Abstract

We analyse the spatial concentration of stop and search (S&S) practices. Previous work argues that the persistent reliance on S&S, despite weak to null deterrent effects on crime, results from a social order maintenance motivation on the part of the police. Expanding previous studies that focused on who tends to be stopped and searched by police officers, we focus on where S&S concentrates and investigate the role of economic inequality. We use data from London in 2019 and demonstrate that a novel measure of salient, spatially granular economic inequality is positively associated with S&S incidence at a small spatial scale, even when controlling for crime rates and other important variables. Police officers more frequently stop and search members of the public in places where the well-off and the economically precarious co-exist. Implications for understanding S&S as a tool that distinguishes between citizens, between those to protect and potential criminals, are discussed.

Introduction

The ability of the police to use coercive powers to control crime and maintain social order is one their defining features (Loader and Walker, 2001). One of the best examples of the use of such coercive powers is the ability of law enforcement agents to stop members of the public, check their identities, and search their possessions, in what has been widely referred to as stop and search (S&S) practices. With the promise to repress criminal behaviour and ensure public safety, police organizations around the globe have increasingly made use of such powers (Weber and Bowling, 2014). In England and Wales, for instance, police forces have relied on S&S ­practices more or less continuously since the 1980s, despite it being controversial, with lively reactions from the public whenever policymakers increase the use of pedestrian searches.

The promise of tackling crime through S&S is premised on the idea that individuals will be deterred from engaging in offensive behaviour if they believe they could likely be stopped by law enforcement agents (Apel and Nagin, 2011). This is particularly true for crimes related to drugs or knife carrying (Weisburd et al., 2016). Previous research shows that some strategies based on focused deterrence and ‘hotspot’ policing have indeed led to modest improvements in some crime rates, especially in the United States (Braga et al., 2018). S&S practices, in particular, are moderately associated with reductions in drug crimes in the United Kingdom (Tiratelli et al., 2018). Yet, in general, the widespread use of S&S powers tends to be, at best, only slightly successful at decreasing specific crime rates at specific locations, and overall is considered a somewhat ineffective tool for crime control (Delsol, 2015; MacDonald et al., 2016; McCandless et al., 2016; Tiratelli et al., 2018)

It is important to understand the impact of S&S on crime. But it is also important to understand the unintended consequences of increased police presence in certain communities. As noted by Manski and Nagin (2017), confrontational proactive policing strategies, such as S&S, must balance the sometimes conflicting objectives of crime deterrence and community trust. For instance, Sharkey (2018) has recently argued how aggressive policing and a general expansion of criminal justice institutions contributed to the decline in violent crime in the United States over the last decades, but also demonstrated the ways in which the reliance on such confrontational practices has taken a heavy toll—with entire communities destabilized by mass incarceration and increased levels of police brutality. Similarly, Bradford (2017) showed how the persistent reliance on S&S practices to reduce crime in England and Wales can undermine public beliefs about police trustworthiness and legitimacy, generating at least as many dis-benefits as benefits in relation to police effectiveness (see also Bradford, 2015).

Given the undermining of trust in the police and the modest efficacy of S&S as a crime-control strategy, previous work has suggested that the persistence of governmental reliance on S&S practices is more related to social order maintenance, widely defined, than to crime-fighting, narrowly defined (Bradford and Loader, 2016; Tiratelli et al., 2018). This argument is sustained, for instance, by the well documented literature on ethnic disparities in the United Kingdom (Bowling and Phillips, 2007; Quinton, 2015), as a growing body of research documents that Black, Asian and Minority Ethnic (BAME) individuals are more likely to be stopped and searched by police than White people (Shiner et al., 2018; Miller et al., 2020; Vomfell and Stewart, 2021). By over-policing BAME groups, police behaviour would be restating power relations within society—i.e. police actions on who to stop and search would then inform how structural forces interact, and how the police contribute to maintain social order.

Complementing this previous research, in this study we focus on where police officers tend to stop and search members of the public. We hypothesize that, if S&S is indeed a tool of social control, economic inequality might be a factor in the concentration of police searches. Stopping and searching citizens can ascribe identity (Bradford and Loader, 2016; Oliveira et al., 2021) and reproduce cultural meanings of the police as an institution that provides protection to some groups of people (i.e. the rich) and suspicion to other, subordinate sections of society (i.e. the poor). Highly unequal neighbourhoods with streets occupied by both the rich and the poor could therefore attract police coercive practice, with officers conducting more searches than in more economically homogeneous locations.

Using a novel measure of salient, spatially granular economic inequality developed by Suss (2021), we investigate spatial association at the Lower Layer Super Output Areas (LSOA) level and show that S&S practices are concentrated in places where the economically deprived and the well-off co-exist. Even when previous crime rates and demographic characteristics are taken into account, economic inequality predicts heightened S&S activity. The role policing plays in maintaining social order—over-patrolling some groups of people in pursuit of other groups of people’s protection—is then evident by the spatial concentration of police searches in more unequal locations.

The rest of the paper proceeds as follows. First, we set the scene and explain the legal context of S&S powers in England and Wales, review the studies that estimate the deterrent effect of S&S on crime rates, discuss S&S as a tool of social control, and address the extent to which a focus on spatial patterns could contribute to the literature. Then we go on to present this study’s data, methods, and results, and conclude with a discussion on the spatial concentration of S&S practices in London.

Background

Stop and search in England and Wales

Police officers acting with suspicion against citizens might be one of the oldest practices involved in policing activities. In the United Kingdom, police powers to stop and search members of the public are currently regulated by various pieces of legislation, but the Police and Criminal Evidence Act 1984 (PACE) is probably the most significant (Bridges, 2015). PACE established legal powers of the police to stop and search a person (or vehicle) in a public space as long as officers have reasonable suspicion that the person is in possession of stolen goods, weapons or other prohibited articles. More recent developments in the legislation have granted further powers to the police, allowing for ‘suspicion-less’ stop and searches under specific circumstances; notably, section 60 of the Criminal Justice and Public Order Act 1994 (hereafter S60), which allows senior police officers to authorize stop and search practices against any person in a particular locality for 24 h (which can be extended), with the goal of fighting violence associated with specific events. Moreover, section 44 of the Terrorism Act 2000 (S44) allows senior police officers to authorize stop and search without reasonable suspicion if deemed necessary to prevent acts of terrorism, though this has since been repealed.

The use of police powers to stop and search citizens in England and Wales varies considerably across time and place (Bradford, 2017), even within London (Tiratelli et al., 2018). As Shiner and Delsol (2015) suggest, variations in the use of such powers are directly associated with the broader politics of crime control. With the promise of deterring future criminal behaviour, increases in the number of stop and searches—particularly the ‘suspicion-less’ variety—can be framed within a ‘toughening of crime control’ political ideology, which is more or less popular depending on the political climate. At the same time, previous research argues that S&S is, at least partly, a tool for social control, as evidenced in the United Kingdom by high levels of ethnic disproportionality in the use of S&S powers (see Bowling and Phillips, 2007; Shiner et al., 2018). This study tackles this double aspect of policing—crime deterrence and social control, or ‘parking tickets and class repression’ (Marenin, 1982)—by assessing where police stops and searches are concentrated in the UK capital.

S&S as a strategy for crime deterrence?

Police powers to stop and search members of the public are justified using a number of reasons. Notably, police reports often emphasize the importance of S&S to investigate crime and to ­apprehend weapons (MET, 2014). But one of the main motivations for the persistence of S&S is the promised deterrent effect on crime—the promise that increasing the number of police stops and searches will have a downstream effect on crimes, especially knife crime and drug offences (see May, 2014).

Deterrence theory premises that rational individuals will be deterred from engaging in offensive behaviour should the costs (e.g. punishment) outweigh potential gains from their behaviour (Becker, 1968). Two important mechanisms are often listed as effective crime deterrents: the certainty and the severity of punishment (Apel and Nagin, 2011). In the case of S&S, the former is the theoretical foundation for its effectiveness: by increasing the number of police stops, potential criminals would perceive a higher likelihood of being caught and therefore be deterred from carrying knives or drugs. An important aspect of S&S is to therefore appear risky for potential offenders. Though theoretically sound, a sensible empirical question is: does it actually work? Is S&S effective in deterring crime (Delsol, 2015)?

This was the primary question posed by Tiratelli, Quinton, and Bradford (2018). Using stop and search data for every borough in London from April 2004 to November 2014, the authors used lagged dependent variable models to estimate the effect of the number of stops and searches in one week or month on a number of crime rates in the following week or month, controlling for previous crime rates. They also relied on interrupted time series models to estimate the effect of the sudden increase in the use of ‘suspicion-less’ stops and searches (based on S60) between 2007 and 2011 on subsequent crime rates. They found very little evidence that S&S has a deterrent effect on crime—there were no effects on robbery and theft, vehicle crime or criminal damage, very small or no effects on burglary, non-domestic violent crime and total crime. Only drug offences appeared to be somewhat affected by variations in S&S, though even in this case the causal mechanism is not clear (Tiratelli et al., 2018, 1224–25). Overall, the authors conclude that S&S practices have relatively little deterrent effect.

Another London-based study was conducted by McCandless et al. (2016), who investigated the effect of Operation BLUNT 2 on crime rates. Operation BLUNT 2 was a large initiative that involved the increase of suspicion-less stop and searches based on S60 in some boroughs. The authors used difference-in-differences to assess the policy impact on knife crime comparing boroughs where the policy was implemented with boroughs where it was not. In summary, they found that the increase in S60-based stop and searches had no effect on police recorded crime. Other studies corroborate the lack of empirical evidence on the effect of similar policing strategies worldwide on crime rates, most notably in the context of New York City’s Stop, Question and Frisk (SQF) policy (Smith et al., 2012; Rosenfeld and Fornango, 2014; Fagan et al., 2016; MacDonald et al., 2016).

Taken together, there is no consensus on the effect of S&S on crime rates. Evidence points to heterogeneous impacts of this type of policy strategy depending on spatial differences and the type of crime (Weisburd et al., 2016), but overall studies find no or little decreases in most crime rates. Given the apparent ineffectiveness of stopping and searching members of the public, another question posed by the literature is around the persistence of S&S practices (Bradford, 2015). Specifically, recent studies have investigated whether an interpretation of S&S practices as a tool of social control provides the explanation.

S&S as a tool of social control?

Tiratelli, Quinton, and Bradford (2018) argue that, apart from officers’ beliefs that this is a useful tool of crime control, S&S practices are not solely about crime—‘it is also a tool of order maintenance, used by police officers seeking to assert power and control in a situation or locale’ (p. 1226). A large body of previous research on the cultural meaning of policing powers has suggested the interpretation of police as an institution of social ordering that provides identities to the policed depending on their position in structural relationships (see Bradford and Loader, 2016). In that sense, stop and search powers contribute to protect the boundaries of inclusion and exclusion (Waddington, 1999) as police contact contains status-relevant information about citizens (see Oliveira et al., 2021).

The interpretation of S&S powers as a tool of social control is often linked to ethnic disparities, which have been extensively documented by previous research (Shiner et al., 2018; Miller et al., 2020; Vomfell and Stewart, 2021). Black, Asian and minority ethnic (BAME) groups are more likely to be stopped and searched than their white counterparts—for instance, as of 2017 Black people were stopped and searched more than eight times the rate of White people (Shiner et al., 2018). Considering the over-representation of BAME groups in S&S, one argument is that police powers to stop and search members of the public would mainly be used by police officers seeking to assert power and control, restating power relations within society and informing order maintenance. Ethnic disparities in the police use of S&S powers is a social problem yet to be tackled, and one that has worsened as the overall number of S&S decreased over the last decade (Shiner et al., 2018).

Rather than focusing on who is stopped and searched, an alternative strategy to investigate claims that S&S should be interpreted more as a tool of social control and order maintenance, widely defined, than as a tool for crime deterrence, narrowly defined, is to investigate where stops and searches tend to cluster in a city. This was suggested by Vomfell and Stewart (2021), who conducted a large-scale study on stop and search in the West Midlands and found that, above and beyond the effects of individual police officers disproportionately targeting BAME populations, the over-patrolling of minority areas was fundamental to explain socially relevant outcomes such as ethnic disparities.

Aside from ethnic and racial discrimination, investigating spatial patterns of S&S practices could partly inform other ways in which police officers seek to assert power and order. In particular, we hypothesize that economic inequality plays an important role in the spatial distribution of S&S, with police officers conducting more searches in more unequal neighbourhoods where the well-off and the economically precarious co-exist.

This study

To address this, we analyse stop and search data from London in 2019, aggregated by lower super output area (LSOA). LSOAs are areas designed for census purposes and to improve local statistics in England and Wales. They are also population-weighted, containing on average 1,691 residents (SD = 263). To introduce the data, Figure 1 shows the spatial distribution of the stop and search count by LSOA—it is a highly heterogeneous distribution, with larger concentrations of stops in the Central, North East and West parts of the UK capital.1

Number of Stop and searches—London, 2019, LSOA level. The figure shows the spatial distribution of police stops and searches for London, 2019. The data were downloaded from https://data.police.uk/.
Fig. 1

Number of Stop and searches—London, 2019, LSOA level. The figure shows the spatial distribution of police stops and searches for London, 2019. The data were downloaded from https://data.police.uk/.

Our hypothesis—based on the interpretation of S&S as a tool of social control—is that economic inequality plays a role, with more unequal locations being more susceptible to S&S practices even when holding previous crime rates and other demographic characteristics constant. According to a model of social control put forward by Choongh (1998), aspects of police behaviour and activity can be seen as seeking to ‘subordinate sections of society’ which are deemed inherently criminal (p. 623). The poor are one such section, and it is therefore in areas where the poor and rich live side-by-side where police are more likely to be deployed or primed to wield S&S as a tool of social control—to protect one segment of society from other subordinate sections of society.

How might this operate? This heightened attention on the part of the police towards economically unequal neighbourhoods might be conscious or unconscious, stemming from top down or bottom up decision-making—i.e. resulting from a perceived, implicit or institutionalized need to maintain social order. We are agnostic over how exactly these policing decisions to over-patrol unequal neighbourhoods relative to economically homogenous neighbourhoods are made, but rather focus on whether our theoretical predictions are empirically verified, thereby leaving an analysis of the exact mechanisms through which local inequality leads to heightened police attention and greater incidence of S&S to future research.

To test our hypothesis, we use a novel measure of salient, spatially granular economic inequality at the LSOA level based on the estimated value of around 23 million UK residences (Suss, 2021). One of the advantages of our measure is that it is more perceptible to individuals, given that it is based on a major feature of the built environment used by individuals to make distributional assessments (i.e. quality of houses), as opposed to unobservable features (e.g. income of residents) (see Suss, 2021). Suss (2021) finds housing value inequality to be substantively associated with individual perceptions of local economic inequality in the United Kingdom across two surveys. This perceptual point is important because we believe that observing the juxtaposition of rich and poor is more likely to trigger the social control motive for S&S. Figure 2 shows the spatial distribution of housing value inequality as measured by the Gini coefficient—a commonly used measure of economic inequality—for London in 2019 at the LSOA level.

Housing value inequality—London, 2019, LSOA level. Note: The figure shows the spatial distribution of within-LSOA housing value inequality for London, 2019. Data on housing values come from Zoopla and is discussed in Suss (2021). Areas with less than 50 observations are omitted (grey).
Fig. 2

Housing value inequality—London, 2019, LSOA level. Note: The figure shows the spatial distribution of within-LSOA housing value inequality for London, 2019. Data on housing values come from Zoopla and is discussed in Suss (2021). Areas with less than 50 observations are omitted (grey).

Data and methods

Our response variable consists of the number of stop and searches in 2019 by LSOA (N = 4,835). The distribution of S&S is highly skewed. 18.4% of locations had no more than five stops and searches in 2018, whereas a single LSOA had 2,108 of such police stops. The median number of stops is 20, while the average is 47.6 and the mode is just 5—which speaks to the highly unequal distribution of S&S powers across Greater London. Figure 1 maps the spatial distribution of stops in London for 2019.

Our main explanatory variable is a measure of economic inequality based on housing values. The data come from the online property aggregator Zoopla and was gathered in September 2019. The size of the data (approximately 23 million observations for the United Kingdom) allows us to measure economic discrepancies at a far more granular level than possible with conventional data sources (Suss, 2021). We estimate inequality down at the LSOA level to match the S&S data (average of 516 housing value observations per LSOA). The Gini coefficient ranges from 0.042 to 0.649, with a mean value of 0.198—see Figure 2 for a map of inequality in London.

Control variables

We control for variables that potentially confound the relationship between economic inequality and the S&S count at the LSOA level. As discussed above, one of the stated promises of confrontational proactive policing methods, such as the use of S&S powers, is their deterrent effect on crime. Although previous studies suggest a relatively weak effect of S&S on future crime, police forces could potentially allocate their resources—including the decision regarding where stop and search members of the public—in pursuit of crime deterrence, and therefore make decisions based on previous crime rates. This is particularly true for previous drug-related crime offences (see Tiratelli et al., 2018). If a given location had more drug-related offences registered in the previous year, it is reasonable to assume that police forces would prioritize such location over other, ‘less risky’ ones. At the same time, local inequality has also been found to be correlated with violence figures (see Kang, 2016). As such, crime rates—particularly previous drug offences—could be potentially confounding the association between local inequality and the number of S&S.

In order to control for previous reported crime, we include the following variables in the analysis:

  • Drug Offences in 2018

We include the rate of drug offences per LSOA for 2018 as a control variable (standardized by LSOA workplace population taken from the 2011 census). This is taken from data made publicly available by the UK police (https://data.police.uk/). Temporally lagged drug offences are our most important control variable, given that drugs is one of the primary reasons cited by police for performing S&S and the only type of offence moderately affected by variations in the use of S&S according to previous research (Tiratelli et al., 2018).

  • Multiple Deprivation Indices, Crime domain scores for 2019

In order to control for the level of crime at the LSOA level more generally, we include the crime domain scores from the 2019 Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD; Government (2019)). The score comprises four indicators, based on data from between 2016 and 2018, related to rates of violence, burglary, theft, and criminal damage. This index is another crucial control variable. Theoretically, police forces could decide on resource allocation—including S&S allocation—based on how violent neighbourhoods were in the past, which is also potentially associated with levels of salient inequality. Crime indicators confound the relationship between structural neighbourhood conditions and number of stop and searches.

Additionally, several other variables also need to be taken into account, most notably those related to neighbourhood structural conditions. Previous research demonstrates how some social disorganization indicators (e.g. concentrated poverty, ethnic diversity, population density, among others; see Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls 1997), which are intrinsically related to economic inequality, also predict variation in police behaviour (Kane, 2002; Sutherland et al., 2013). The additional control variables we include in the analysis are the following:

  • Ethnic composition in 2011

We include a measure of ethnic composition at the LSOA level using data from the 2011 census. We construct this as the percent of residents who are non-White.2

  • Multiple Deprivation Income scores for 2019

We include the income domain score from the IMD. This comprises indicators for the number of families per area receiving income support, jobseeker’s allowance, employment and support allowance, and pension credit. For the detailed description of the domains and methodology, see Government (2019).

  • Average property value (log scale)

We include the average property value for the area (logged). This, along with the income score, allows us to control for the level of affluence per LSOA.

  • Distance to nearest transport hub

We introduce a measure of the distance (in meters) from each LSOA centroid and the nearest Transport for London (TfL) station (i.e. including underground, overground and rail services).

  • Density

Finally, we control for population density (defined as the usual resident plus workplace population divided by hectares) from the 2011 UK census. Table 1 provides descriptive statistics for all variables.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for control variables

StatisticNMeanSDMinPctl(25)Pctl(75)Max
Stops4,83547.599102.28408482,108
Gini coefficient4,8350.1980.0860.0420.1360.2460.649
Average property value (GBP thousands)4,835604.579409.5491833786775,064
Density (workday population/hectares)4,835136.985117.7922.20068.450174.4001,432.300
Income deprivation score4,8350.1360.0760.0060.0750.1890.437
Crime deprivation score4,8350.2580.571−2.354−0.1170.6372.377
Drugs rate (offenses/workplace population)4,8310.2580.3030.0000.0620.3383.180
Distance to nearest TfL station (meters)4,835404.474445.071072.0585.44,927
Non-white percentage4,83539.38720.3671.84122.61154.20496.592
StatisticNMeanSDMinPctl(25)Pctl(75)Max
Stops4,83547.599102.28408482,108
Gini coefficient4,8350.1980.0860.0420.1360.2460.649
Average property value (GBP thousands)4,835604.579409.5491833786775,064
Density (workday population/hectares)4,835136.985117.7922.20068.450174.4001,432.300
Income deprivation score4,8350.1360.0760.0060.0750.1890.437
Crime deprivation score4,8350.2580.571−2.354−0.1170.6372.377
Drugs rate (offenses/workplace population)4,8310.2580.3030.0000.0620.3383.180
Distance to nearest TfL station (meters)4,835404.474445.071072.0585.44,927
Non-white percentage4,83539.38720.3671.84122.61154.20496.592

The table provides descriptive statistics for all variables.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for control variables

StatisticNMeanSDMinPctl(25)Pctl(75)Max
Stops4,83547.599102.28408482,108
Gini coefficient4,8350.1980.0860.0420.1360.2460.649
Average property value (GBP thousands)4,835604.579409.5491833786775,064
Density (workday population/hectares)4,835136.985117.7922.20068.450174.4001,432.300
Income deprivation score4,8350.1360.0760.0060.0750.1890.437
Crime deprivation score4,8350.2580.571−2.354−0.1170.6372.377
Drugs rate (offenses/workplace population)4,8310.2580.3030.0000.0620.3383.180
Distance to nearest TfL station (meters)4,835404.474445.071072.0585.44,927
Non-white percentage4,83539.38720.3671.84122.61154.20496.592
StatisticNMeanSDMinPctl(25)Pctl(75)Max
Stops4,83547.599102.28408482,108
Gini coefficient4,8350.1980.0860.0420.1360.2460.649
Average property value (GBP thousands)4,835604.579409.5491833786775,064
Density (workday population/hectares)4,835136.985117.7922.20068.450174.4001,432.300
Income deprivation score4,8350.1360.0760.0060.0750.1890.437
Crime deprivation score4,8350.2580.571−2.354−0.1170.6372.377
Drugs rate (offenses/workplace population)4,8310.2580.3030.0000.0620.3383.180
Distance to nearest TfL station (meters)4,835404.474445.071072.0585.44,927
Non-white percentage4,83539.38720.3671.84122.61154.20496.592

The table provides descriptive statistics for all variables.

Analytic strategy

Our hypothesis is that local economic inequality is a driver of S&S. In order to properly assess this hypothesis, we need to take into account potential spatial dependency as well as other confounders (e.g. crime rates). It could be that LSOAs that are close to each other are similar in a number of observed and unobserved features, with police behaviour and structural conditions being just two of those features. The tests we perform in this article aim to rule out alternative explanations for the spatial association between the number of S&S and salient inequality at LSOA level.

Our analytic approach is two pronged. Our goal is to investigate whether there is a spatial association between the number of S&S and the level of economic inequality at the LSOA level. First, we assess the extent to which the relationship between the two variables remain significant when controlling for spatial effects alongside all control variables described above. To do so, we compare an OLS model that treats stops as a continuous variable with a spatial Durbin model (SDM).3 The SDM includes spatial lags—taken as the average of all adjoining LSOAs (i.e. the Queen configuration)—for the outcome and regressor variables. This accounts for possible dependence between, for example, stops and searches in any given area with the level of inequality and drugs crime rate in neighbouring LSOAs. More formally:

where yj is the number of stops for area j, W is the row-standardized spatial weight matrix (non-zero elements indicate a neighbouring LSOA), Xj is a vector of covariates with associated β parameters to be estimated, ρ and λ are the parameters for the dependent and independent spatial lags, ζj is a vector for the Borough of each area j (i.e. Borough fixed-effects), and ϵj is the error term.

Second, given that the number of S&S by LSOA is a count variable we employ negative binomial regression models to evaluate the relationship between police stops and inequality. The variance of stops exceeds the average (i.e. overdispersion), so the negative binomial model specification is better suited versus the Poisson (log-linear) regression model, which assumes that the average is equal to the variance. However, we are unable to account for spatial effects using the negative binomial model given the limitations that result from including the spatial lag in the exponential function (Glaser, 2017), hence why we first control for spatial effects when treating the number of stops as a continuous variable. The negative binomial model takes the form:

where μj is the expected value of our outcome measure for area iwith variance μ+μ2/θ, β0 is the intercept, Xj a vector of independent variables, and β a vector of coefficients to be estimated (along with θ) using maximum likelihood.

Results

First, we compare the OLS coefficient estimates with that of the SDM. The Moran’s I test statistic on the OLS residuals reveals spatial correlation (p < 0.01), but this is no longer the case when introducing the spatial lags—a Monte-Carlo simulation of Moran’s I reveals the SDM model residuals are not spatially correlated (p = 0.587; based on 1,000 simulations).

Table 2 displays the output from the OLS and Durbin models. Both models control for the average property value (log scale), income and crime deprivation scores, population density (log scale), the percent of non-White residents, distance to the nearest TfL station and the 2018 rate of drug crimes. The coefficients for the spatial lags of the drug crime rate and TfL station distance are found to be significant (p < 0.01; not shown), as is ρ, the coefficient on the lag of the dependent variable (p < 0.01). Taking into account the spatial autocorrelation of neighbouring LSOAs, we can see that the greater the level of economic inequality of a location, the higher the number of stops and searches—this is the case in both the OLS and the SDM models, with the size of the coefficient becoming slightly smaller in magnitude once spatial effects are taken into account. Specifically, considering Model 2 of Table 2, a standard deviation increase in the Gini coefficient (SD = 0.086) is associated with more than six additional police stops.

Table 2.

OLS and SDM regression results

Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
OLSSDM
(1)(2)
Gini7.285***5.763***
(1.983)(1.944)
Average property value (log)−1.495−5.111
(3.063)(4.741)
Density (log)15.499***7.666***
(1.801)(2.214)
Income deprivation−10.228***−10.001***
(2.275)(2.184)
Crime deprivation13.159***9.107***
(1.710)(1.888)
Drugs rate41.258***39.516***
(1.535)(1.560)
Non-white (%)3.765*10.044**
(2.240)(4.657)
TfL station distance−5.441***−14.793***
(1.407)(2.933)
Borough fixed effectsYY
Rho0.4
Observations4,8314,831
R20.267
Adjusted-R20.261
Log likelihood−28,206.270
Sigma26,692.817
Akaike Inf. Crit.56,578.530
Residual SE87.950 (df = 4,790)
F Statistic43.671*** (df = 40; 4790)
Wald test495.973*** (df = 1)
LR test435.011*** (df = 1)
Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
OLSSDM
(1)(2)
Gini7.285***5.763***
(1.983)(1.944)
Average property value (log)−1.495−5.111
(3.063)(4.741)
Density (log)15.499***7.666***
(1.801)(2.214)
Income deprivation−10.228***−10.001***
(2.275)(2.184)
Crime deprivation13.159***9.107***
(1.710)(1.888)
Drugs rate41.258***39.516***
(1.535)(1.560)
Non-white (%)3.765*10.044**
(2.240)(4.657)
TfL station distance−5.441***−14.793***
(1.407)(2.933)
Borough fixed effectsYY
Rho0.4
Observations4,8314,831
R20.267
Adjusted-R20.261
Log likelihood−28,206.270
Sigma26,692.817
Akaike Inf. Crit.56,578.530
Residual SE87.950 (df = 4,790)
F Statistic43.671*** (df = 40; 4790)
Wald test495.973*** (df = 1)
LR test435.011*** (df = 1)

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one).

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Table 2.

OLS and SDM regression results

Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
OLSSDM
(1)(2)
Gini7.285***5.763***
(1.983)(1.944)
Average property value (log)−1.495−5.111
(3.063)(4.741)
Density (log)15.499***7.666***
(1.801)(2.214)
Income deprivation−10.228***−10.001***
(2.275)(2.184)
Crime deprivation13.159***9.107***
(1.710)(1.888)
Drugs rate41.258***39.516***
(1.535)(1.560)
Non-white (%)3.765*10.044**
(2.240)(4.657)
TfL station distance−5.441***−14.793***
(1.407)(2.933)
Borough fixed effectsYY
Rho0.4
Observations4,8314,831
R20.267
Adjusted-R20.261
Log likelihood−28,206.270
Sigma26,692.817
Akaike Inf. Crit.56,578.530
Residual SE87.950 (df = 4,790)
F Statistic43.671*** (df = 40; 4790)
Wald test495.973*** (df = 1)
LR test435.011*** (df = 1)
Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
OLSSDM
(1)(2)
Gini7.285***5.763***
(1.983)(1.944)
Average property value (log)−1.495−5.111
(3.063)(4.741)
Density (log)15.499***7.666***
(1.801)(2.214)
Income deprivation−10.228***−10.001***
(2.275)(2.184)
Crime deprivation13.159***9.107***
(1.710)(1.888)
Drugs rate41.258***39.516***
(1.535)(1.560)
Non-white (%)3.765*10.044**
(2.240)(4.657)
TfL station distance−5.441***−14.793***
(1.407)(2.933)
Borough fixed effectsYY
Rho0.4
Observations4,8314,831
R20.267
Adjusted-R20.261
Log likelihood−28,206.270
Sigma26,692.817
Akaike Inf. Crit.56,578.530
Residual SE87.950 (df = 4,790)
F Statistic43.671*** (df = 40; 4790)
Wald test495.973*** (df = 1)
LR test435.011*** (df = 1)

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one).

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Next, we present the results for the negative binomial regression models in Table 3. Model 1 just contains the inequality variable, and Model 2 adds all the control variables. The results indicate that the coefficient on our measure of inequality is significant and slightly larger when introducing the controls. We can get a sense of the substantiveness of this effect by taking the exponent of the coefficient, known as the incidence rate ratio (IRR). For Model 2, the IRR is 1.33, which indicates that the expected number of stops is expected to increase by 33% for a one standard deviation increase in inequality, controlling for all other variables in the model. This is a large figure. To put it into perspective, the IRR for the rate of drug offences in 2017 is 1.52 and the crime deprivation score 1.25, so the expected percent increase in S&S stemming from inequality is over half of the effect of temporally lagged drug offences and 8 percentage points larger than the effect of the crime score.

Table 3.

Negative binomial regression results

Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
(1)(2)(3)
Gini0.170***0.282***0.259***
(0.018)(0.022)(0.023)
Average property value (log)−0.187***−0.229***
(0.035)(0.035)
Density (log)0.159***0.158***
(0.020)(0.020)
Income deprivation−0.033−0.063**
(0.025)(0.026)
Crime deprivation0.224***0.218***
(0.019)(0.019)
Drugs rate0.417***0.420***
(0.017)(0.017)
Non-white (%)0.213***0.210***
(0.025)(0.025)
TfL station distance−0.132***−0.136***
(0.016)(0.016)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.097***
(0.017)
Borough fixed effectsYYY
Observations4,8314,8314,831
Log likelihood−23,155.140−21,673.430−21,657.930
theta0.642*** (0.012)1.076*** (0.021)1.082*** (0.021)
Akaike information criterion46,314.29043,428.86043,399.870
Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
(1)(2)(3)
Gini0.170***0.282***0.259***
(0.018)(0.022)(0.023)
Average property value (log)−0.187***−0.229***
(0.035)(0.035)
Density (log)0.159***0.158***
(0.020)(0.020)
Income deprivation−0.033−0.063**
(0.025)(0.026)
Crime deprivation0.224***0.218***
(0.019)(0.019)
Drugs rate0.417***0.420***
(0.017)(0.017)
Non-white (%)0.213***0.210***
(0.025)(0.025)
TfL station distance−0.132***−0.136***
(0.016)(0.016)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.097***
(0.017)
Borough fixed effectsYYY
Observations4,8314,8314,831
Log likelihood−23,155.140−21,673.430−21,657.930
theta0.642*** (0.012)1.076*** (0.021)1.082*** (0.021)
Akaike information criterion46,314.29043,428.86043,399.870

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one).

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Table 3.

Negative binomial regression results

Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
(1)(2)(3)
Gini0.170***0.282***0.259***
(0.018)(0.022)(0.023)
Average property value (log)−0.187***−0.229***
(0.035)(0.035)
Density (log)0.159***0.158***
(0.020)(0.020)
Income deprivation−0.033−0.063**
(0.025)(0.026)
Crime deprivation0.224***0.218***
(0.019)(0.019)
Drugs rate0.417***0.420***
(0.017)(0.017)
Non-white (%)0.213***0.210***
(0.025)(0.025)
TfL station distance−0.132***−0.136***
(0.016)(0.016)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.097***
(0.017)
Borough fixed effectsYYY
Observations4,8314,8314,831
Log likelihood−23,155.140−21,673.430−21,657.930
theta0.642*** (0.012)1.076*** (0.021)1.082*** (0.021)
Akaike information criterion46,314.29043,428.86043,399.870
Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
(1)(2)(3)
Gini0.170***0.282***0.259***
(0.018)(0.022)(0.023)
Average property value (log)−0.187***−0.229***
(0.035)(0.035)
Density (log)0.159***0.158***
(0.020)(0.020)
Income deprivation−0.033−0.063**
(0.025)(0.026)
Crime deprivation0.224***0.218***
(0.019)(0.019)
Drugs rate0.417***0.420***
(0.017)(0.017)
Non-white (%)0.213***0.210***
(0.025)(0.025)
TfL station distance−0.132***−0.136***
(0.016)(0.016)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.097***
(0.017)
Borough fixed effectsYYY
Observations4,8314,8314,831
Log likelihood−23,155.140−21,673.430−21,657.930
theta0.642*** (0.012)1.076*** (0.021)1.082*** (0.021)
Akaike information criterion46,314.29043,428.86043,399.870

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one).

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

For a more complete picture of the substantiveness of the results, we calculate the marginal effects for Model 2 in Figure 3. We do so in two ways: first, the marginal effect when each covariate is at its average value, and second, the average partial effect over every observation. From the first approach, we find that a one standard deviation increase in the level of inequality is expected to increase the number of stops by between (roughly) 7 and 11. The equivalent range for the average partial effect is approximately 12 and 18.

Marginal and average partial effect of inequality and other covariates on S&S. The figure shows the marginal effect and average partial effect for each covariate for Model 2 of Table 3 with 95% confidence intervals.
Fig. 3

Marginal and average partial effect of inequality and other covariates on S&S. The figure shows the marginal effect and average partial effect for each covariate for Model 2 of Table 3 with 95% confidence intervals.

Model 3 in Table 3 introduces an interaction term between inequality and the income deprivation score, which we find is negative and significant. This indicates that the effect of inequality on S&S depends on the local level of affluence, with the magnitude of the effect larger as affluence increases. This is a striking finding. It means that while there appears to be an effect of economic inequality on the number of police stops and searches, this effect is not homogeneous across LSOAs. When a given location is characterized by high levels of income deprivation (i.e. is relatively poorer), the relationship between economic inequality and police behaviour is weaker; when a location is relatively affluent, the expected effect of economic inequality on the number of S&S is much higher. Figure 4 plots the predicted number of stops for Model 2 as compared to Model 3 with the interaction term included and helps visualize the interaction effect. The figure demonstrates that relatively affluent areas (defined as being two standard deviations below the mean income deprivation score), are expected to see approximately five times fewer police stops when inequality is low versus high (defined as two standard deviations below versus above the mean Gini value).

Effect of interaction term on predicted S&S. The figure shows the how the predicted stops is affected by introducing a interaction term between inequality and income deprivation (Model 3 of Table 3). We fix all other covariates at their mean values. Shaded areas are 95% confidence intervals.
Fig. 4

Effect of interaction term on predicted S&S. The figure shows the how the predicted stops is affected by introducing a interaction term between inequality and income deprivation (Model 3 of Table 3). We fix all other covariates at their mean values. Shaded areas are 95% confidence intervals.

Robustness checks

We perform a number of robustness checks. First, We check whether our results are London specific or whether the relationship between economic inequality and S&S generalizes to other urban contexts. To evaluate this, we re-run the negative binomial specifications for other major cities in England: Birmingham, Liverpool and Manchester. Results can be found in  Table A.1 of Annex, but it suffices to say that they remain virtually unaltered—local economic inequality is associated with a higher number of police stops and searches in these cities as well.

Second, we examine whether our results are sensitive to how we model the outcome measure. Rather than raw counts of stops per LSOA, we take the rate of stops in each LSOA (per 1000 residents).  Table A.2 presents the results when we use this outcome measure (log scale) for the OLS and SDM regression. Again, we find consistent results, with both the main effect of inequality on the stops rate, as well as the interaction between inequality and income deprivation, statistically significant.

Finally, we also check whether the results hold when removing Westminster Borough, given that the area has neighbourhoods with extremely high levels of inequality as well as stops. The results are qualitatively the same for this sub-sample analysis—see  Table A.3 in Annex.

Discussion and conclusion

Criminologists have long studied the purpose and potential effects of police powers to stop and search members of the public. Based on either reasonable suspicion or completely ‘suspicion-less’ (such as those based on S60), increases in the use of S&S powers in the United Kingdom are often followed by intense public discussions about the effectiveness of such policies. Policymakers usually defend S&S practices praising its effective deterrent effects, especially on drug offences and knife crimes. However, previous work both in the United Kingdom and elsewhere has demonstrated that stopping and searching citizens has at best a marginal effect on preventing future offending behaviour (MacDonald et al., 2016; McCandless et al., 2016). In London specifically, only drug offences appear to be affected by an increase in S&S practices, and only to a small extent (Tiratelli et al., 2018). What is behind the persistence of this policing strategy?

Previous research over decades has suggested that S&S is not solely about crime-fighting, rather it is also about social order maintenance—i.e. it is a tool used by officers to assert power and control in a situation or locale (Choongh, 1998; Tiratelli et al., 2018). This argument finds empirical evidence in studies about who tends to be stopped and searched by police officers: in England and Wales, ethnic disparities in S&S suggest that police officers tend to discriminate BAME groups in comparison with White citizens (Shiner et al., 2018; Vomfell and Stewart, 2021).

In this article, we focus on where S&S practices tend to concentrate in the city of London in 2019. We hypothesize that if S&S is used by police officers as a tool to assert power over members of the public and emphasize a social order in which the police serve to protect citizens from potential criminals, highly unequal locations occupied by several economic groups of people could draw the attention of officers conducting police searches.

The empirical analysis in this article finds that economic inequality predicts the S&S incidence at a small spatial scale. We used a novel measure of salient, spatially granular economic inequality developed by Suss (2021). This data provide estimated housing values for around 23 million addresses in the United Kingdom in 2019, allowing us to investigate the effects of local inequality at a level not before possible. Furthermore, using housing values rather than income figures is theoretically sound as it has been shown to substantively affect people’s perceptions of economic discrepancies (Suss, 2021). Drawing upon publicly available S&S data from London in 2019 at the LSOA level and keeping previous crime rates, drug offences and ethnic composition constant, among other demographic variables, we demonstrate that police officers tend to engage in more searches in more economically unequal locations.

Furthermore, we find a significant and negative interaction between economic inequality and income deprivation. This means that, while there is an association between inequality and the number of searches, the effect depends on the level of affluence. The more affluent an LSOA is—i.e. low levels of income deprivation—the stronger the effect of inequality on S&S. This is a striking result. The fact that income deprivation alone does not influence the spatial concentration of searches but economic inequality does suggests that affluence alone is not a factor taken into account when officers decide to employ their S&S powers. Instead, the factor is the distribution of wealth. While a homogeneously wealthy or poor neighbourhood will have relatively few searches, a neighbourhood where the gap between wealthy and poor residents is large will have plenty. Among those heterogeneous locations, the more affluent the neighbourhood is, the more searches will occur.

These results suggest that S&S powers are indeed employed as a tool of social control, protecting and asserting power over some segments of society. The police contribute to maintain social order by symbolically reproducing two groups of people: law-abiding citizens, the ones police officers protect; and potential criminals, the ones police officers police (Choongh, 1998; Bradford and Loader, 2016). Highly unequal neighbourhoods where the rich and the poor co-exist are areas in which the two groups collide, with citizens to protect and individuals to police both occupying the streets, thus attracting a heavier police presence. By disproportionately stopping and searching members of the public in economically unequal locations, police officers contribute to ascribe identity and maintain social order.

Limitations should, of course, be acknowledged. First, we do not claim causality. There could be omitted variable bias confounding the relationship between economic inequality and the number of searches by LSOA, given that we—obviously—did not randomly assign levels of inequality, nor did we engage in any particular identification strategy. Future research should employ quasi-experimental designs to potentially assess the causal effect of the association we found. Second, we only analysed data from London and other English cities. Other contexts, such as the Stop, Question, and Frisk policies in the United States could have different spatial properties relative to those in the United Kingdom. Third, we did not distinguish between searches based on reasonable suspicion and ‘suspicion-less’ searches based on S60—it is possible that they have different spatial distributions. We would welcome studies focusing on more nuanced analysis of S&S in the United Kingdom and elsewhere. Finally, we only analysed data from 2019—future research could model time series assessing the extent to which structural neighbourhood conditions continuously influence the use of S&S powers.

Despite these limitations, the results presented in this article provide new and valuable evidence on the relationship between economic inequality and policing behaviour and on S&S policies as a form of social control.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are grateful to Ben Ansell, Paul Dolan, John Hills, Jonathan Jackson, David Kirk, Kate Laffan, Neil Lee, Charles Lanfear, Krisztián Pósch and two anonymous referees for helpful advice and comments.

REFERENCES

Apel
,
R.
and
Nagin
,
D. S.
(
2011
), ‘
General Deterrence: A Review of Recent Evidence’
,
Crime and Public Policy
,
4
:
411
36
.

Becker
,
G. S.
(
1968
), ‘
Crime and Punishment: An Economic Approach’
,
Journal of Political Economy
,
76
:
169
217
.

Bowling
,
B.
and
Phillips
,
C.
(
2007
),
‘Disproportionate and Discriminatory: Reviewing the Evidence on Police Stop and Search’
,
The Modern Law Review
,
70
:
936
61
.

Bradford
,
B.
(
2015
),
‘Unintended Consequences’,
in
Stop and Search: The Anatomy of a Police Power
,
102
22
.
Springer
.

———. (

2017
),
Stop and Search and Police Legitimacy
.
Routledge
.

Bradford
,
B.
and
Loader
,
I.
(
2016
),
‘Police, Crime and Order: The Case of Stop and Search’,
in
Bradford
,
B.
,
Jaugueri
,
B.
,
Loader
,
I.
and
Steinberg
,
J.
, eds.
The Sage Handbook of Global Policing
,
241
60
.
SAGE Publications
.

Braga
,
A. A.
,
Weisburd
,
D.
and
Turchan
,
B.
(
2018
),
‘Focused Deterrence Strategies and Crime Control: An Updated Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Empirical Evidence’,
Criminology & Public Policy
,
17
:
205
50
.

Bridges
,
L.
(
2015
),
‘The Legal Powers and Their Limits’,
in
Delsol
,
R.
and
Shiner
,
M.
, eds.
Stop and Search: The Anatomy of a Police Power
,
9
30
.
Springer
.

Choongh
,
S.
(
1998
),
‘Policing the Dross: A Social Disciplinary Model of Policing’,
The British Journal of Criminology
,
38
:
623
34
.

Delsol
,
R.
(
2015
),
‘Effectiveness’,
in
Stop and Search: The Anatomy of a Police Power
,
79
101
.
Springer
.

Fagan
,
J.
,
Braga
,
A.
,
Brunson
,
R.
and
Pattavina
,
A.
(
2016
),
‘Stops and Stares: Street Stops, Surveillance, and Race in the New Policing’,
Fordham Urban Law Journal
,
43
:
539
614
.

Glaser
,
S.
(
2017
),
‘A Review of Spatial Econometric Models for Count Data’
, Hohenheim Discussion Papers in Business, Economics and Social Sciences, no. 19-2017, available online at https://ideas.repec.org/p/zbw/hohdps/192017.html.

Kane
,
R. J.
(
2002
),
‘The Social Ecology of Police Misconduct’,
Criminology
,
40
:
867
96
.

Kang
,
S.
(
2016
),
‘Inequality and Crime Revisited: Effects of Local Inequality and Economic Segregation on Crime’,
Journal of Population Economics
,
29
:
593
626
.

Loader
,
I.
and
Walker
,
N.
(
2001
),
‘Policing as a Public Good: Reconstituting the Connections Between Policing and the State’
,
Theoretical Criminology
,
5
:
9
35
.

MacDonald
,
J.
,
Fagan
,
J.
and
Geller
,
A.
(
2016
),
‘The Effects of Local Police Surges on Crime and Arrests in New York City’
,
PLoS One
,
11
:
1
13
.

Manski
,
C. F.
and
Nagin
,
D. S.
(
2017
),
‘Assessing Benefits, Costs, and Disparate Racial Impacts of Confrontational Proactive Policing’
,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
114
:
9308
13
.

Marenin
,
O.
(
1982
),
‘Parking Tickets and Class Repression: The Concept of Policing in Critical Theories of Criminal Justice’
,
Contemporary Crises
,
6
:
241
66
.

May
,
T.
(
2014
),
‘Statement to Parliament on Stop and Search’
,
Hansard
,
Column
:
831
47
.

McCandless
,
R
,
Feist
,
A.
,
Allan
,
J.
and
Morgan
,
N.
(
2016
),
Do Initiatives Involving Substantial Increases in Stop and Search Reduce Crime? Assessing the Impact of Operation Blunt 2.
London
:
Home Office
.

Metropolitan Police Service (MET)
(
2014
),
‘Stop and Search’,
Metropolitan Police Website
.

Miller
,
J.
,
Quinton
,
P.
,
Alexandrou
,
B.
and
Packham
,
D.
(
2020
),
‘Can Police Training Reduce Ethnic/Racial Disparities in Stop and Search? Evidence from a Multisite UK Trial’,
Criminology & Public Policy
,
19
:
1259
87
.

Oliveira
,
T. R.
,
Jackson
J.
,
Bradford
,
B.
and
Murphy
,
K.
(
2021
),
‘Are Trustworthiness and Legitimacy ’Hard to Win and Easy to Lose’? A Longitudinal Test of the Asymmetry Thesis of Police-Citizen Contact’,
Journal of Quantitative Criminology
,
37
:
1003
45
.

Quinton
,
P.
(
2015
),
‘Race Disproportionality and Officer Decision-Making’,
in
Stop and Search: The Anatomy of a Police Power
,
57
78
.
Springer
.

Rosenfeld
,
R.
and
Fornango
,
R.
(
2014
),
‘The Impact of Police Stops on Precinct Robbery and Burglary Rates in New York City, 2003–2010’,
Justice Quarterly
,
31
:
96
122
.

Sampson
,
R. J.
,
Raudenbush
,
S. W.
and
Earls
,
F.
(
1997
),
‘Neighborhoods and Violent Crime: A Multilevel Study of Collective Efficacy’,
Science
,
277
:
918
24
.

Sharkey
,
P.
(
2018
),
Uneasy Peace: The Great Crime Decline, the Renewal of City Life, and the Next War on Violence
.
WW Norton & Company
.

Shiner
,
M.
and
Delsol
,
R.
(
2015
),
‘The Politics of the Powers’,
in
Stop and Search: The Anatomy of a Police Power
,
31
56
.
Springer
.

Shiner
,
M.
,
Carre
,
Z.
,
Delsol
,
R.
and
Eastwood
,
N.
(
2018
),
‘The Colour of Injustice: “race”, Drugs and Law Enforcement in England and Wales
’,
available online at https://www.release.org.uk/sites/default/files/pdf/publications/The%20Colour%20of%20Injustice.pdf

Smith
,
D.
,
Purtell
,
R.
and
Guerrero
,
S.
(
2012
),
‘Is Stop, Question and Frisk an Effective Tool in the Fight Against Crime’,
in Annual Research Conference of the Association of Public Policy and Management, Baltimore, MD.

Suss
,
J. H.
(
2021
),
‘Measuring Local, Salient Economic Inequality in the UK’
, available online at https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3958731.

Sutherland
,
A.
,
Brunton-Smith
,
I.
and
Jackson
,
J.
(
2013
),
‘Collective Efficacy, Deprivation and Violence in London’,
British Journal of Criminology
,
53
:
1050
74
.

Tiratelli
,
M.
,
Quinton
,
P.
and
Bradford
,
B.
(
2018
),
‘Does Stop and Search Deter Crime? Evidence from Ten Years of London-Wide Data’,
The British Journal of Criminology
,
58
:
1212
31
.

Vomfell
,
L.
and
Stewart
,
N.
(
2021
),
‘Officer Bias, over-Patrolling and Ethnic Disparities in Stop and Search’,
Nature Human Behaviour
,
5
:
566
75
.

Waddington
,
P. A.
(
1999
),
‘Police (Canteen) Sub-Culture. An Appreciation’,
The British Journal of Criminology
,
39
:
287
309
.

Weber
,
L.
and
Bowling
,
B.
(
2014
),
Stop and Search: Police Power in Global Context
.
Routledge
.

Weisburd
,
D.
,
Wooditch
,
A.
,
Weisburd
,
S.
and
Yang
,
S.-M.
(
2016
),
‘Do Stop, Question, and Frisk Practices Deter Crime? Evidence at Microunits of Space and Time,’
Criminology & Public Policy
,
15
:
31
56
.

Footnotes

1

All police forces in the United Kingdom make S&S and crime data publicly available: https://data.police.uk/.

2

In robustness checks (unreported), we confirm that our results are not sensitive to whether we measure ethnic composition using the Herfindahl–Hirschman concentration index using the broad ethnic categories in the census or the percent of residents who are black.

3

The SDM aligns with our theoretical expectations for the process that might create spatial dependence between the regressors and police stops; however, we also explore the fit of other spatial regression models and whether these affect our coefficient estimate for inequality (unreported). They do not.

Annex

Table A.1.

Stop and searches: Birmingham, Liverpool and Manchester (2019)

BirminghamLiverpoolManchester
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)
Gini0.299***0.297***0.197***0.198***0.267***0.269***
(0.029)(0.029)(0.036)(0.036)(0.045)(0.045)
Average property value (log)−0.308***−0.329***−0.114−0.112−0.118−0.134
(0.057)(0.058)(0.072)(0.073)(0.090)(0.093)
Density (log)0.168***0.170***0.063*0.063*0.330***0.327***
(0.031)(0.031)(0.037)(0.037)(0.051)(0.051)
Income deprivation−0.132***−0.154***0.184***0.186***−0.074−0.087
(0.046)(0.048)(0.058)(0.061)(0.070)(0.072)
Crime deprivation0.270***0.272***0.385***0.385***0.332***0.332***
(0.035)(0.035)(0.047)(0.047)(0.058)(0.058)
Drugs rate0.310***0.309***0.374***0.374***0.277***0.278***
(0.027)(0.027)(0.035)(0.035)(0.041)(0.041)
Non−white (%)0.331***0.337***0.163***0.163***0.125***0.128***
(0.031)(0.031)(0.033)(0.033)(0.046)(0.046)
Rail station distance−0.250***−0.247***−0.187***−0.187***−0.223***−0.223***
(0.030)(0.030)(0.031)(0.031)(0.043)(0.043)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.0390.005−0.026
(0.026)(0.034)(0.040)
LAD fixed effectsYYYYYY
Observations1,6801,6809109101,6731,673
Log likelihood−5,246.513−5,245.421−3,392.191−3,392.182−2,726.814−2,726.591
theta1.271*** (0.053)1.273*** (0.053)1.517*** (0.078)1.517*** (0.078)0.624*** (0.038)0.624*** (0.038)
Akaike information criterion10,523.03010,522.8406,810.3836,812.3655,489.6275,491.181
BirminghamLiverpoolManchester
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)
Gini0.299***0.297***0.197***0.198***0.267***0.269***
(0.029)(0.029)(0.036)(0.036)(0.045)(0.045)
Average property value (log)−0.308***−0.329***−0.114−0.112−0.118−0.134
(0.057)(0.058)(0.072)(0.073)(0.090)(0.093)
Density (log)0.168***0.170***0.063*0.063*0.330***0.327***
(0.031)(0.031)(0.037)(0.037)(0.051)(0.051)
Income deprivation−0.132***−0.154***0.184***0.186***−0.074−0.087
(0.046)(0.048)(0.058)(0.061)(0.070)(0.072)
Crime deprivation0.270***0.272***0.385***0.385***0.332***0.332***
(0.035)(0.035)(0.047)(0.047)(0.058)(0.058)
Drugs rate0.310***0.309***0.374***0.374***0.277***0.278***
(0.027)(0.027)(0.035)(0.035)(0.041)(0.041)
Non−white (%)0.331***0.337***0.163***0.163***0.125***0.128***
(0.031)(0.031)(0.033)(0.033)(0.046)(0.046)
Rail station distance−0.250***−0.247***−0.187***−0.187***−0.223***−0.223***
(0.030)(0.030)(0.031)(0.031)(0.043)(0.043)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.0390.005−0.026
(0.026)(0.034)(0.040)
LAD fixed effectsYYYYYY
Observations1,6801,6809109101,6731,673
Log likelihood−5,246.513−5,245.421−3,392.191−3,392.182−2,726.814−2,726.591
theta1.271*** (0.053)1.273*** (0.053)1.517*** (0.078)1.517*** (0.078)0.624*** (0.038)0.624*** (0.038)
Akaike information criterion10,523.03010,522.8406,810.3836,812.3655,489.6275,491.181

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one).

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Table A.1.

Stop and searches: Birmingham, Liverpool and Manchester (2019)

BirminghamLiverpoolManchester
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)
Gini0.299***0.297***0.197***0.198***0.267***0.269***
(0.029)(0.029)(0.036)(0.036)(0.045)(0.045)
Average property value (log)−0.308***−0.329***−0.114−0.112−0.118−0.134
(0.057)(0.058)(0.072)(0.073)(0.090)(0.093)
Density (log)0.168***0.170***0.063*0.063*0.330***0.327***
(0.031)(0.031)(0.037)(0.037)(0.051)(0.051)
Income deprivation−0.132***−0.154***0.184***0.186***−0.074−0.087
(0.046)(0.048)(0.058)(0.061)(0.070)(0.072)
Crime deprivation0.270***0.272***0.385***0.385***0.332***0.332***
(0.035)(0.035)(0.047)(0.047)(0.058)(0.058)
Drugs rate0.310***0.309***0.374***0.374***0.277***0.278***
(0.027)(0.027)(0.035)(0.035)(0.041)(0.041)
Non−white (%)0.331***0.337***0.163***0.163***0.125***0.128***
(0.031)(0.031)(0.033)(0.033)(0.046)(0.046)
Rail station distance−0.250***−0.247***−0.187***−0.187***−0.223***−0.223***
(0.030)(0.030)(0.031)(0.031)(0.043)(0.043)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.0390.005−0.026
(0.026)(0.034)(0.040)
LAD fixed effectsYYYYYY
Observations1,6801,6809109101,6731,673
Log likelihood−5,246.513−5,245.421−3,392.191−3,392.182−2,726.814−2,726.591
theta1.271*** (0.053)1.273*** (0.053)1.517*** (0.078)1.517*** (0.078)0.624*** (0.038)0.624*** (0.038)
Akaike information criterion10,523.03010,522.8406,810.3836,812.3655,489.6275,491.181
BirminghamLiverpoolManchester
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)
Gini0.299***0.297***0.197***0.198***0.267***0.269***
(0.029)(0.029)(0.036)(0.036)(0.045)(0.045)
Average property value (log)−0.308***−0.329***−0.114−0.112−0.118−0.134
(0.057)(0.058)(0.072)(0.073)(0.090)(0.093)
Density (log)0.168***0.170***0.063*0.063*0.330***0.327***
(0.031)(0.031)(0.037)(0.037)(0.051)(0.051)
Income deprivation−0.132***−0.154***0.184***0.186***−0.074−0.087
(0.046)(0.048)(0.058)(0.061)(0.070)(0.072)
Crime deprivation0.270***0.272***0.385***0.385***0.332***0.332***
(0.035)(0.035)(0.047)(0.047)(0.058)(0.058)
Drugs rate0.310***0.309***0.374***0.374***0.277***0.278***
(0.027)(0.027)(0.035)(0.035)(0.041)(0.041)
Non−white (%)0.331***0.337***0.163***0.163***0.125***0.128***
(0.031)(0.031)(0.033)(0.033)(0.046)(0.046)
Rail station distance−0.250***−0.247***−0.187***−0.187***−0.223***−0.223***
(0.030)(0.030)(0.031)(0.031)(0.043)(0.043)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.0390.005−0.026
(0.026)(0.034)(0.040)
LAD fixed effectsYYYYYY
Observations1,6801,6809109101,6731,673
Log likelihood−5,246.513−5,245.421−3,392.191−3,392.182−2,726.814−2,726.591
theta1.271*** (0.053)1.273*** (0.053)1.517*** (0.078)1.517*** (0.078)0.624*** (0.038)0.624*** (0.038)
Akaike information criterion10,523.03010,522.8406,810.3836,812.3655,489.6275,491.181

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one).

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Table A.2.

Stop and search rate per 1,000 residents

Dependent variable:
Ln(Stops/1,000 residents)
OLSSDM
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Gini0.153***0.146***0.146***0.137***
(0.019)(0.019)(0.019)(0.016)
Average property value (log)−0.133***−0.145***−0.175***−0.187***
(0.029)(0.029)(0.039)(0.041)
Density (log)−0.019−0.019−0.114***−0.116***
(0.017)(0.017)(0.024)(0.023)
Income deprivation0.084***0.075***0.077**0.067*
(0.021)(0.022)(0.039)(0.036)
Crime deprivation0.225***0.223***0.175***0.174***
(0.016)(0.016)(0.019)(0.018)
Drugs rate0.357***0.358***0.337***0.337***
(0.014)(0.014)(0.016)(0.015)
Non-white (%)0.126***0.126***0.0630.070
(0.021)(0.021)(0.101)(0.090)
TfL station distance−0.075***−0.076***−0.133***−0.132***
(0.013)(0.013)(0.029)(0.032)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.032**−0.047***
(0.014)(0.016)
Borough fixed effectsYYYY
Rho0.060.06
Observations4,8314,8314,8314,831
R20.4190.420
Adjusted R20.4140.415
Log likelihood−5,881.457−5,877.020
Sigma20.6680.667
Akaike information criterion11,928.91011,924.040
Residual SE0.830 (df = 4790)0.829 (df = 4789)
F statistic86.409*** (df = 40; 4790)84.505*** (df = 41; 4789)
Wald test (df = 1)6.019**5.885**
LR test (df = 1)5.976**6.262**
Dependent variable:
Ln(Stops/1,000 residents)
OLSSDM
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Gini0.153***0.146***0.146***0.137***
(0.019)(0.019)(0.019)(0.016)
Average property value (log)−0.133***−0.145***−0.175***−0.187***
(0.029)(0.029)(0.039)(0.041)
Density (log)−0.019−0.019−0.114***−0.116***
(0.017)(0.017)(0.024)(0.023)
Income deprivation0.084***0.075***0.077**0.067*
(0.021)(0.022)(0.039)(0.036)
Crime deprivation0.225***0.223***0.175***0.174***
(0.016)(0.016)(0.019)(0.018)
Drugs rate0.357***0.358***0.337***0.337***
(0.014)(0.014)(0.016)(0.015)
Non-white (%)0.126***0.126***0.0630.070
(0.021)(0.021)(0.101)(0.090)
TfL station distance−0.075***−0.076***−0.133***−0.132***
(0.013)(0.013)(0.029)(0.032)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.032**−0.047***
(0.014)(0.016)
Borough fixed effectsYYYY
Rho0.060.06
Observations4,8314,8314,8314,831
R20.4190.420
Adjusted R20.4140.415
Log likelihood−5,881.457−5,877.020
Sigma20.6680.667
Akaike information criterion11,928.91011,924.040
Residual SE0.830 (df = 4790)0.829 (df = 4789)
F statistic86.409*** (df = 40; 4790)84.505*** (df = 41; 4789)
Wald test (df = 1)6.019**5.885**
LR test (df = 1)5.976**6.262**

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one).

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Table A.2.

Stop and search rate per 1,000 residents

Dependent variable:
Ln(Stops/1,000 residents)
OLSSDM
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Gini0.153***0.146***0.146***0.137***
(0.019)(0.019)(0.019)(0.016)
Average property value (log)−0.133***−0.145***−0.175***−0.187***
(0.029)(0.029)(0.039)(0.041)
Density (log)−0.019−0.019−0.114***−0.116***
(0.017)(0.017)(0.024)(0.023)
Income deprivation0.084***0.075***0.077**0.067*
(0.021)(0.022)(0.039)(0.036)
Crime deprivation0.225***0.223***0.175***0.174***
(0.016)(0.016)(0.019)(0.018)
Drugs rate0.357***0.358***0.337***0.337***
(0.014)(0.014)(0.016)(0.015)
Non-white (%)0.126***0.126***0.0630.070
(0.021)(0.021)(0.101)(0.090)
TfL station distance−0.075***−0.076***−0.133***−0.132***
(0.013)(0.013)(0.029)(0.032)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.032**−0.047***
(0.014)(0.016)
Borough fixed effectsYYYY
Rho0.060.06
Observations4,8314,8314,8314,831
R20.4190.420
Adjusted R20.4140.415
Log likelihood−5,881.457−5,877.020
Sigma20.6680.667
Akaike information criterion11,928.91011,924.040
Residual SE0.830 (df = 4790)0.829 (df = 4789)
F statistic86.409*** (df = 40; 4790)84.505*** (df = 41; 4789)
Wald test (df = 1)6.019**5.885**
LR test (df = 1)5.976**6.262**
Dependent variable:
Ln(Stops/1,000 residents)
OLSSDM
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Gini0.153***0.146***0.146***0.137***
(0.019)(0.019)(0.019)(0.016)
Average property value (log)−0.133***−0.145***−0.175***−0.187***
(0.029)(0.029)(0.039)(0.041)
Density (log)−0.019−0.019−0.114***−0.116***
(0.017)(0.017)(0.024)(0.023)
Income deprivation0.084***0.075***0.077**0.067*
(0.021)(0.022)(0.039)(0.036)
Crime deprivation0.225***0.223***0.175***0.174***
(0.016)(0.016)(0.019)(0.018)
Drugs rate0.357***0.358***0.337***0.337***
(0.014)(0.014)(0.016)(0.015)
Non-white (%)0.126***0.126***0.0630.070
(0.021)(0.021)(0.101)(0.090)
TfL station distance−0.075***−0.076***−0.133***−0.132***
(0.013)(0.013)(0.029)(0.032)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.032**−0.047***
(0.014)(0.016)
Borough fixed effectsYYYY
Rho0.060.06
Observations4,8314,8314,8314,831
R20.4190.420
Adjusted R20.4140.415
Log likelihood−5,881.457−5,877.020
Sigma20.6680.667
Akaike information criterion11,928.91011,924.040
Residual SE0.830 (df = 4790)0.829 (df = 4789)
F statistic86.409*** (df = 40; 4790)84.505*** (df = 41; 4789)
Wald test (df = 1)6.019**5.885**
LR test (df = 1)5.976**6.262**

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one).

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Table A.3.

Regression results without Westminster Borough

Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
(1)(2)(3)
Gini0.085***0.272***0.253***
(0.019)(0.023)(0.023)
Average property value (log)−0.196***−0.232***
(0.035)(0.035)
Density (log)0.132***0.131***
(0.020)(0.020)
Income deprivation−0.028−0.059**
(0.026)(0.026)
Crime deprivation0.211***0.207***
(0.019)(0.019)
Drugs rate0.430***0.433***
(0.017)(0.017)
Non-white (%)0.218***0.216***
(0.025)(0.025)
TfL station distance−0.132***−0.135***
(0.016)(0.016)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.085***
(0.017)
Borough fixed effectsYYY
Observations4,7044,7044,704
Log likelihood−22,394.250−20,948.670−20,938.030
Theta0.652*** (0.012)1.098*** (0.022)1.102*** (0.022)
Akaike Inf. Crit.44,792.50041,977.35041,958.070
Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
(1)(2)(3)
Gini0.085***0.272***0.253***
(0.019)(0.023)(0.023)
Average property value (log)−0.196***−0.232***
(0.035)(0.035)
Density (log)0.132***0.131***
(0.020)(0.020)
Income deprivation−0.028−0.059**
(0.026)(0.026)
Crime deprivation0.211***0.207***
(0.019)(0.019)
Drugs rate0.430***0.433***
(0.017)(0.017)
Non-white (%)0.218***0.216***
(0.025)(0.025)
TfL station distance−0.132***−0.135***
(0.016)(0.016)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.085***
(0.017)
Borough fixed effectsYYY
Observations4,7044,7044,704
Log likelihood−22,394.250−20,948.670−20,938.030
Theta0.652*** (0.012)1.098*** (0.022)1.102*** (0.022)
Akaike Inf. Crit.44,792.50041,977.35041,958.070

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one). All models exclude Westminster Borough.

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

Table A.3.

Regression results without Westminster Borough

Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
(1)(2)(3)
Gini0.085***0.272***0.253***
(0.019)(0.023)(0.023)
Average property value (log)−0.196***−0.232***
(0.035)(0.035)
Density (log)0.132***0.131***
(0.020)(0.020)
Income deprivation−0.028−0.059**
(0.026)(0.026)
Crime deprivation0.211***0.207***
(0.019)(0.019)
Drugs rate0.430***0.433***
(0.017)(0.017)
Non-white (%)0.218***0.216***
(0.025)(0.025)
TfL station distance−0.132***−0.135***
(0.016)(0.016)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.085***
(0.017)
Borough fixed effectsYYY
Observations4,7044,7044,704
Log likelihood−22,394.250−20,948.670−20,938.030
Theta0.652*** (0.012)1.098*** (0.022)1.102*** (0.022)
Akaike Inf. Crit.44,792.50041,977.35041,958.070
Dependent variable:
Stop and searches, London (2019)
(1)(2)(3)
Gini0.085***0.272***0.253***
(0.019)(0.023)(0.023)
Average property value (log)−0.196***−0.232***
(0.035)(0.035)
Density (log)0.132***0.131***
(0.020)(0.020)
Income deprivation−0.028−0.059**
(0.026)(0.026)
Crime deprivation0.211***0.207***
(0.019)(0.019)
Drugs rate0.430***0.433***
(0.017)(0.017)
Non-white (%)0.218***0.216***
(0.025)(0.025)
TfL station distance−0.132***−0.135***
(0.016)(0.016)
Gini: Income deprivation−0.085***
(0.017)
Borough fixed effectsYYY
Observations4,7044,7044,704
Log likelihood−22,394.250−20,948.670−20,938.030
Theta0.652*** (0.012)1.098*** (0.022)1.102*** (0.022)
Akaike Inf. Crit.44,792.50041,977.35041,958.070

All independent variables are standardized (mean equal to zero, standard deviation equal to one). All models exclude Westminster Borough.

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.