We use cookies to enhance your experience on our website. By continuing to use our website, you are agreeing to our use of cookies. You can change your cookie settings at any time. Find out more Behavioral Ecology Skip to Main Content

Predator-elicited visual signal: why the turquoise-browed motmot wag-displays its racketed tail

Behav Ecol (2006) 17 (4): 547-553.
This article has been cited by the following articles in journals that are participating in CrossRef Cited-by Linking.
Tail Movements in Birds—Current Evidence and New Concepts
Ornithological Science (2016) 15 (1): 1.
Motion-triggered defensive display in a tephritid fly
Journal of Ethology (2016) 34 (1): 31.
The fear of unseen predators: ground squirrel tail flagging in the absence of snakes signals vigilance
Behavioral Ecology (2015) 26 (1): 185.
Vocal repertoire of cooperatively breeding Smooth-billed Anis
Journal of Field Ornithology (2015) 86 (2): 130.
How do novel signals originate? The evolution of fairy-wren songs from predator to display contexts
Animal Behaviour (2014) 88: 57.
White tail spots and tail-flicking behavior enhance foraging performance in the Hooded Warbler
The Auk (2014) 131 (2): 141.
Behavioral decisions in sensory landscapes: crayfish use chemical signals to make habitat use choices
Journal of Crustacean Biology (2014) 34 (5): 559.
Same trait, different receiver response: unlike females, male American goldfinches do not signal status with bill colour
Animal Behaviour (2014) 93: 121.
A multifunctional visual display in elegant trogons targets conspecifics and heterospecifics
Behavioral Ecology (2014) 25 (1): 27.
Carotenoid-Based Ornaments of Female and Male American Goldfinches (Spinus tristis) Show Sex-Specific Correlations with Immune Function and Metabolic Rate
Physiological and Biochemical Zoology (2012) 85 (4): 348.
The evolution of female ornaments and weaponry: social selection, sexual selection and ecological competition
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences (2012) 367 (1600): 2274.
It isn't always sexy when both are bright and shiny: considering alternatives to sexual selection in elaborate monomorphic species
Ibis (2012) 154 (3): 439.
Characterisation of Predator-Directed Displays in Tephritid Flies
Ethology (2012) 118 (12): 1165.
Condition and brightness of structural blue-green: motmot tail-racket brightness is related to speed of feather growth in males, but not in females
Biological Journal of the Linnean Society (2012) 106 (3): 673.
Tail Pumping by the Black Phoebe
The Wilson Journal of Ornithology (2011) 123 (4): 766.
Pursuit deterrence, predation risk, and escape in the lizard Callisaurus draconoides
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (2011) 65 (9): 1833.
Origin and maintenance of mechanosensory feather ornaments
Animal Behaviour (2010) 79 (3): 637.
Sex differences in antipredator tail-waving displays of the diurnal yellow-headed gecko Gonatodes albogularis from tropical forests of Colombia
Journal of Ethology (2010) 28 (2): 305.
Tail-racket removal increases hematocrit in male Turquoise-browed Motmots (Eumomota superciliosa)
Journal of Ornithology (2010) 151 (1): 241.
A predator-elicited vocalisation in the Variegated Fairy-wren (Malurus lamberti)
Emu (2010) 110 (2): 165.
Pursuit deterrence varies with predation risks affecting escape behaviour in the lizard Callisaurus draconoides
Animal Behaviour (2010) 80 (2): 249.
Tail pumping by Eastern Phoebes: an honest, persistent predator-deterrent signal?
Journal of Field Ornithology (2009) 80 (2): 163.
Carotenoid-based status signaling by females in the tropical streak-backed oriole
Behavioral Ecology (2009) 20 (5): 1000.
Why some rails have white tails: the evolution of white undertail plumage and anti-predator signaling
Evolutionary Ecology (2009) 23 (6): 943.
Anti-Predator Signals as Advertisements: Evidence in White-Throated Magpie-Jays
Ethology (2009) 115 (6): 522.
Display of an Inedible Prop as a Signal of Aggression? Adaptive Significance of Leaf-Display by the Turquoise-Browed Motmot, Eumomota superciliosa
Ethology (2008) 114 (1): 16.
Ink is a conspecific alarm cue in the Caribbean reef squid, Sepioteuthis sepioidea
Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology (2008) 367 (1): 11.
LACK OF ASSORTATIVE MATING FOR TAIL, BODY SIZE, OR CONDITION IN THE ELABORATE MONOMORPHIC TURQUOISE-BROWED MOTMOT (EUMOMOTA SUPERCILIOSA)
The Auk (2008) 125 (1): 11.
Anti-Predator Signals in the ChaffinchFringilla coelebsin Response to Habitat Structure and Different Predator Types
Ethology (2008) 114 (11): 1033.
Personality and Nest Defence in the Great Tit (Parus major)
Ethology (2008) 114 (4): 405.
Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus
Animal Behaviour (2008) 76 (5): 1465.
Racketed tail of the male and female turquoise-browed motmot: male but not female tail length correlates with pairing success, performance, and reproductive success
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (2007) 61 (6): 911.
Dishonest ‘preemptive’ pursuit-deterrent signal? Why the turquoise-browed motmot wags its tail before feeding nestlings
Animal Behaviour (2007) 73 (6): 965.
Lack of melanized keratin and barbs that fall off: how the racketed tail of the turquoise-browed motmot Eumomota superciliosa is formed
Journal of Avian Biology (2007) 0 (0): 070222075911001.
Observational and Experimental Evidence for the Function of Tail Flicking in Eurasian Moorhen Gallinula chloropus
Ethology (2007) 113 (7): 629.
Lack of melanized keratin and barbs that fall off: how the racketed tail of the turquoise-browed motmot Eumomota superciliosa is formed
Journal of Avian Biology (2007) 38 (2): 139.
This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

View Article Abstract & Purchase Options

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Subscribe Now