FEP in verbal memory (d = 0.33), visual memory (d = 0.27), executive function (d = 0.38), processing speed (d = 0.19), attention (d = 0.26), and fluency (d = 014) (table 2 and figure 1). Only in the verbal working memory domain was improvement (d = 0.13) not significant. Heterogeneity for the distribution of effect sizes was very minimal for each of these domains (I2 = 0–0.07). In individual task analyses, there were significant improvements in list learning (d = 0.26), trail making A (d = 0.25) and B (d = 0.19), WCST per se errors (d = 0.43) and CPT d sensitivity score (d = 0.23). Heterogeneity for the distribution of effect sizes was minimal for each of the individual tasks (I2 = 0–0.17).

Fig. 1.

Cognitive improvement at follow-up in ultra-high risk (UHR), first-episode psychosis (FEP), and healthy controls.

Ultra-high Risk.

The pattern and magnitude of cognitive improvements were quite similar to FEP (figure 1). These cognitive improvements reached significance in attention (d = 0.31), verbal memory (d = 0.39), verbal working memory (d = 0.20), processing speed (d = 0.22), executive function (d = 0.36) domains (table 1). Heterogeneity for the distribution of effect sizes was very minimal for each of the 7 domains (I2 = 0–0.05). In individual task analyses, there were significant improvements in list learning (d = 0.33) and CPT d (d = 0.31). Distribution of effect sizes was very homogenous for individual tasks (I2 = 0–0.01) except trail making B test (I2 = 0.20).

Healthy Controls.

Performances of healthy controls significantly improved in all cognitive domains (d = 0.27–0.45) (table 2 and figure 1). Heterogeneity for the distribution of effect sizes was very minimal for each of these domains (I2 = 0–0.06). Individual task analyses showed significant (d = 0.26–0.59) improvements for list learning, trail making A and B, WCST per errors, CPT d sensitivity, and letter fluency. Heterogeneity for the distribution of effect sizes was very minimal for each of these domains (I2 = 0–0.04).

Cognitive Improvement in FEP and UHR in Comparison With Healthy Controls

Magnitude of improvement was not significantly different across groups for cognitive domains other than fluency and verbal working memory. In verbal working memory, improvement was significantly more pronounced in healthy controls than FEP (Qbet = 4.10, P = .04). In the fluency domain, improvement in performance was significantly more pronounced in healthy controls than FEP (Qbet = 4.9, P = .03) and UHR (Qbet = 6.2, P = .01).

The Effect of Changes in Symptoms on Cognition

Reduction in negative symptoms was significantly associated with greater improvement in executive functions and verbal working memory at follow-up of FEP. Decrease in positive symptoms was associated with improvement of visual memory performance (see online Supplementary Data). It was not possible to do a similar analysis in UHR as relevant data were not reported in most studies.

Effect of Outcome of UHR on Longitudinal Cognitive Changes

Only 5 published studies reported baseline cognitive performances of UHR-P and UHR-NP samples, and only 1 of these studies reported cognitive decline or less cognitive improvement in UHR-P than in UHR-NP in some domains.36 Four of these studies are included into the current meta-analysis, and there was no significant difference of longitudinal cognitive changes of UHR-P (d = 0.09, CI = −0.33 to 0.50, Z = 0.40, P = .69) and UHR-NP (d = 0.18, CI = −0.11 to 0.47, Z = 1.19, P = .23) (Qbet = 0.13, P = .72). Also transition rate to psychosis had no significant effect on cognitive change (see online Supplementary Data).

Medication Effects

In a meta-regression analysis, the ratio of patients taking antipsychotics was not significantly associated with longitudinal cognitive changes. In 3 studies, FEP patients were antipsychotic naive at the baseline, and these studies had a more pronounced improvement in global cognition at follow-up (d = 0.48, CI = 0.27–0.69, Z = 4.5, P < .001) (Qbet = 4.45, P = .03); however, 2 of these studies also reported the largest scale reductions in positive symptoms among all other studies (d = 3.3 and 4.0).

Meta-regression analyses of the percentage of patients receiving first-generation antipsychotics (FGA) were also not significant. In 3 studies, all patients were treated with FGA, and the magnitude of longitudinal change in cognition was not significantly different from that in other studies (d = 0.38, CI = 0.06–0.72, Z = 2.29, P = .02, Qbet = 0.45, P = .50).

It was not possible to quantitatively analyze effects of medication on cognitive change in the UHR group due to lack of reported information. In one of the UHR studies, patients taking second-generation antipsychotics had no improvement in cognition unlike other patients who took antidepressants or were medication naive.38

Discussion

The aim of this present meta-analysis was to investigate whether there is a cognitive decline over, or after, the onset of FEP. Our findings suggest that there is no evidence of such deterioration in follow-up studies of FEP and UHR. Indeed, as with healthy control subjects, there are improvements in cognitive abilities in both groups. These findings do not support neuroprogressive or staging models of schizophrenia.

Studies of FEP samples clearly showed no decline within 5 years after the onset of psychosis. FEP studies not included in this meta-analysis have shown similar findings, including samples followed for 10 years.61 Analysis of those studies that included only patients with first-episode schizophrenia gave a very similar result, suggesting that cognitive trajectories of different diagnoses within FEP are likely to be similar. However, more studies examining first-episode affective psychoses are necessary. Together with the data in established schizophrenia,24,25 these findings suggest that there is no evidence of the loss of acquired cognitive skills after the onset of psychosis. Many of the studies assessed cognition in multiple points (some also included additional early assessment 6–12 wk after first assessment), and practice effects are likely to play a significant role in cognitive improvements observed in all 3 groups. In accordance with this view, the most consistent improvements across groups were observed in tasks with significant practice effects (i.e., WCST, memory tasks), and less consistent improvements were observed in tasks with poor practice effects (i.e., letter fluency).62,63 Also the pattern of cognitive improvement seem to fit the well-known pattern of practice effects (relatively substantial early improvement followed by a plateau).45,51,52 In addition to practice effects, reductions in symptoms are likely to contribute to cognitive improvements in FEP as, in most FEP studies, there is stabilization of symptoms at follow-up. Our findings showing an association between reduction in negative symptoms (likely to be secondary negative symptoms) and improvement in working memory and executive functions, and relationship between improvement of positive symptoms and visual memory, supports this argument.

Similarly, studies in UHR subjects found no cognitive decline, thus failing to support the idea of a critical cognitive decline before the onset of psychosis. Some argue that “cognitive decline” in UHR might be specific to UHR-P. However, so far only 1 study has supported such an argument, and there were no significant differences in longitudinal cognitive changes between UHR-P and UHR-NP in this meta-analysis.36 It should be noted that in Wood et al,36 all patients in UHR-P but only 1 of UHR-NP subjects were treated with antipsychotics. This might be an important consideration given that Bowie et al38 found that antipsychotic use in UHR was associated with relatively negative effects on cognition at follow-up unlike those UHR subjects with similar characteristics, who were untreated or taking antidepressants. Therefore, current evidence does not support a cognitive decline in UHR-P subjects over the onset of psychosis. However, it should be noted that the current meta-analysis might be underpowered to detect small differences among longitudinal changes in UHR-P and UHR-NP due to the small number of studies included. Therefore, future studies examining longitudinal changes in cognition should include larger sample sizes that control for medication and symptoms.

The evidence regarding stability of cognitive functions before and after onset of psychosis contradicts the idea that schizophrenia is a progressive dementia. Findings of longitudinal brain imaging studies in schizophrenia have been interpreted by many as indicating a progressive brain disorder. However, lack of cognitive decline in prodromal and first-episode patients raises important questions regarding the nature of the supposedly “progressive” brain imaging abnormalities reported in UHR and FEP, as well as in chronic schizophrenia samples.64–67 These structural changes might be related to factors other than neurodegenerative processes intrinsic to the illness. One potential factor is the effect of antipsychotics as long-term treatment with these medications has been associated with cortical gray matter reductions.68 Also these findings might be reflections of normal but delayed cortical changes as gray matter reduction is a normal part of brain development. Other factors such as decreased environmental stimuli related to social isolation might also play a role.

Our findings support a neurodevelopmental model rather than neurodegenerative and related staging models of schizophrenia. Available evidence suggests that cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are already evident before the onset of FEP. However, timing and developmental trajectories of cognitive abnormalities in schizophrenia need to be addressed as there is relative increase of patient-control cognitive differences with age.27 In a substantial minority of the cases, these deficits develop quite early as moderate, and borderline intellectual disabilities are evident in many individuals with schizophrenia.7,69 In others, cognitive abnormalities seem to be more subtle and become evident later in development during late childhood and early adolescence.70,71 Cognitive deficits observed in schizophrenia seem to be best explained by problems in acquisition during neurodevelopment.

Limitations of this meta-analysis include differences in methodology, such as follow-up duration, different versions of cognitive tests used, as well as the small number of studies included in the UHR meta-analysis. Furthermore, many studies did not report variables that might have affected cognition such as positive/negative symptoms, functioning, cannabis, and other drug use. Unlike FEP, we were not able to examine the effect of symptom change on cognition in UHR. Also some might consider that FEP is a heterogeneous group, and cognitive decline might be evident only in first-episode schizophrenia. However, our findings showed no cognitive decline in those studies that only examined schizophrenia either. Advantages of this study include being the first meta-analysis of longitudinal cognitive changes in FEP and UHR, examination of confounding factors, and homogenous distribution of effect sizes.

In conclusion, the present meta-analysis of follow-up studies of cognition in FEP and UHR provided no evidence of cognitive decline. It is likely that cognitive abnormalities in schizophrenia develop long before the onset of the FEP as a result of abnormalities in neurodevelopment.

Acknowledgments

The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study. Dr Murray had received honoraria for lectures from Astra-Zenica, Janssen, Lilly, Roche, and BMS.

References

1.

Gold
JM
Harvey
PD
.
Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia
.
Psychiatr Clin North Am
.
1993
;
16
:
295
312
.

2.

Heinrichs
RW
Zakzanis
KK
.
Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence
.
Neuropsychology
.
1998
;
12
:
426
445
.

3.

Bora
E
Yücel
M
Pantelis
C
.
Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and affective psychoses: implications for DSM-V criteria and beyond
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2010
;
36
:
36
42
.

4.

Weinberger
DR
.
The pathogenesis of schizophrenia: a neurodevelopmental theory
. In:
Nasrallah
RA
Weinberger
DR
, eds.
The Neurology of Schizophrenia
. Amsterdam, the Netherlands:
Elsevier
;
1986
:
387
405
.

5.

Murray
RM
Lewis
SW
.
Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental disorder?
BMJ (Clin Res Ed)
.
1987
;
295
:
681
682
.

6.

Zipursky
RB
Reilly
TJ
Murray
RM
.
The Myth of Schizophrenia as a Progressive Brain Disease
.
Schizophr Bull
. In press.

7.

Reichenberg
A
Caspi
A
Harrington
H
et al. 
Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
2010
;
167
:
160
169
.

8.

Maccabe
JH
.
Population-based cohort studies on premorbid cognitive function in schizophrenia
.
Epidemiol Rev
.
2008
;
30
:
77
83
.

9.

Woodberry
KA
Giuliano
AJ
Seidman
LJ
.
Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
2008
;
165
:
579
587
.

10.

Simon
AE
Cattapan-Ludewig
K
Zmilacher
S
et al. 
Cognitive functioning in the schizophrenia prodrome
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2007
;
33
:
761
771
.

11.

Pantelis
C
Wood
SJ
Proffitt
TM
et al. 
Attentional set-shifting ability in first-episode and established schizophrenia: relationship to working memory
.
Schizophr Res
.
2009
;
112
:
104
113
.

12.

Harrison
PJ
.
The neuropathology of schizophrenia. A critical review of the data and their interpretation
.
Brain
.
1999
;
122 (Pt 4)
:
593
624
.

13.

Meyer
U
.
Developmental neuroinflammation and schizophrenia
.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry
.
2013
;
42
:
20
34
.

14.

Monji
A
Kato
TA
Mizoguchi
Y
et al. 
Neuroinflammation in schizophrenia especially focused on the role of microglia
.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry
.
2013
;
42
:
115
121
.

15.

Howes
OD
Fusar-Poli
P
Bloomfield
M
Selvaraj
S
McGuire
P
.
From the prodrome to chronic schizophrenia: the neurobiology underlying psychotic symptoms and cognitive impairments
.
Curr Pharm Des
.
2012
;
18
:
459
465
.

16.

Keshavan
MS
Anderson
S
Pettegrew
JW
.
Is schizophrenia due to excessive synaptic pruning in the prefrontal cortex? The Feinberg hypothesis revisited
.
J Psychiatr Res
.
1994
;
28
:
239
265
.

17.

Feinberg
I
.
Schizophrenia: caused by a fault in programmed synaptic elimination during adolescence?
J Psychiatr Res
.
1982
;
17
:
319
334
.

18.

McGlashan
TH
.
A selective review of recent North American long-term followup studies of schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1988
;
14
:
515
542
.

19.

McGlashan
TH
Fenton
WS
.
Subtype progression and pathophysiologic deterioration in early schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1993
;
19
:
71
84
.

20.

McGorry
PD
Hickie
IB
Yung
AR
Pantelis
C
Jackson
HJ
.
Clinical staging of psychiatric disorders: a heuristic framework for choosing earlier, safer and more effective interventions
.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry
.
2006
;
40
:
616
622
.

21.

Giuliano
AJ
Li
H
Mesholam-Gately
RI
Sorenson
SM
Woodberry
KA
Seidman
LJ
.
Neurocognition in the psychosis risk syndrome: a quantitative and qualitative review
.
Curr Pharm Des
.
2012
;
18
:
399
415
.

22.

Fusar-Poli
P
Deste
G
Smieskova
R
et al. 
Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
.
2012
;
69
:
562
571
.

23.

Mesholam-Gately
RI
Giuliano
AJ
Goff
KP
Faraone
SV
Seidman
LJ
.
Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review
.
Neuropsychology
.
2009
;
23
:
315
336
.

24.

Szöke
A
Trandafir
A
Dupont
ME
Méary
A
Schürhoff
F
Leboyer
M
.
Longitudinal studies of cognition in schizophrenia: meta-analysis
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
2008
;
192
:
248
257
.

25.

Irani
F
Kalkstein
S
Moberg
EA
Moberg
PJ
.
Neuro psychological performance in older patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2011
;
37
:
1318
1326
.

26.

Hedman
AM
van Haren
NE
van Baal
CG
Kahn
RS
Hulshoff Pol
HE
.
IQ change over time in schizophrenia and healthy individuals: a meta-analysis
.
Schizophr Res
.
2013
;
146
:
201
208
.

27.

McIntosh
AM
Gow
A
Luciano
M
et al. 
Polygenic risk for schizophrenia is associated with cognitive change between childhood and old age
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2013
;
73
:
938
943
. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.011
.

28.

Bozikas
VP
Andreou
C
.
Longitudinal studies of cognition in first episode psychosis: a systematic review of the literature
.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry
.
2011
;
45
:
93
108
.

29.

Addington
J
Barbato
M
.
The role of cognitive functioning in the outcome of those at clinical high risk for developing psychosis
.
Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci
.
2012
;
21
:
335
342
.

30.

Lin
A
Reniers
RL
Wood
SJ
.
Clinical staging in severe mental disorder: evidence from neurocognition and neuroimaging
.
Br J Psychiatry Suppl
.
2013
;
54
:
s11
s17
.

31.

Napal
O
Ojeda
N
Elizagárate
E
Peña
J
Ezcurra
J
Gutiérrez
M
.
The course of the schizophrenia and its impact on cognition: a review of literature
.
Actas Esp Psiquiatr
.
2012
;
40
:
198
220
.

32.

Lewandowski
KE
Cohen
BM
Ongur
D
.
Evolution of neuropsychological dysfunction during the course of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder
.
Psychol Med
.
2011
;
41
:
225
241
.

33.

Stroup
DF
Berlin
JA
Morton
SC
et al. 
Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group
.
JAMA
.
2000
;
283
:
2008
2012
.

34.

Albus
M
Hubmann
W
Scherer
J
et al. 
A prospective 2-year follow-up study of neurocognitive functioning in patients with first-episode schizophrenia
.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
.
2002
;
252
:
262
267
.

35.

Nopoulos
P
Flashman
L
Flaum
M
Arndt
S
Andreasen
N
.
Stability of cognitive functioning early in the course of schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res
.
1994
;
14
:
29
37
.

36.

Wood
SJ
Brewer
WJ
Koutsouradis
P
et al. 
Cognitive decline following psychosis onset: data from the PACE clinic
.
Br J Psychiatry Suppl
.
2007
;
51
:
s52
s57
.

37.

Keefe
RS
Perkins
DO
Gu
H
Zipursky
RB
Christensen
BK
Lieberman
JA
.
A longitudinal study of neurocognitive function in individuals at-risk for psychosis
.
Schizophr Res
.
2006
;
88
:
26
35
.

38.

Bowie
CR
McLaughlin
D
Carrión
RE
Auther
AM
Cornblatt
BA
.
Cognitive changes following antidepressant or antipsychotic treatment in adolescents at clinical risk for psychosis
.
Schizophr Res
.
2012
;
137
:
110
117
.

39.

Becker
HE
Nieman
DH
Wiltink
S
et al. 
Neurocognitive functioning before and after the first psychotic episode: does psychosis result in cognitive deterioration?
Psychol Med
.
2010
;
40
:
1599
1606
.

40.

Jahshan
C
Heaton
RK
Golshan
S
Cadenhead
KS
.
Course of neurocognitive deficits in the prodrome and first episode of schizophrenia
.
Neuropsychology
.
2010
;
24
:
109
120
.

41.

Niendam
TA
Bearden
CE
Zinberg
J
Johnson
JK
O’Brien
M
Cannon
TD
.
The course of neurocognition and social functioning in individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2007
;
33
:
772
781
.

42.

Lin
A
Wood
SJ
Nelson
B
et al. 
Neurocognitive predictors of functional outcome two to 13 years after identification as ultra-high risk for psychosis
.
Schizophr Res
.
2011
;
132
:
1
7
.

43.

Barbato
M
Colijn
MA
Keefe
RS
et al. 
The course of cognitive functioning over six months in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2013
;
206
:
195
199
.

44.

Keefe
RS
Seidman
LJ
Christensen
BK
et al. 
HGDH Research Group
.
Long-term neurocognitive effects of olanzapine or low-dose haloperidol in first-episode psychosis
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2006
;
59
:
97
105
.

45.

Chan
KK
Xu
JQ
Liu
KC
Hui
CL
Wong
GH
Chen
EY
.
Executive function in first-episode schizophrenia: a three-year prospective study of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test
.
Schizophr Res
.
2012
;
135
:
62
67
.

46.

Rund
BR
Melle
I
Friis
S
et al. 
The course of neurocognitive functioning in first-episode psychosis and its relation to premorbid adjustment, duration of untreated psychosis, and relapse
.
Schizophr Res
.
2007
;
91
:
132
140
.

47.

Peña
J
Ojeda
N
Segarra
R
Eguiluz
JI
García
J
Gutiérrez
M
.
Executive functioning correctly classified diagnoses in patients with first-episode psychosis: evidence from a 2-year longitudinal study
.
Schizophr Res
.
2011
;
126
:
77
80
.

48.

Addington
J
Saeedi
H
Addington
D
.
The course of cognitive functioning in first episode psychosis: changes over time and impact on outcome
.
Schizophr Res
.
2005
;
78
:
35
43
.

49.

Rodríguez-Sánchez
JM
Pérez-Iglesias
R
González-Blanch
C
et al. 
1-year follow-up study of cognitive function in first-episode non-affective psychosis
.
Schizophr Res
.
2008
;
104
:
165
174
.

50.

Albus
M
Hubmann
W
Mohr
F
et al. 
Neurocognitive functioning in patients with first-episode schizophrenia: results of a prospective 5-year follow-up study
.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
.
2006
;
256
:
442
451
.

51.

Hill
SK
Schuepbach
D
Herbener
ES
Keshavan
MS
Sweeney
JA
.
Pretreatment and longitudinal studies of neuropsychological deficits in antipsychotic-naïve patients with schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res
.
2004
;
68
:
49
63
.

52.

Kopala
LC
Good
KP
Milliken
H
et al. 
Treatment of a first episode of psychotic illness with quetiapine: an analysis of 2 year outcomes
.
Schizophr Res
.
2006
;
81
:
29
39
.

53.

Townsend
LA
Norman
RM
Malla
AK
Rychlo
AD
Ahmed
RR
.
Changes in cognitive functioning following comprehensive treatment for first episode patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2002
;
113
:
69
81
.

54.

Gold
S
Arndt
S
Nopoulos
P
O’Leary
DS
Andreasen
NC
.
Longitudinal study of cognitive function in first-episode and recent-onset schizophrenia
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1999
;
156
:
1342
1348
.

55.

The Scottish Schizophrenia Research Group
.
The Scottish First Episode Schizophrenia Study V. One-year follow-up
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1988
;
152
:
470
476
.

56.

Purdon
SE
Jones
BD
Stip
E
et al. 
Neuropsychological change in early phase schizophrenia during 12 months of treatment with olanzapine, risperidone, or haloperidol. The Canadian Collaborative Group for research in schizophrenia
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
.
2000
;
57
:
249
258
.

57.

de Mello Ayres
A
Scazufca
M
Menezes
PR
et al. 
Cognitive functioning in subjects with recent-onset psychosis from a low-middle-income environment: multiple-domain deficits and longitudinal evaluation
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2010
;
179
:
157
164
.

58.

Woodberry
KA
McFarlane
WR
Giuliano
AJ
et al. 
Change in neuropsychological functioning over one year in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis
.
Schizophr Res
.
2013
;
146
:
87
94
. doi:
10.1016/j.schres.2013.01.017
.

59.

Bax
L
Yu
LM
Ikeda
N
Tsuruta
H
Moons
KG
.
Development and validation of MIX: comprehensive free software for meta-analysis of causal research data
.
BMC Med Res Methodol
.
2006
;
6
:
50
.

60.

Higgins
JP
Thompson
SG
.
Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis
.
Stat Med
.
2002
;
21
:
1539
1558
.

61.

Hoff
AL
Svetina
C
Shields
G
Stewart
J
DeLisi
LE
.
Ten year longitudinal study of neuropsychological functioning subsequent to a first episode of schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res
.
2005
;
78
:
27
34
.

62.

Basso
MR
Bornstein
RA
Lang
JM
.
Practice effects on commonly used measures of executive function across twelve months
.
Clin Neuropsychol
.
1999
;
13
:
283
292
.

63.

Basso
MR
Lowery
N
Ghormley
C
Bornstein
RA
.
Practice effects on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-64 Card version across 12 months
.
Clin Neuropsychol
.
2001
;
15
:
471
478
.

64.

Vita
A
De Peri
L
Deste
G
Sacchetti
E
.
Progressive loss of cortical gray matter in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis and meta-regression of longitudinal MRI studies
.
Transl Psychiatry
.
2012
;
2
:
e190
.

65.

Olabi
B
Ellison-Wright
I
McIntosh
AM
Wood
SJ
Bullmore
E
Lawrie
SM
.
Are there progressive brain changes in schizophrenia? A meta-analysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging studies
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2011
;
70
:
88
96
.

66.

Kempton
MJ
Stahl
D
Williams
SC
DeLisi
LE
.
Progressive lateral ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of longitudinal MRI studies
.
Schizophr Res
.
2010
;
120
:
54
62
.

67.

Pantelis
C
Velakoulis
D
McGorry
PD
et al. 
Neuroanatomical abnormalities before and after onset of psychosis: a cross-sectional and longitudinal MRI comparison
.
Lancet
.
2003
;
361
:
281
288
.

68.

Ho
BC
Andreasen
NC
Ziebell
S
Pierson
R
Magnotta
V
.
Long-term antipsychotic treatment and brain volumes: a longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
.
2011
;
68
:
128
137
.

69.

Morgan
VA
Leonard
H
Bourke
J
Jablensky
A
.
Intellectual disability co-occurring with schizophrenia and other psychiatric illness: population-based study
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
2008
;
193
:
364
372
.

70.

Russell
AJ
Munro
JC
Jones
PB
Hemsley
DR
Murray
RM
.
Schizophrenia and the myth of intellectual decline
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1997
;
154
:
635
639
.

71.

Maccabe
JH
Wicks
S
Löfving
S
et al. 
Decline in cognitive performance between ages 13 and 18 years and the risk for psychosis in adulthood: a Swedish Longitudinal Cohort Study in males
.
JAMA Psychiatry
.
2013
;
70
:
261
270
. doi:
10.1001/2013jamapsychiatry.43
.

Supplementary data