Abstract

Kleine–Levin syndrome (KLS) is a rare disorder with symptoms that include periodic hypersomnia, cognitive and behavioural disturbances. Large series of patients are lacking. In order to report on various KLS symptoms, identify risk factors and analyse treatment response, we performed a systematic review of 195 articles, written in English and non-English languages, which are available on Medline dating from 1962 to 2004. Doubtful or duplicate cases, case series without individual details and reviews (n = 56 articles) were excluded. In addition, the details of 186 patients from 139 articles were compiled. Primary KLS cases (n = 168) were found mostly in men (68%) and occurred sporadically worldwide. The median age of onset was 15 years (range 4–82 years, 81% during the second decade) and the syndrome lasted 8 years, with seven episodes of 10 days, recurring every 3.5 months (median values) with the disease lasting longer in women and in patients with less frequent episodes during the first year. It was precipitated most frequently by infections (38.2%), head trauma (9%), or alcohol consumption (5.4%). Common symptoms were hypersomnia (100%), cognitive changes (96%, including a specific feeling of derealization), eating disturbances (80%), hypersexuality (43%), compulsions (29%), and depressed mood (48%). In 75 treated patients (213 trials), somnolence decreased using stimulants (mainly amphetamines) in 40% of cases, while neuroleptics and antidepressants were of poor benefit. Only lithium (but not carbamazepine or other antiepileptics) had a higher reported response rate (41%) for stopping relapses when compared to medical abstention (19%). Secondary KLS (n = 18) patients were older and had more frequent and longer episodes, but had clinical symptoms and treatment responses similar to primary cases. In conclusion, KLS is a unique disease which may be more severe in female and secondary cases.

Introduction

Kleine–Levin syndrome (KLS) is a rare disease characterized by recurrent episodes of hypersomnia and, to various degrees, behavioural or cognitive disturbances, compulsive eating behaviour and hypersexuality (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2005). The disease predominantly affects adolescent males. Although no population-based studies reporting on KLS prevalence are available, it is generally considered an exceptionally rare disease.

What appears to be the first case of KLS was reported by Brierre de Boismont in 1862. It is notable that this case occurred several decades prior to the 1916–1927 epidemic of encephalitis lethargica. Multiple cases of recurrent hypersomnia were first collected and reported in Frankfurt by Willi Kleine in 1925 (Kleine, 1925). Max Levin (1929, 1936) emphasized the association of periodic somnolence with morbid hunger in 1929 and 1936. Critchley (1962), reviewed 15 previously published cases, added 11 of his own personal cases, notably young marines in the British Royal Navy where he had served during World War II (Critchley and Hoffman, 1942) and gave the eponymous name to the disease, ‘Kleine–Levin syndrome’. He pointed out the male predominance, the onset during adolescence, the compulsive rather than bulimic nature of the eating disorder, and the trend of the disease to spontaneously disappear.

Numerous KLS case-reports have been published since, speculating on the yet unknown cause of this condition. Psychiatrists noticed early that KLS reoccurs as endogenous depression does, and that mood stabilizing drugs occasionally benefit patients. Additionally, some patients appear depressed during episodes and temporarily hypomanic upon recovery. Psychoanalytic and psychodynamic hypotheses about KLS emerged in the seventies (Markman, 1967; Miller, 1970; Schlierf, 1975). The striking clinical features of the disease, selected reports of definite neuropathology in post-mortem cases and the high frequency of infections at disease onset led to a more organic hypothesis, most notably the possibility of a viral or post-infectious autoimmune encephalitis with primary impact on the hypothalamus. In favour of autoimmunity, a European group recently identified the human leucocyte antigen (HLA) subtype DQB1*02 as possibly being associated with the disease (Dauvilliers et al., 2002).

Only a limited number of reviews have been published on KLS (Smolik and Roth, 1988; Lemire, 1993; Billiard and Carlander, 1998) and these articles have not systematically analysed all available data. Rather, these publications included only a limited number of patients or reviewed a subset of available papers in selected languages, most notably Czech, English and French. We have undertaken a careful systematic review of all published cases extensively described since 1962 with the aim of better characterizing the spectrum of the disease, risk factors and possible treatments.

Methods

Literature searches

A Medline search was performed on December 7, 2004 using ‘Kleine–Levin’, ‘Kleine–Levin syndrome’, and ‘periodic hypersomnia’. The search was limited to 1962–2004 and generated a total of 195 articles. Non-English language articles were systematically translated. An initial screening eliminated 41 articles that reported on cases published twice (including reviews or viewpoints), cases that did not meet ICSD diagnostic criteria for recurrent hypersomnia (Lamote de Grignon and Fernandez Alvarez, 1967; Smirne et al., 1970; Plasse et al., 1982; Zeitlhofer et al., 1982; Cuetter, 1985; Menkes, 1992; Muller et al., 1998; Sethi and Bhargava, 2002), or that had hypersomnia or hyperphagia in the context of recurrent depressive episodes (Cante and Marocchino, 1970). Cases with menstruation-linked hypersomnia were also removed (Billiard et al., 1975; Lenz, 1980; Papy et al., 1982; Sachs et al., 1982). Abstracts of scientific meetings were not included. Finally, the large recent series of 30 patients by Dauvilliers et al. (2002) and 34 patients by Gadoth et al. (2001) were not included due to potential overlap with other published cases and insufficient individual clinical data. We thoroughly reviewed the 148 remaining articles reporting on 186 detailed ‘true’ KLS cases. A list of included references and cases is posted as supplementary material at http://brain.oxfordjournals.org.

Data collected

A mean of four new cases/year was reported over the 40-year review period, with a tendency toward increasing values in more recent years: 1970s (2.7/year), 1980s (3.5/year) and 1990s (5.8/year). Single cases were reported in 84% of the articles, while the remaining 16% reported on 2–16 patients. The following qualitative and quantitative variables were obtained (percentage of data found indicated in parentheses) for each case: country of residence (100), sex (100), age at onset (99), familial (63) and medical history (62), indication of normal or delayed psychomotor development (67), disease duration (64), mean patient duration of KLS episodes (87), mean duration of inter-episode interval(s) (92), number of episodes (72), presence and nature of precipitating events at KLS onset (61), presence of hypersomnia (100, in at least two episodes), eating behaviour disorder (93, in at least one episode), sexual behaviour disorder (93, in at least one episode), other behavioural disorders (35), cognitive impairment (59), hallucinations, delusion and derealization (55), mood disorders (51), irritability (47). Additionally, we collected information on each case pertaining to autonomic dysfunction, measurements of blood virus titres, urinary and blood hormone levels (27), CSF measurements (42), characteristics of the EEG during and after an episode (64), sleep monitoring during (28) and after (15) an episode, brain imagery (45), and indication of first (52), second, and third treatments used, with results. Due to the unpredictable course of the disease, a treatment was considered as ‘effective’ when its administration was followed by a reported cessation of relapses, even if the authors did not know if the cessation was spontaneous or drug-related.

Stratification and statistics

We classified patients as primary or secondary KLS cases, depending on the absence or presence of neurological symptoms prior to KLS onset that persisted between episodes. The median disease course was calculated using a Kaplan–Meier survival curve analysis in all subjects, with ‘censored data’ in patients without information regarding termination of KLS included as still affected at the last time of evaluation and ‘non-censored data’ in patients with terminated KLS. In this analysis, the disease was considered as ‘terminated’ if: (i) the patient was re-evaluated and found to be free of episodes for at least twice the length of the previous mean in-between-episode interval(s), or (ii) the last observed episodes had become shorter, less frequent and with less hypersomnia, suggesting an ending to the disease. This definition does not rule out the possibility of occasional but unlikely relapses later in life for a few of these patients. Between-group comparisons were performed using Student's t-tests for quantitative measures, and chi-square tests for qualitative measures. Disease duration was compared between groups using the log rank test. Data are presented as median or mean ± SD.

Results

Demographics

Of 186 reported cases, 168 were primary and 18 secondary. Patients with primary KLS were described worldwide across all continents: 34 from the Americas (27 in the United States of America, 2 in Canada, 3 in Brazil, 1 in Argentina), 75 from Europe (22 in Germany, 12 in Great Britain, 10 in France, 7 in Czechoslovakia, 7 in Italy, 6 in Sweden, 3 in the Netherlands, 1 in Ireland, 2 in Spain, 2 in Denmark, 1 in Belgium, 1 in Croatia, 1 in Cyprus, 1 in Poland), 1 from Africa (Nigeria), 53 from Asia (27 in Israel, 10 in India, 6 in Japan, 4 in Turkey, 3 in Iran, 1 in Taiwan, 1 in China, 1 in Pakistan), and 3 from Australia. The large majority (98%) of cases were sporadic, with the exception of a family with a brother and a sister affected (Katz and Ropper, 2002), another with an uncle and nephew affected (Thacore et al., 1969) and one family with two first-degree cousins affected and one uncle with narcolepsy (Janicki et al., 2001).

One hundred and fifteen (68.4%) patients were male giving a 2:1 male–female ratio. The age at KLS onset was 16.9 ± 8.5 years, with a median of 15 years and a range of 4 (Zhou, 2004) to 82 years (Badino et al., 1992). In 81% of cases, KLS onset occurred during the second decade (Fig. 1). Six patients had late onset (>35 years) but otherwise usual symptoms (Gallinek, 1962; Yassa and Nair, 1978; Carpenter et al., 1982; Smolik and Roth, 1988; Hegarty and Merriam, 1990; Badino et al., 1992; Manni et al., 1993).

Fig. 1

Age of 168 patients at KLS onset. The y-axis displays the number of patients in each class of age.

Fig. 1

Age of 168 patients at KLS onset. The y-axis displays the number of patients in each class of age.

Precipitating factors and infections

While primary KLS may develop insidiously, various precipitating factors listed in Table 1 were reported in 61% of patients. The most frequent was a trivial infection such as a flu-like illness or a non-specific fever (Lavie et al., 1979; Shukla et al., 1982; Reynolds et al., 1984; Jensen, 1985; Visscher et al., 1990; Sadeghu, 1999; Rosenow et al., 2000; Katz and Ropper, 2002), an upper respiratory tract infection and tonsillitis (Fresco et al., 1971; Iakhno, 1980; Goldberg, 1983; Fernandez et al., 1990; Chesson et al., 1991; Manni et al., 1993; Salter and White, 1993; Pike and Stores, 1994; Crumley, 1997; Rosenow et al., 2000; Muratori et al., 2002; Poppe et al., 2003), and, more rarely, a summer gastro-enteritis (Gallinek, 1962; Lu et al., 2000; Portilla et al., 2002; Zhou, 2004) or a severe infection. Urinary or eye infections were never reported. In infection-triggered KLS, symptoms of KLS occurred shortly (between 3 and 5 days) after the onset of fever. The agents responsible for the first infection were rarely identified and included Epstein–Barr virus and varicella–zoster virus (Salter and White, 1993), Asian influenza virus (Garland et al., 1965), enterovirus (Fernandez et al., 1990), post-typhoid vaccine (Smolik and Roth, 1988), and Streptococcus in the context of a septicaemia (Gallinek, 1967), and scarlet fever (Smolik and Roth, 1988). Serum virus titres were performed and found normal in 7 of 9 patients. Similarly, there were no increases in whole blood white cell counts, except in one case. Cerebrospinal fluid was obtained through lumbar puncture during KLS episodes in 70 cases. All samples had normal white cell counts/glucose levels and negative bacterial cultures. Other triggers were more rare and variable. In a few cases, a large or first alcohol consumption or marijuana use on an evening was followed by the first episode the following morning (Wilkus and Chiles, 1975; Chiles and Wilkus, 1976; Mayer et al., 1998; Janicki et al., 2001; Landtblom et al., 2002, 2003). Head trauma, including a knock-out during boxing (Smolik and Roth, 1988; Will et al., 1988; Janicki et al., 2001), physical exertion and psychological distress (Kellett, 1977; Masi et al., 2000; Arias et al., 2002), surgery with general or local (dental) anaesthesia, lactation and menses (Lavie et al., 1981), but not at menarche, and not as in menstruation-linked hypersomnia (Gilbert, 1964; Duffy and Davison, 1968; Papy et al., 1982; Shukla et al., 1982; Janicki et al., 2001) were other factors occasionally reported at KLS onset.

Table 1

Precipitating factors reported at onset and before reoccurrences of Kleine–Levin syndrome in 168 patients

Event at KLS onset
 
No. of patients
 
Frequency at KLS onset (%)
 
Frequency before KLS reoccurrences (%)
 
None reported 66 39 84 
Infection or fever 72 42.8 8.9 
    Unspecific fever, flu-like fever 42 25  
    Upper respiratory tract infection: Tonsillitis, sore throat, cough 20 12  
    Gastro enteritis  
    Identified virus/bacteria: Streptococcus, Asian influenza, chicken pox and mononucleosis, enterovirus, post-typhoid vaccine fever  
Alcohol or marijuana 4.2 0.6 
Head trauma 2.4 
Sleep deprivation mental effort, stress 
Menses or lactation 3.6 0.6 
Miscellaneous: local/general anaesthesia, physical exertion, clavicle fracture 3.6 4.8 
Event at KLS onset
 
No. of patients
 
Frequency at KLS onset (%)
 
Frequency before KLS reoccurrences (%)
 
None reported 66 39 84 
Infection or fever 72 42.8 8.9 
    Unspecific fever, flu-like fever 42 25  
    Upper respiratory tract infection: Tonsillitis, sore throat, cough 20 12  
    Gastro enteritis  
    Identified virus/bacteria: Streptococcus, Asian influenza, chicken pox and mononucleosis, enterovirus, post-typhoid vaccine fever  
Alcohol or marijuana 4.2 0.6 
Head trauma 2.4 
Sleep deprivation mental effort, stress 
Menses or lactation 3.6 0.6 
Miscellaneous: local/general anaesthesia, physical exertion, clavicle fracture 3.6 4.8 

Events triggering subsequent episodes were described in only 27 cases (Table 1) and contained a similarly high proportion of infections (fever, sore throat or zoster before each episode in five patients). Surgery with anaesthesia—one case had three episodes, each one after general anaesthesia for orthopaedic or dental surgeries (Turgman and Braham, 1977)—physical exertion or mental effort, sunstroke, alcohol, jet lag, and a bout of hemiparetic migraine were also reported as subsequent triggers for episodes.

Duration of the disease, episodes and intervals in-between episodes

The duration of primary KLS was reported or could be calculated in 110 patients. It ranged from 0.5 to 41 years. Using Kaplan–Meier analysis with 41% of the data censored, KLS as a disease lasted a median of 8 years (Fig. 2). Termination was reported in 65 subjects, with 39 subjects having no more episodes for more than twice the mean inter-episode length and 26 subjects where a clear decrease in symptom severity and episode frequency was noted at the last visit. The median in the 65 cases with a known reported termination was 4 years. The mean age at the end of KLS for these 65 cases was 23 ± 12 years. There was no correlation between age at onset and disease duration (r = −0.01, P = 0.95), suggesting no preferential termination at a determined and possibly more mature age. In 33 patients with a described reported end of the disease, KLS episodes decreased in frequency, duration and intensity (less pronounced hypersomnia) prior to termination in 30 patients, while the frequency decreased but duration increased in one patient and frequency increased in two patients.

Fig. 2

Duration of KLS (n = 110 patients, 41% censored), presented as a Kaplan–Meier analysis. On the x-axis is the total duration of the disease (years), on the y-axis is the cumulative decreasing percentage of patients still presenting with KLS episode. The median duration of the disease (dotted line) is 8 years.

Fig. 2

Duration of KLS (n = 110 patients, 41% censored), presented as a Kaplan–Meier analysis. On the x-axis is the total duration of the disease (years), on the y-axis is the cumulative decreasing percentage of patients still presenting with KLS episode. The median duration of the disease (dotted line) is 8 years.

The 110 patients presented with a median of seven episodes (range: 2–130 episodes, mean ± SD: 12 ± 15 episodes). The episodes lasted between 2.5 and 80 days, with a median of 10 days, and a mean of 12 ± 9 days (Fig. 3, left panel). Inter-episode duration ranged from 0.5 to 72 months, with a median of 3.5 months and a mean of 6 ± 10 months (Fig. 3, right panel).

Fig. 3

Histogram of the duration of KLS episodes, in days (left panel), with a median of 10 days, and of the duration of the interval between episodes, in months (right panel), with a median of 3 months.

Fig. 3

Histogram of the duration of KLS episodes, in days (left panel), with a median of 10 days, and of the duration of the interval between episodes, in months (right panel), with a median of 3 months.

Symptoms of KLS

Hypersomnia

Hypersomnia, a major clinical symptom of KLS, is mandatory for diagnosis and was present in all cases. When reported, usual sleep duration during episodes ranged from 12 to 24 h/day (mean: 18 ± 2 h, median: 18 h). Qualitative information such as ‘the patient spent most of the night and the day asleep’ was reported in other cases. Prodromic symptoms included sudden overwhelming tiredness, i.e. ‘feeling drawn towards his bed’, or ‘reluctant to get up in the morning’ (Prabhakaran et al., 1970). Several authors noted that their patients remained arousable, waking up spontaneously to void and eat, but were irritable or aggressive when awakened or prevented from sleep. The need for sleep was so intense that a male teenager ‘was found sleeping under a neighbour's porch’ (Powers and Gunderman, 1978), another ‘left his classroom during a lesson, lay down on the floor of the corridor and fell asleep’ (Frank et al., 1974), while an adult patient was ‘found asleep on the pavement of the street’ (Prabhakaran et al., 1970). At the end of an episode, a short-lasting insomnia was noted in three cases (Gallinek, 1967; Frösher et al., 1991; Russell and Grunstein, 1992). Sleep symptoms changed from frank hypersomnia during the first episodes to a heavy fatigue accompanied by a feeling ‘as if in twilight between sleep and waking’ during later episodes. Cataplexy and sleep paralysis were never mentioned as co-morbid symptoms.

Sleep was monitored during an episode in 40 patients, using daytime nap (n = 5), nocturnal sleep (n = 19), multiple sleep latency tests (n = 7, mean sleep latency: 3.6 ± 1.1 min), and 24–72 h continuous recordings (n = 10). Mean total sleep time was 445 ± 122 min during the night (Stage 1, 6 ± 4%; Stage 2, 56 ± 9%; Stage 3–4, 19 ± 11%; REM sleep, 19 ± 6%) and 838 ± 288 min on a 24 h basis. REM sleep was commonly reported during daytime sleep recordings or multiple sleep latency tests, with six of 19 patients (21%) having a narcolepsy-like pattern (≥2 sleep onset in REM periods). In other cases, the duration of sleep stages during daytime sleep contained an excess of Stages 1–2. Only two patients had a moderately decreased nocturnal sleep efficiency (Reynolds et al., 1980; Gadoth et al., 1987) but they were not monitored during the daytime. Rare sleep abnormalities such as an absence of REM sleep in one patient (Striano et al., 1986), an absence of slow wave sleep in one patient (Reynolds et al., 1980), and recurrent interruption of Stage 2 by fear and stereotypical movements in one patient (Striano et al., 1986), were also found. Mean polysomnographic recording results in 29 subjects were similar and not statistically different from those reported in similar independent samples (Gadoth et al., 2001; Dauvilliers et al., 2002).

Cognitive disturbances

Almost all patients had cognitive disturbances such as confusion, concentration, attention and memory defects. These were evident when interviewed during episodes (such as abnormal responses to question), or reported on subsequent interviews, as a recall of a previous episode (Table 2).

Table 2

Frequency of symptoms during episodes of Kleine–Levin syndrome

Symptoms
 
Patients with the symptom/total
 
Percentage
 
Hypersomnia 168/168 100 
Cognitive disorders 98/102 96 
    Abnormal speech 35/58 60 
    Confusion 24/47 51 
    Amnesia 24/50 48 
    Derealization 22/93 24 
    Hallucinations 13/93 14 
    Delusions 15/93 16 
Eating behaviour disorders 125/157 80 
    Megaphagia 97/125 78 
    Craving for sweets 15/125 12 
    Increased drinking 10/125 
    Binge eating 7/125 
    Decreased appetite 6/125 
    Food utilization behaviour 5/125 
Depression 41/86 48 
Irritability 79/86 92 
Other behavioural disorders   
    Hypersexuality 67/155 43 
    Compulsions to sing, write, pace 17/59 29 
Symptoms
 
Patients with the symptom/total
 
Percentage
 
Hypersomnia 168/168 100 
Cognitive disorders 98/102 96 
    Abnormal speech 35/58 60 
    Confusion 24/47 51 
    Amnesia 24/50 48 
    Derealization 22/93 24 
    Hallucinations 13/93 14 
    Delusions 15/93 16 
Eating behaviour disorders 125/157 80 
    Megaphagia 97/125 78 
    Craving for sweets 15/125 12 
    Increased drinking 10/125 
    Binge eating 7/125 
    Decreased appetite 6/125 
    Food utilization behaviour 5/125 
Depression 41/86 48 
Irritability 79/86 92 
Other behavioural disorders   
    Hypersexuality 67/155 43 
    Compulsions to sing, write, pace 17/59 29 

Abnormal speech was reported in two-thirds of cases. This included being mute (n = 5), without spontaneous speech (n = 6), using monosyllabic or short sentences with limited vocabulary (n = 9), having slurred, muddled, incoherent (n = 6), or childish (n = 1) stereotypical language, slow to speak and to comprehend (n = 13), with verbal perseverations (such as answering with the time at each question) or echoing questions (n = 4). Formal cognitive and memory tests were rarely used and when they were, their validity was questionable in uncooperative, irritable, sleepy, and inattentive subjects (Malhotra et al., 1997; Landtblom et al., 2002, 2003). In one report it was determined that a teenager had an intelligence quotient of 66 during an episode and 88 thereafter (Fresco et al., 1971). This state of mental ‘viscosity/slowness’ was qualitatively described by some patients as a ‘struggle to follow a thought’ (Chesson et al., 1991) requiring ‘too much energy with racing thoughts’, while ‘everything was going fast’ (Crumley, 1997). A 13-year-old girl found multiple simultaneous stimuli ‘overwhelming’, while her brother compared his way of thinking during an episode to ‘a single-channel TV versus a 100-channel TV between episodes’(Katz and Ropper, 2002). In addition, a 16-year-old boy did not know how to eat a steak with cutlery (Rosenow et al., 2000), suggesting apraxia. Many patients reported amnesia of the events that occurred during an attack. Between episodes, 96.4% patients were described as totally normal. In a few cases (8/168), however, patients reported academic decline and a mild, long-lasting memory dysfunction between episodes (Fresco et al., 1971; Sagar et al., 1990; Masi et al., 2000). The possibility of residual dysfunction after KLS termination was also reported in three cases (Smolik and Roth, 1988; Landtblom et al., 2003).

Derealization, hallucination and delusion

A feeling of unreality (surroundings seemed wrong, distorted or unreal, as in a dream) or of disconnected thinking during episodes was reported by most patients and felt to be the most specific symptom of the syndrome (Table 2). Five patients discussing together agreed that it was the most important and disabling symptom (Landtblom et al., 2003). Altered perception was expressed qualitatively as feeling ‘strange’, ‘detached’ or ‘different’ (Thacore et al., 1969; George, 1970; Papacostas, 2003). Objects were perceived to be a long way off and voices to be distant—one patient's own voice appearing strange to himself (Kellett, 1977; Mayer et al., 1998; Masi et al., 2000)—with an ‘unpleasant perception, bizarre and wrong’, ‘with a nightmarish sense of the surroundings’ (Katz and Ropper, 2002) or ‘with the feeling of being almost in a dream’ (Green and Cracco, 1970; Mukaddes et al., 1999a). This feeling also included depersonalization, anguish, a belief of splitting between mind and body (Mukaddes et al., 1999a) and ‘a persistent sense of unreality and disconnection’ from the environment, like being ‘underwater’ (Katz and Ropper, 2002). Other more unusual changes included a blurred vision and the eyes being ‘dull’ or ‘glassy’ (Roth et al., 1980).

Aside from this feeling of unreality, some patients experienced visual or auditory hallucinations and paranoid or paranoiac delusions. Patients reported seeing ‘scary snakes on TV’ (Fresco et al., 1971); seeing ‘distorted faces, including Jesus’; seeing ‘the dead bodies of her parents’ (Duffy and Davison, 1968). Another girl felt that someone was trying to eat her, and that she was being filmed during the night (Overweg, 1971). One teenager was afraid of people trying to kill him, heard threatening voices (mostly at night), and took his razor to the nurse's desk to prevent people from killing him (Powers and Gunderman, 1978). Another patient believed his wife was trying to poison him and that neighbours would steal from him. He changed his lock five times and stopped eating for fear of being poisoned (Carpenter et al., 1982).

Eating behaviour disorders

Three quarters of the patients had changes in eating behaviours during episodes (Table 2). The majority typically ate larger amounts of food (megaphagia). Increased food intake ranged from a mild increase to ‘three times his usual diet’ (Shukla et al., 1982) or ‘6–8 meals a day’ (Hart, 1985) with a 7–30 lb (3.2–13.6 kg) weight gain. A patient was hospitalized for breathlessness caused by a distended abdomen, due to recent enormous meals (Prabhakaran et al., 1970). Increased drinking of water and juice was also occasionally present, but was never observed alone. A minority of patients (5%) had an aversion to food or ate less during one or several episodes, but would overeat during other episodes (Kellett, 1977; Manni et al., 1993; Portilla et al., 2002; Poppe et al., 2003). Several authors noted that the symptoms were distinct from bulimia, since patients never alternated with periods of self-induced vomiting and voluntary fasting. Food cravings and megaphagia were the most critical elements. Some patients stole food in shops or off the plates of other patients in the hospital (George, 1970; Prabhakaran et al., 1970; Rosenow et al., 2000), searched for food in dustbins (Prabhakaran et al., 1970) and stuffed food in their mouths with both hands (Duffy and Davison, 1968). Five other patients (4% of the group) had an inability to restrain themselves from eating in the presence of food, reminiscent of the general behaviour of utilization described in frontal syndrome. One patient ‘ate anything within reach’ (Hegarty and Merriam, 1990), and another was offered ‘multiple second servings shortly after he had eaten his regular meal, which he also finished’ (Elian, 1968). Two patients would ‘grab any food in sight’ (Sagar et al., 1990), while another would ‘eat mechanically, finishing whatever amount was given’ (Katz and Ropper, 2002). The food craving could be specifically directed toward sweets; patients added eight teaspoons of sugar to cereal (Chiles and Wilkus, 1976), ate ‘six bowls of desserts, six chocolate bars in a semi-automatic manner’ (Russell and Grunstein, 1992) or drank several bottles per day of pure chocolate or blackberry syrup (Garland et al., 1965; Haberland and Weissman, 1968). Interestingly, some patients would eat things they would have refused in the past, such as a Turkish girl who ate watermelon rinds (Mukaddes et al., 1999a) or a vegetarian Indian who ate non-vegetarian food (Shukla et al., 1982).

Mood disorders and irritability

Half of the patients had a depressive mood during episodes (Table 2). Fifteen percent of the patients reported suicidal thoughts and two patients attempted suicide (Gallinek, 1962; Vlach, 1962). In most cases, the depressed mood resolved at the end of each episode, although in rare cases it persisted longer. A few cases (8%) reported to be hypomanic for a couple of days at the end of a KLS episode (Reynolds et al., 1980; Goldberg, 1983). Another 8% had a flattened affect, and 7% were anxious, with two of them panicking when left alone.

Contrasting with the high frequency of siblings affected in patients with bipolar disorders, a familial history of severe depression was found in only six KLS patients, parental alcoholism in seven patients, and parental schizophrenia in only two patients. Irritability was present in almost all patients, especially when sleep, sexual or food drive were prohibited. It culminated in rare but severe aggressive behaviour. A child beat his grandmother (Powers and Gunderman, 1978), an adult patient beat his dog (Yassa and Nair, 1978), another child bit his father (Bouchard and Levasseur, 2001), one teenager spat in the face in his physician (Mukaddes et al., 1999b), one young man threw stones and was caught by the police (Prabhakaran et al., 1970), while another teenager had such a rage outburst at school that the police evacuated the classroom (Crumley, 1997).

Hypersexuality and other compulsive behaviours

Nearly half of the patients had symptoms consistent with hypersexuality during episodes. In males, these included increased/overt masturbation, exposing oneself, obscene language, fondling genitalia and making unwanted sexual advances. Inappropriate sexual advances included the assaulting of female nursing staff, female visitors, patient's sisters, daughter or other female relatives, and in three cases another man (Garland et al., 1965; Fresco et al., 1971; Yassa and Nair, 1978). Of interest, these symptoms were also reported, although more rarely, in women (Duffy and Davison, 1968; Hart, 1985; Kesler et al., 2000) and in three pre-pubescent children (Sagar et al., 1990; Salter and White, 1993; Pike and Stores, 1994). The plasma levels of sex hormones (testosterone, luteinizing hormone, follicle-stimulating hormone) were normal in 14 patients and mildly decreased in two patients. Interestingly, no increase in testosterone has been found in KLS patients during KLS episodes.

Other compulsions that occurred during the episodes included inappropriate and compulsive singing in eight patients (Garland et al., 1965; Chiles and Wilkus, 1976; Ferguson, 1986; Malhotra et al., 1997; Sadeghu, 1999; Muratori et al., 2002), body rocking (Green and Cracco, 1970; Papacostas and Hadjivasilis, 2000), chewing lips (Thacore et al., 1969), compulsive writing on walls (or on the sole of the patient's foot in two patients) and stripping down wallpaper in two other patients (Will et al., 1988; Mukaddes et al., 1999b), continuously switching lights on and off (Jensen, 1985), pacing, wringing hands and tearing out hair (Duffy and Davison, 1968) and the compulsion to set fire in one patient (Powers and Gunderman, 1978).

Medical examinations and tests

Clinical examination was unremarkable in all cases with primary KLS. In particular, the absence of neurological signs indicative of a focal lesion or of meningitis was notable. Signs of autonomic dysfunction were rare and included a flushed face (Russell and Grunstein, 1992), thermoregulatory changes (Smolik and Roth, 1988), hyperventilation (Fukunishi and Hosokawa, 1989), short episodes of flushes, profuse sweating, excessive salivation, hypertension and tachycardia (Hegarty and Merriam, 1990), hypotension and bradycardia (Koerber et al., 1984; Domzal-Stryga et al., 1986; Gillberg, 1987; Manni et al., 1993; Muratori et al., 2002). One patient died of cardio-respiratory arrest following an ataxic respiratory pattern. There was no evidence of neuronal damage in his hypothalamus.

The medical tests in KLS patients were mainly aimed at eliminating epilepsy (EEG), focal brain lesions (brain imaging), and meningitis or encephalitis (CSF analysis) as potential causes. Many, if not most, were conducted during episodes.

Cerebrospinal fluid analysis

CSF white cell counts and protein levels were normal in all patients, ruling out infectious meningitis. Immuno-electrophoresis of the CSF was performed and found to be normal in four patients. This excludes the possibility of frequent oligoclonal secretion of antibodies as observed in multiple sclerosis, another remittent neurological disease (Billard et al., 1978; Powers and Gunderman, 1978; Da Silveira Neto and Da Silveira, 1991; Pike and Stores, 1994). CSF levels of serotonin and a serotonin metabolite were increased (five times and twice the normal values, respectively) in one patient (Koerber et al., 1984), but not in four other patients, as were dopamine and norepinephrine metabolite levels (Carpenter et al., 1982; Hart, 1985; Hasegawa et al., 1998; Landtblom et al., 2002). The CSF levels of hypocretin-1, a hypothalamic peptide that has been shown to be deficient in narcolepsy, were found within normal ranges in five KLS patients but slightly decreased (111 and 137 pmol·l−1) in two patients during an episode (Katz and Ropper, 2002; Mignot et al., 2002; Dauvilliers et al., 2003).

Electroencephalograms and brain imaging

One fourth of the patients had a normal EEG during episodes. In 70% of the patients, a non-specific diffuse slowing of background EEG activity, such as the alpha frequency band being slowed toward 7–8 Hz, was observed. Less often, low frequency high amplitude waves (delta or theta) occurred in isolation or in sequence, mainly in the bilateral temporal or temporofrontal areas. A remarkable finding was the ubiquitous absence of epileptic activity; an intra-cerebral sphenoid electrode was even recorded in a few patients. Rarely, isolated spike discharges (Elian, 1968), self-limited photo-paroxysmal response (Papacostas, 2003) or sharp waves (Malhotra et al., 1997) were observed, but were considered of no clinical significance.

Brain computerized tomography and magnetic resonance imaging were normal in all cases. Functional imaging measuring cerebral blood flow by single photoemission tomography was performed in nine patients aged 13–27 years. Cerebral blood flow was normal in four patients and reduced in five patients. The reduction occurred in the temporal or temporofrontal areas of either or both sides (Yassa and Nair, 1978; Argentino and Sideri, 1980; Lu et al., 2000; Arias et al., 2002; Landtblom et al., 2002, 2003; Portilla et al., 2002) and in the basal ganglia (Lu et al., 2000). Brain neuropathological examinations (Table 3) were performed after the death of two patients with primary KLS (Carpenter et al., 1982; Koerber et al., 1984) and in two patients with secondary KLS. The cortex was intact in all but one patient (a patient with paraneoplastic syndrome). There were intense signs of inflammatory encephalitis within the hypothalamus in two patients, mild inflammation in one patient and none in the last patient.

Table 3

Neuropathological findings in 4 cases with Kleine–Levin syndrome (KLS)

 Primary KLS
 
 Secondary KLS
 
 

 
Carpenter et al., 1982
 
Koerber et al., 1984
 
Takrani et al., 1976
 
Fenzi et al., 1993
 
Patient Male, 46 years Male, 17 years Female, 50 years Female, 6 years 
Typical signs Hypersomnia, megaphagia, sexual disinhibition, seven attacks Hypersomnia, megaphagia, masturbation, several attacks Hypersomnia, megaphagia, aggressivity, four attacks Hypersomnia, megaphagia, agitation, two attacks 
Atypical signs Late onset, some attacks lasted 3 months Autonomic dysfunction, muscle weakness Late onset, Uterine carcinoma Upward-gaze palsy, mild ptosis 
Cause of death Aspiration pneumonia (due to megaphagia) Cardiopulmonary arrest Complications of cancer Pulmonary embolism after a bone fracture when agitated during an episode 
Lesions     
    Cortex Normal Normal Perivascular temporal infiltrate Normal 
    Amygdala Normal Normal Perivascular infiltrate Normal 
    Thalamus (medial and intralaminar) Major new and old lesions of the thalamus: abundant infiltrates of inflammatory cells, with microglial proliferation. Cuffing of veins with monocytes and lymphocytes Normal Normal Perivascular lympho-monocyte infiltrate in the thalamus: scattered foci of cellular infiltrates with parenchymal nodular-microglial proliferation 
    Hypothalamus Very mild proliferation of subependymal astrocytes on the third ventricle wall, small amounts of lymphocytic cuffing in the lateral hypothalamus Normal Perivascular infiltrate Perivascular lympho-monocyte infiltrate in the hypothalamus, and floor of the third ventricle 
    Brainstem Normal Mildly depigmented substantia nigra and locus coeruleus, no Lewy bodies or tangles  Microglial nodule in the peri-aqueductal grey region and in the oculomotor nerve nuclei 
 Primary KLS
 
 Secondary KLS
 
 

 
Carpenter et al., 1982
 
Koerber et al., 1984
 
Takrani et al., 1976
 
Fenzi et al., 1993
 
Patient Male, 46 years Male, 17 years Female, 50 years Female, 6 years 
Typical signs Hypersomnia, megaphagia, sexual disinhibition, seven attacks Hypersomnia, megaphagia, masturbation, several attacks Hypersomnia, megaphagia, aggressivity, four attacks Hypersomnia, megaphagia, agitation, two attacks 
Atypical signs Late onset, some attacks lasted 3 months Autonomic dysfunction, muscle weakness Late onset, Uterine carcinoma Upward-gaze palsy, mild ptosis 
Cause of death Aspiration pneumonia (due to megaphagia) Cardiopulmonary arrest Complications of cancer Pulmonary embolism after a bone fracture when agitated during an episode 
Lesions     
    Cortex Normal Normal Perivascular temporal infiltrate Normal 
    Amygdala Normal Normal Perivascular infiltrate Normal 
    Thalamus (medial and intralaminar) Major new and old lesions of the thalamus: abundant infiltrates of inflammatory cells, with microglial proliferation. Cuffing of veins with monocytes and lymphocytes Normal Normal Perivascular lympho-monocyte infiltrate in the thalamus: scattered foci of cellular infiltrates with parenchymal nodular-microglial proliferation 
    Hypothalamus Very mild proliferation of subependymal astrocytes on the third ventricle wall, small amounts of lymphocytic cuffing in the lateral hypothalamus Normal Perivascular infiltrate Perivascular lympho-monocyte infiltrate in the hypothalamus, and floor of the third ventricle 
    Brainstem Normal Mildly depigmented substantia nigra and locus coeruleus, no Lewy bodies or tangles  Microglial nodule in the peri-aqueductal grey region and in the oculomotor nerve nuclei 

Hormonal tests

Changes in levels of pituitary hormones were only rarely found in KLS patients. Hormonal measurements were performed during episodes in 45 patients. The pituitary axis was considered ‘normal’ without published details in seven cases. The plasma levels of thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH, 21 patients), cortisol at 8 a.m. and 4 p.m. (20 patients) and adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH, 4 patients) were always normal, while those of growth hormone (GH) were either normal (10 of 12 patients), increased (1 of 12 patients, Rosenow et al., 2000) or decreased (1 of 12 patients, Chesson et al., 1991). The diurnal profiles of secretion of GH, melatonin, TSH and cortisol were unchanged during and after episodes in five of five patients (Mayer et al., 1998) suggesting that circadian systems were basically intact. The dynamic testing of hypothalamic functioning was rarely done (three patients) and yielded inconsistent results. The TSH response to thyroid-releasing hormone (TRH) and cortisol and ACTH responses to hypoglycaemic stimulation were abolished (Fernandez et al., 1990) or blunted (Malhotra et al., 1997) during an episode and normalized thereafter in two patients. A patient had a paradoxical GH response to TRH (Gadoth et al., 1987), while another had a normal GH in response to hypoglycaemia (Malhotra et al., 1997).

Therapeutic attempts

In 75 patients, one or several drug therapies were attempted, constituting a total of 213 open-labelled trials (Table 4). The results obtained with these therapies were compared to natural evolution, as reported in a group of 26 patients who did not receive drug treatment. Among stimulants, only amphetamines significantly reduced sleepiness in patients. Importantly, however, it was noted they did not improve the more troublesome behavioural and cognitive disturbances (Gallinek, 1962). Other less potent stimulants were rarely beneficial or increased their hypersexuality. In two patients, flumazenil, a benzodiazepine receptor antagonist failed to elicit wakefulness. Neuroleptics (chlorpromazine, levomepromazine, trifluoperazine, haloperidol, thioridazine, clozapine and risperidone) were notably ineffective against derealization, psychotic and behavioural symptoms.

Table 4

Treatments used in patients with Kleine–Levin syndrome and reported effects

Treatment
 
No. patients
 
No effect
 
Moderate benefit
 
Clear benefit
 
Response rate (%)
 
One relapse then ended
 
No more relapses
 
Treatments of symptoms during an episode        
    Stimulants (reduction of hypersomnia) 40 19 16 40   
    Modafinil   
    Methylphenidate 15 20   
    Pemoline-piracetam-meclofenoxate 25   
    Amphetamines 17 12 71*   
    Flumazepil   
    Neuroleptics (derealization, behavioural disorders) 28 25 11   
Treatments aimed at preventing relapses        
    No drug treatment 32 16   16 11 
    Phototherapy   
    Antidepressants 23 21   
Mood stabilizers        
    Lithium 29   41* 12 
    Carbamazepine 19 14   21 
    Valproate, phenobarbital, phenytoin 10   20 
Various        
    Antiviral (i.v. acyclovir)   
    Melatonin   
    Benzodiazepines   
    Levodopa + benserazide   
    Electroconvulsive therapy   
Treatment
 
No. patients
 
No effect
 
Moderate benefit
 
Clear benefit
 
Response rate (%)
 
One relapse then ended
 
No more relapses
 
Treatments of symptoms during an episode        
    Stimulants (reduction of hypersomnia) 40 19 16 40   
    Modafinil   
    Methylphenidate 15 20   
    Pemoline-piracetam-meclofenoxate 25   
    Amphetamines 17 12 71*   
    Flumazepil   
    Neuroleptics (derealization, behavioural disorders) 28 25 11   
Treatments aimed at preventing relapses        
    No drug treatment 32 16   16 11 
    Phototherapy   
    Antidepressants 23 21   
Mood stabilizers        
    Lithium 29   41* 12 
    Carbamazepine 19 14   21 
    Valproate, phenobarbital, phenytoin 10   20 
Various        
    Antiviral (i.v. acyclovir)   
    Melatonin   
    Benzodiazepines   
    Levodopa + benserazide   
    Electroconvulsive therapy   
*

P < 0.05 for a difference with no treatment, chi-square.

Numerous antidepressants including tricyclics (imipramine, clomipramine, amineptine) and serotonin-acting drugs (fluoxetine, fluvoxamine, sertraline, methylsergide, trazodone) had no effect on preventing relapses, except for one isolated reported case of recovery with the use of the monoamine oxidase inhibitor moclobemide (Chaudhry, 1992). Electroconvulsive therapy, ranging from 7 to 47 shocks (Gallinek, 1962; Vlach, 1962; Duffy and Davison, 1968; Chiles and Wilkus, 1976; Yassa and Nair, 1978), and insulin coma therapy (Savet et al., 1986) had no effect on KLS symptoms (and even worsened confusion in the case of electroconvulsive therapy).

Various mood stabilizers, such as lithium and antiepileptic drugs were tried (Table 4). Only lithium had a reported response rate significantly higher than medical abstention (odds-ratio = 3.8, P = 0.02). Lithium was not a last chance treatment, given after other trials and thereby potentially tried closer to the natural end of the disease, as the option rank (i.e. if lithium was tried as first, second, third or fourth therapeutic option) and the number of episodes that preceded the lithium trial did not influence the reported response rate (P = 0.22 for option rank, P = 0.95 for pre-lithium number of episodes). In addition, recovery was imputable to lithium in three cases, with KLS episodes stopping when the drug was introduced, KLS relapsing soon after stopping the drug, and recovering again when lithium was re-introduced (Kellett, 1977; Smolik and Roth, 1988; Poppe et al., 2003). Such a pattern was observed with carbamazepine in a single case (Mukaddes et al., 1999b).

Risk factors for longer KLS course

Women had a longer disease course than men (9 ± 8.7 years versus 5.4 ± 5.6 years, P = 0.01), despite a comparable age at KLS onset (17.6 ± 8.9 years versus 16.6 ± 8.3 years) and an absence of differences in the duration of episodes and symptoms-free intervals. Women had the same frequency of megaphagia and psychotic symptoms, but a lower frequency of hypersexuality (24 versus 51%, P = 0.002) and cognitive impairment (80 versus 98%, P = 0.004).

Patients with a high number of episodes during the first year of KLS had a somewhat shorter KLS disease duration (r = −0.23, P = 0.005, n = 65 patients). In contrast, the age at KLS onset, the presence of megaphagia, cognitive disturbances, psychotic signs and hypersexuality did not influence the course of the disease. Most notably, 39 patients with ‘full-blown’ KLS (suffering from hypersomnia, cognitive disturbances, eating disorders and hypersexuality) did not have a different disease duration (6.4 ± 6.6 years) when compared to 63 patients with ‘incomplete’ KLS (6.3 ± 6.9 years, P = 0.97).

Cases with secondary KLS

Causes

In 18 patients, KLS-like symptoms were observed in association with stroke or post-traumatic brain haematoma (n = 5), genetic or developmental diseases (n = 6), multiple sclerosis (Testa et al., 1987), hydrocephalus (Lobzin et al., 1973), paraneoplasia in the context of a carcinoma of the cervix utero (Takrani and Cronin, 1976), an autoimmune encephalitis (Fenzi et al., 1993) or a severe infectious encephalitis (n = 3). The types of stroke reported were a multi-infarct dementia (Drake, 1987), a thalamic ischaemic stroke (McGilchrist et al., 1993) and traumatic haemorrhages of the right hemisphere (Chiu et al., 1989; Kostic et al., 1998; Pelin et al., 2004). The genetic diseases were heterogeneous and included a case of mosaicism with Robert's syndrome, phocomelia, mild mental retardation, optic atrophy, bilateral facial palsy (Hasegawa et al., 1998), a case with Prader–Willi syndrome (Gau et al., 1996), an unidentified disease with mental retardation and bilateral pyramidal syndrome (Livrea et al., 1977), another complex case of consanguinity, mental retardation, an ectodermal disorder (incontinentia pigmenti), acanthosis nigricans, and hereditary exostosis (Reimao and Shimizu, 1998) and developmental Asperger's disease in two patients, one with cortical dysplasia and retinitis pigmentosa (Berthier et al., 1992). As for the three patients with infectious encephalitis of unknown origin, one had an acute viral meningo-encephalitis and high CSF lymphocyte counts (Merriam, 1986) while another had a meningo-encephalitis with neurological sequels, including left hypo-sensitivity, central facial palsy, concentric loss of visual fields and bilateral facial spasms (Persson et al., 1969). KLS also occurred in a context of gastrointestinal symptoms in the 1930's in a woman with recurrent episodes of variable hypersomnia, sometimes severe insomnia, and diarrhoea lasting over a 30-year period (Wilder, 1972). The author discussed a possible encephalitis lethargica, while we would also suspect Whipple's disease.

Symptoms in secondary KLS

Compared to patients with primary KLS, the symptoms occurred significantly later in patients with secondary KLS (Table 5). They also experienced three times the number of episodes, which lasted three times longer and thus the time they were incapacitated was dramatically increased. The disease did not, however, last longer, and the cardinal signs (hypersomnia, megaphagia, cognitive disturbances, hallucinations and behaviour disorders) occurred with a similar frequency. Symptoms were often described in similar terms, for example, ‘eating compulsively, without complaints of hunger or expression of satiety’ (Drake, 1987) and the greedy consumption of ‘0.5 kg of biscuits, six tarts and several ice-creams’ (Chiu et al., 1989). A teenager saw ghosts and famous TV actors and believed he was being pursued by armed attackers (Merriam, 1986). Compulsions were also observed, such as nail-biting, hair pulling, scratching skin, laughing and crying, walking along straight lines (Wilder, 1972) and writing on clothes and extremities (Gau et al., 1996).

Table 5

Differences between patients with primary and secondary Kleine–Levin syndrome


 
Primary KLS
 
Secondary KLS
 
P
 
No. of patients 168 18  
Sex-ratio (% men) 69% 67% 0.83 
Age of onset, years 18.9 ± 72.3 26.1 ± 17.5* 0.0002 
Disease course, years median ±SE 8 ± 2 10 ± 2 0.24 
Episode duration, days 11.7 ± 8.9 31.4 ± 56.5* 0.0001 
Interval duration, months 5.9 ± 9.6 6.8 ± 9.3 0.73 
Number of episodes 11.9 ± 14.5 38.3 ± 72.6* 0.0005 
Time incapacitated, days 135.5 ± 168.5 673.5 ± 1245* <0.0001 
Megaphagia, % patients 80% 83% 0.95 
Hallucinations, delusions, % 26% 44% 0.10 
Hypersexuality, % 43% 28% 0.2 

 
Primary KLS
 
Secondary KLS
 
P
 
No. of patients 168 18  
Sex-ratio (% men) 69% 67% 0.83 
Age of onset, years 18.9 ± 72.3 26.1 ± 17.5* 0.0002 
Disease course, years median ±SE 8 ± 2 10 ± 2 0.24 
Episode duration, days 11.7 ± 8.9 31.4 ± 56.5* 0.0001 
Interval duration, months 5.9 ± 9.6 6.8 ± 9.3 0.73 
Number of episodes 11.9 ± 14.5 38.3 ± 72.6* 0.0005 
Time incapacitated, days 135.5 ± 168.5 673.5 ± 1245* <0.0001 
Megaphagia, % patients 80% 83% 0.95 
Hallucinations, delusions, % 26% 44% 0.10 
Hypersexuality, % 43% 28% 0.2 

Data are mean ± SD, unless specified.

*

Significant difference with primary KLS, Student's t-test, except for disease course (Log rank test in Kaplan–Meier analysis), and for % of symptoms (chi-square test).

The neurological signs that were observed between the episodes were various, with very few commonalities between patients. They included objective sensory disturbances of the extremities (Wilder, 1972); impaired verbal abilities (Takrani and Cronin, 1976); bilateral pyramidal signs and mental retardation (Livrea et al., 1977); frontal, pseudo-bulbar and pyramidal syndromes (Drake, 1987); left hemiplegia (Chiu et al., 1989); parkinsonism (Berthier et al., 1992); central facial palsy (McGilchrist et al., 1993); upward-gaze palsy with mild ptosis (Fenzi et al., 1993); and mental retardation (Gau et al., 1996; Hasegawa et al., 1998). Sleep recordings were performed in half of these patients and yielded the same abnormalities as in primary cases, including short REM sleep latency in two cases (Drake, 1987; Berthier et al., 1992), hypersomnia of the harmonious type, with proportional increase of all sleep stages (Merriam, 1986) or with excess of REM sleep (Hasegawa et al., 1998) and decreased sleep efficiency (Berthier et al., 1992).

Pharmacological therapy (18 trials) was initiated in 8 of 18 patients. As in primary cases, antidepressants (3 trials), neuroleptics (2 trials) and a sedative (1 trial) had no effect, while carbamazepine and lithium were associated with a reduction (but not an ending) in the number of attacks in 1 of 3 patients and 3 of 4 patients, respectively.

Discussion

This systematic review reports on the largest number of KLS patients ever presented, with inclusion of all non-English language articles. The striking commonality of symptoms across patients is again demonstrated, suggesting a unique disease entity. Episodic hypersomnia and cognitive disturbances may constitute the core abnormality, while behavioural, eating and sexual disturbances are more variable and may occur only in a subset of episodes even within single patients. Age of onset, sex-ratio, triggering factors and frequency of symptoms are similar to two recent small case series reported in Europe by Dauvilliers et al. (2002) and Israel by Gadoth et al. (2001). The analysis shows, for the first time, a worldwide distribution for KLS. Of note, one-sixth of the patients reported were Israeli, suggesting either a publication bias or a higher vulnerability in subjects with Jewish heritage. Follow-up prospective studies will be needed to shed light on potential ethnic differences.

Triggering factors

The occurrence of an infection at the disease onset in more than two-thirds of the patients, already stressed by some authors, seems too frequent and too closely associated for KLS to be due to chance. Unfortunately, however, in rare cases where an infectious agent was identified, it differed from one patient to another. These agents may thus decompensate a previously existing disease, or a coexisting infection with another yet undetected infectious agent may be responsible. Additional work in this area is needed as the studies of some notable infectious agents known to cause confusion, hypersomnia and various neuro-psychiatric symptoms such as Whipple's disease, malaria, California encephalitis or encephalitis lethargica-like agents (Dale et al., 2004), have not been explored as possible aetiological agents. Infection, head trauma and alcohol are all known to increase the blood–brain barrier permeability (Rapoport et al., 1971; Lo et al., 2001; Nassif et al., 2002) and could therefore facilitate the passage of a circulating pathogenic agent or immunoglobulin to the brain.

Duration of the disease

This study is also the first to report on the median duration of the disease, a relatively longer than expected 4–8 years, which is an important variable to report to patients first presenting with the disease. Four years was a lower estimate based on patients with disease terminated at time of publication (other subjects with longer course duration are more likely not to be published), while 8 years may be slightly overestimated if patients lost in follow-up are generally cured. One long-term follow-up study via phone or in-person interviews reported that 25 patients were in good health several years after the cessation of their KLS episodes, suggesting that complete recovery and a good prognosis is the rule for KLS (Gadoth et al., 2001). They did not, however, report on the final duration of the disease. In <4% of cases, a permanent cognitive decline was observed, though it was difficult to causally associate it to KLS, missed school days, or the sedative side-effects of treatments. If confirmed in a series with long-term follow-up, it would raise the idea that some rare patients may present permanent lesions, which would support early aggressive medical intervention (Landtblom et al., 2003).

Secondary KLS cases

The older age in secondary KLS is probably due to the presence of stroke as a cause in five patients, a disease that affects preferentially older subjects. Since symptoms are similar in nature and frequency to those with primary KLS, as are responses to treatment and disease duration, one can hypothesize that secondary cases are not actually ‘secondary’, but rather primary cases incidentally coexisting with genetic, vascular or inflammatory neural lesions. These lesions would only facilitate KLS, causing longer and more frequent episodes.

Treatment

The evaluation of treatment, based on case-reports, was hard to assess because of the unpredictable spontaneous course of the disease and of the absence of placebo-controlled studies. It was evaluated in a large population of patients, and, as generally predicted, results were extremely disappointing. Amphetamine-stimulants significantly improved sleepiness (Table 4) but not the other more serious symptoms, suggesting a very imperfect therapeutic relief. The potential benefit of lithium at preventing relapses (Table 4), only administered in 29 cases, if confirmed, should be balanced against its known difficulty of use and unfavourable side-effect profile. We also noted that antiepileptic mood stabilizers (especially carbamazepine) were commonly prescribed, probably based on the possible efficacy of lithium, but results were similar to no drug treatment, strongly suggesting that this practice has no justification. Antidepressant therapies were similarly ineffective. We believe that additional therapeutic trials using other medications, such as immunosuppressive or novel antiviral agents, with double-blind placebo-controlled multicentre design, are warranted.

Limitations of the study

There are several limitations to our study that are inherent to any systematic review. Only published case-reports are considered, and, even if KLS cases are more likely to be published when compared to other diseases because of the rarity of the syndrome and its striking symptoms, the published cases may not be representative of the general KLS population. A likely bias may be a trend towards publishing details of patients with a more complete series of symptoms (hypersomnia, cognitive changes, megaphagia and sexual disinhibition) and to report more severe and/or unusual cases, rather than subjects with just recurrent isolated hypersomnia. This could lead to a seemingly apparent homogeneity of the published disease. Importantly, however, it is an accepted fact that even recurrent hypersomnia without ancillary symptoms is a rare occurrence in clinical practice. The second limitation pertains to data available in published studies. The rate of information we found in articles was unusually high for symptom occurrence and disease course, but was as low as 27% for selected data such as hormonal levels or sleep studies, reducing the validity of these last findings. For the same reason, it is possible that the apparent success of any treatment will be more often published than a failure to respond.

Potential pathophysiological mechanisms

This study also highlights possible pathophysiological mechanisms for the disorder. Partial complex epilepsy, a condition that can produce episodes of recurrent abnormal behaviour, can be ruled out. The poor response of the condition to antiepileptic therapy also substantiates this hypothesis. A local brain lesion is also unlikely, considering the polymorphism of the symptomatology. Finally, EEG, brain flow SPECT, or neuropathological data showed that frontal, temporal and sometimes occipital and parietal lobes can be involved, not to mention the thalamus (Huang et al., 2005).

The finding of a possible Jewish predisposition, occasional familial clustering, and the association with infectious triggering factors suggest that KLS is due to environmental factors acting on a vulnerable genetic background. This general picture and the fluctuating symptomatology in KLS are consistent with the recent report of an HLA association in KLS and the possibility of an autoimmune mediation of the disorder.

References

American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders—Revised. Hauri P Ed, Chicago, IL
2005
, pp 297.
Argentino C, Sideri G. Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Riv Neurol
 
1980
;
50
:
26
–31.
Arias M, Crespo Iglesias JM, Perez J, Requena-Caballero I, Sesar-Ignacio A, Peleteiro-Fernandez M. Kleine–Levin syndrome: contribution of brain SPECT in diagnosis.
Rev Neurol
 
2002
;
35
:
531
–3.
Badino R, Caja A, Del Conte I, Guida C, Ivaldi M. Kleine–Levin syndrome in an 82 year old man.
Ital J Neurol Sci
 
1992
;
13
:
355
–6.
Berthier ML, Santamaria J, Encabo H, Tolosa ES. Recurrent hypersomnia in two adolescent males with Asperger's syndrome.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
 
1992
;
31
:
735
–8.
Billiard M, Carlander B. Wake disorders. I. Primary wake disorders.
Rev Neurol (Paris)
 
1998
;
154
:
111
–29.
Billiard M, Guilleminault C, Dement WC. A menstruation-linked periodic hypersomnia. Kleine–Levin syndrome or new clinical entity?
Neurology
 
1975
;
25
:
436
–43.
Billard C, Ponsot G, Lyon G, Arfel G. Kleine–Levin syndrome. A propos of a case.
Arch Fr Pediatr
 
1978
;
35
:
424
–31.
Bouchard C, Levasseur M. Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Rev Neurol (Paris)
 
2001
;
157
:
344
–5.
Brierre de Boismont A. Des hallucinations. Third edition. Germer Baillère Ed, Paris 1862. pp 339–40.
Cante C, Marocchino R. Kleine–Levin syndrome: observations during a course of hypothymic episodes.
Osp Psichiatr
 
1970
;
38
:
603
–13.
Carpenter S, Yassa R, Ochs R. A pathologic basis for Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Arch Neurol
 
1982
;
39
:
25
–8.
Chaudhry HR. Clinical use of moclobemide in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1992
;
161
:
720
.
Chesson AL Jr, Levine SN, Kong LS, Lee SC. Neuroendocrine evaluation in Kleine–Levin syndrome: evidence of reduced dopaminergic tone during periods of hypersomnolence.
Sleep
 
1991
;
14
:
226
–32.
Chiles JA, Wilkus RJ. behavioural manifestations of the Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Dis Nerv Syst
 
1976
;
37
:
646
–8.
Chiu HF, Li SW, Lee S. Kleine–Levin syndrome 15 years later.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry
 
1989
;
23
:
425
–7.
Critchley M, Hoffman H. The syndrome of periodic somnolence and morbic hunger (Kleine–Levin syndrome).
BMJ
 
1942
;
1
:
137
–9.
Critchley M. Periodic hypersomnia and megaphagia in adolescent males.
Brain
 
1962
;
85
:
627
–56.
Crumley FE. Valproic acid for Kleine–Levin syndrome.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
 
1997
;
36
:
868
–9.
Cuetter AC. Sleep apnea and the Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Mil Med
 
1985
;
150
:
286
–8.
Da Silveira Neto O, Da Silveira OA. Kleine–Levin syndrome. Report of a case.
Arq Neuropsiquiatr
 
1991
;
49
:
330
–2.
Dale RC, Church AJ, Surtees RA, Lees AJ, Adcock JE, Harding B, et al. Encephalitis lethargica syndrome: 20 new cases and evidence of basal ganglia autoimmunity.
Brain
 
2004
;
127
:
21
–33.
Dauvilliers Y, Mayer G, Lecendreux M, Neidhart E, Peraita-Adrados R, Sonka K, et al. Kleine–Levin syndrome: an autoimmune hypothesis based on clinical and genetic analyses.
Neurology
 
2002
;
59
:
1739
–45.
Dauvilliers Y, Baumann CR, Carlander B, Bischof M, Blatter T, Lecendreux M, et al. CSF hypocretin-1 levels in narcolepsy, Kleine–Levin syndrome, and other hypersomnias and neurological conditions.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
 
2003
;
74
:
1667
–73.
Domzal-Stryga A, Emeryk-Szajewska B, Kowalski J. A case of hypersomnia resembling Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Neurol Neurochir Pol
 
1986
;
20
:
158
–60.
Drake ME Jr. Kleine–Levin syndrome after multiple cerebral infarctions.
Psychosomatics
 
1987
;
28
:
329
–30.
Duffy JP, Davison K. A female case of the Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1968
;
114
:
77
–84.
Elian M. Periodic hypersomnia.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
 
1968
;
24
:
192
–3.
Fenzi F, Simonati A, Crosato F, Ghersini L, Rizzuto N. Clinical features of Kleine–Levin syndrome with localized encephalitis.
Neuropediatrics
 
1993
;
24
:
292
–5.
Ferguson BG. Kleine–Levin syndrome: a case report.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
 
1986
;
27
:
275
–8.
Fernandez JM, Lara I, Gila L, O'Neill of Tyrone A, Tovar J, Gimeno A. Disturbed hypothalamic-pituitary axis in idiopathic recurring hypersomnia syndrome.
Acta Neurol Scand
 
1990
;
82
:
361
–3.
Frank Y, Braham J, Cohen BE. The Kleine–Levin syndrome. Case report and review of the literature.
Am J Dis Child
 
1974
;
127
:
412
–3.
Fresco R, Giudicelli S, Poinso Y, Tatossian A, Mouren P. Kleine–Levin syndrome—recurrent hypersomnia of male adolescents.
Ann Med Psychol (Paris)
 
1971
;
1
:
625
–68.
Frösher W, Maier V, Fritschni T. Periodic hypersomnia: case-report with biochemical and EEG findings.
Sleep
 
1991
;
14
:
460
–3.
Fukunishi I, Hosokawa K. A female case with the Kleine–Levin syndrome and its physiopathologic aspects.
Jpn J Psychiatry Neurol
 
1989
;
43
:
45
–9.
Gadoth N, Dickerman Z, Bechar M, Laron Z, Lavie P. Episodic hormone secretion during sleep in Kleine–Levin syndrome: evidence for hypothalamic dysfunction.
Brain Dev
 
1987
;
9
:
309
–15.
Gadoth N, Kesler A, Vainstein G, Peled R, Lavie P. Clinical and polysomnographic characteristics of 34 patients with Kleine–Levin syndrome.
J Sleep Res
 
2001
;
10
:
337
–41.
Gallinek A. The Kleine–Levin syndrome: hypersomnia, bulimia, and abnormal mental states.
World Neurol
 
1962
;
3
:
235
–43.
Gallinek A. The Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Dis Nerv Syst
 
1967
;
28
:
448
–51.
Garland H, Sumner D, Fourman P. The Kleine–Levin syndrome. Some further observations.
Neurology
 
1965
;
15
:
1161
–7.
Gau SF, Soong WT, Liu HM, Hou JW, Tsai WY, Chiu YN, et al. Kleine–Levin syndrome in a boy with Prader-Willi syndrome.
Sleep
 
1996
;
19
:
13
–7.
George HR. A case of the Kleine–Levin syndrome of long duration.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1970
;
117
:
521
–3.
Gilbert GJ. Periodic hypersomnia and bulimia. The Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Neurology
 
1964
;
14
:
844
–50.
Gillberg C. Kleine–Levin syndrome: unrecognized diagnosis in adolescent psychiatry.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
 
1987
;
26
:
793
–4.
Goldberg MA. The treatment of Kleine–Levin syndrome with lithium.
Can J Psychiatry
 
1983
;
28
:
491
–3.
Green LN, Cracco RQ. Kleine–Levin syndrome. A case with EEG evidence of periodic brain dysfunction.
Arch Neurol
 
1970
;
22
:
166
–75.
Haberland C, Weissman S. The Kleine–Levin syndrome. A case study with a psychopathologic approach.
Acta Psychiatr Scand
 
1968
;
44
:
1
–10.
Hart EJ. Kleine–Levin syndrome: normal CSF monoamines and response to lithium therapy.
Neurology
 
1985
;
35
:
1395
–6.
Hasegawa Y, Morishita M, Suzumura A. Novel chromosomal abberation in a patient with a unique sleep disorder.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
 
1998
;
64
:
113
–6.
Hegarty A, Merriam AE. Autonomic events in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Am J Psychiatry
 
1990
;
147
:
951
–2.
Huang YS, Guilleminault C, Kao PF, Liu FY. SPECT findings in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Sleep
 
2005
;
28
:
955
–60.
Iakhno NN. Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Sov Med
 
1980
;
109
–11.
Janicki S, Franco K, Zarko R. A case report of Kleine–Levin syndrome in an adolescent girl.
Psychosomatics
 
2001
;
42
:
350
–2.
Jensen J. The Kleine–Levin syndrome. Periodic hypersomnia and hyperphagia with abnormal behaviour.
Ugeskr Laeger
 
1985
;
147
:
709
–10.
Katz JD, Ropper AH. Familial Kleine–Levin syndrome: two siblings with unusually long hypersomnic spells.
Arch Neurol
 
2002
;
59
:
1959
–61.
Kellett J. Lithium prophylaxis of periodic hypersomnia.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1977
;
130
:
312
–6.
Kesler A, Gadoth N, Vainstein G, Peled R, Lavie P. Kleine–Levin syndrome (KLS) in young females.
Sleep
 
2000
;
23
:
563
–7.
Kleine W. Periodische schlafsucht.
Monats Psychiatr Neurol
 
1925
;
57
:
285
–320.
Koerber RK, Torkelson R, Haven G, Donaldson J, Cohen SM, Case M. Increased cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxytryptamine and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Neurology
 
1984
;
34
:
1597
–600.
Kostic VS, Stefanova E, Svetel M, Kozic D. A variant of the Kleine–Levin syndrome following head trauma.
Behav Neurol
 
1998
;
11
:
105
–8.
Lamote de Grignon C, Fernandez Alvarez E. Contribution to the study of the Kleine–Levin syndrome in early childhood.
Rev Neuropsychiatr Infant
 
1967
;
15
:
365
–72.
Landtblom AM, Dige N, Schwerdt K, Safstrom P, Granerus G. A case of Kleine–Levin syndrome examined with SPECT and neuropsychological testing.
Acta Neurol Scand
 
2002
;
105
:
318
–21.
Landtblom AM, Dige N, Schwerdt K, Safstrom P, Granerus G. Short-term memory dysfunction in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Acta Neurol Scand
 
2003
;
108
:
363
–7.
Lavie P, Gadoth N, Gordon CR, Goldhammer G, Bechar M. Sleep patterns in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
 
1979
;
47
:
369
–71.
Lavie P, Klein E, Gadoth N, Bental E, Zomer J, Bechar M, et al. Further observations on sleep abnormalities in Kleine–Levin syndrome: abnormal breathing pattern during sleep.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
 
1981
;
52
:
98
–101.
Lemire I. Review of Kleine–Levin syndrome: toward an integrated approach.
Can J Psychiatry
 
1993
;
38
:
277
–84.
Lenz H. Kleine–Levin-syndrome.
Wien Med Wochenschr
 
1980
;
130
:
373
–5.
Levin M. Narcolepsy (Gelineau's syndrome) and other varieties of morbid somnolence.
Arch Neurol Psychiat
 
1929
;
22
:
1172
–200.
Levin M. Periodic somnolence and morbid hunger: a new syndrome.
Brain
 
1936
;
59
:
494
–504.
Livrea P, Puca F, Barnaba A, Di Reda L. Abnormal central monoamine metabolism in humans with ‘true hypersomnia’ and ‘subawkefulness’.
Eur Neurol
 
1977
;
15
:
71
–6.
Lo EH, Singhal AB, Torchilin VP, Abbott NJ. Drug delivery to damaged brain.
Brain Res Brain Res Rev
 
2001
;
38
:
140
–8.
Lobzin VS, Shamrei RK, Churilov Iu K. Pathophysiologic mechanisms of periodic sleep and the Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Zh Nevropatol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova
 
1973
;
73
:
1719
–24.
Lu ML, Liu HC, Chen CH, Sung SM. Kleine–Levin syndrome and psychosis: observation from an unusual case.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol
 
2000
;
13
:
140
–2.
Malhotra S, Das MK, Gupta N, Muralidharan R. A clinical study of Kleine–Levin syndrome with evidence for hypothalamic-pituitary axis dysfunction.
Biol Psychiatry
 
1997
;
42
:
299
–301.
Manni R, Martinetti M, Ratti MT, Tartara A. Electrophysiological and immunogenetic findings in recurrent monosymptomatic-type hypersomnia: a study of two unrelated Italian cases.
Acta Neurol Scand
 
1993
;
88
:
293
–5.
Markman RA. Kleine–Levin syndrome: report of a case.
Am J Psychiatry
 
1967
;
123
:
1025
–6.
Masi G, Favilla L, Millepiedi S. The Kleine–Levin syndrome as a neuropsychiatric disorder: a case report.
Psychiatry
 
2000
;
63
:
93
–100.
Mayer G, Leonhard E, Krieg J, Meier-Ewert K. Endocrinological and polysomnographic findings in Kleine–Levin syndrome: no evidence for hypothalamic and circadian dysfunction.
Sleep
 
1998
;
21
:
278
–84.
McGilchrist I, Goldstein LH, Jadresic D, Fenwick P. Thalamo-frontal psychosis.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1993
;
163
:
113
–5.
Menkes DB. Triazolam-induced nocturnal bingeing with amnesia.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry
 
1992
;
26
:
320
–1.
Merriam AE. Kleine–Levin syndrome following acute viral encephalitis.
Biol Psychiatry
 
1986
;
21
:
1301
–4.
Mignot E, Lammers GJ, Ripley B, Okun M, Nevsimalova S, Overeem S, et al. The role of cerebrospinal fluid hypocretin measurement in the diagnosis of narcolepsy and other hypersomnias.
Arch Neurol
 
2002
;
59
:
1553
–62.
Miller DL. Kleine–Levin syndrome: an atypical case?
Psychiatr Q
 
1970
;
44
:
26
–35.
Mukaddes NM, Alyanak B, Kora ME, Polvan O. The psychiatric symptomatology in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev
 
1999
;
29
:
253
–8.
Mukaddes NM, Kora ME, Bilge S. Carbamazepine for Kleine–Levin syndrome.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
 
1999
;
38
:
791
–2.
Muller T, Kuhn W, Bornke C, Buttner T, Przuntek H. Kleine–Levin syndrome and Parkinsonian symptoms—a case report.
J Neurol Sci
 
1998
;
157
:
214
–6.
Muratori F, Bertini N, Masi G. Efficacy of lithium treatment in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Eur Psychiatry
 
2002
;
17
:
232
–3.
Nassif X, Bourdoulous S, Eugene E, Couraud PO. How do extracellular pathogens cross the blood–brain barrier?
Trends Microbiol
 
2002
;
10
:
227
–32.
Overweg J. Lethargy and gluttony as an organic syndrome (Kleine–Levin syndrome).
Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd
 
1971
;
115
:
556
–8.
Papacostas SS. Photosensitivity during the hypersomnic phase in a patient with Kleine–Levin syndrome.
J Child Neurol
 
2003
;
18
:
432
–3.
Papacostas SS, Hadjivasilis V. The Kleine–Levin syndrome. Report of a case and review of the literature.
Eur Psychiatry
 
2000
;
15
:
231
–5.
Papy JJ, Conte-Devolx B, Sormani J, Porto R, Guillaume V. The periodic hypersomnia and megaphagia syndrome in a young female, correlated with menstrual cycle.
Rev Electroencephalogr Neurophysiol Clin
 
1982
;
12
:
54
–61.
Pelin Z, Ozturk L, Bozluolcay M. Posttraumatic Kleine–Levin syndrome: a case report.
Eur Psychiatry
 
2004
;
19
:
521
–2.
Persson T, Olsson L, Ortman E. The Kleine–Levin syndrome: report of a case and discussion.
Behav NeuroPsychiatry
 
1969
;
1
:
4
–6.
Pike M, Stores G. Kleine–Levin syndrome: a cause of diagnostic confusion.
Arch Dis Child
 
1994
;
71
:
355
–7.
Plasse M, Joannard A, Maynard R, Jouk PS, Gilly R, Beaudoing A. Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Pediatrie
 
1982
;
37
:
601
–5.
Poppe M, Friebel D, Reuner U, Todt H, Koch R, Heubner G. The Kleine–Levin syndrome—effects of treatment with lithium.
Neuropediatrics
 
2003
;
34
:
113
–9.
Portilla P, Durand E, Chalvon A, Habert M, Navelet Y, Prigent A, et al. SPECT-identified hypoperfusion of the left temporomesial structures in a Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Rev Neurol (Paris)
 
2002
;
158
:
593
–5.
Powers PS, Gunderman R. Kleine–Levin syndrome associated with fire setting.
Am J Dis Child
 
1978
;
132
:
786
–9.
Prabhakaran N, Murthy GK, Mallya UL. A case of Kleine–Levin syndrome in India.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1970
;
117
:
517
–9.
Rapoport SI, Hori M, Klatzo I. Reversible osmotic opening of the blood–brain barrier.
Science
 
1971
;
173
:
1026
–8.
Reimao R, Shimizu MH. Kleine–Levin syndrome. Clinical course, polysomnography and multiple sleep latency test. Case report.
Arq Neuropsiquiatr
 
1998
;
56
:
650
–4.
Reynolds CF 3rd, Black RS, Coble P, Holzer B, Kupfer DJ. Similarities in EEG sleep findings for Kleine–Levin syndrome and unipolar depression.
Am J Psychiatry
 
1980
;
137
:
116
–8.
Reynolds CF, Kupfer DJ, Christiansen CL, Auchenbach RC, Brenner RP, Sewitch DE, et al. Multiple sleep latency test findings in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
J Nerv Ment Dis
 
1984
;
172
:
41
–4.
Rosenow F, Kotagal P, Cohen BH, Green C, Wyllie E. Multiple sleep latency test and polysomnography in diagnosing Kleine–Levin syndrome and periodic hypersomnia.
J Clin Neurophysiol
 
2000
;
17
:
519
–22.
Roth B, Smolik P, Soucek K. Kleine–Levin syndrome—lithium prophylaxis.
Cesk Psychiatr
 
1980
;
76
:
156
–62.
Russell J, Grunstein R. Kleine–Levin syndrome: a case report.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry
 
1992
;
26
:
119
–23.
Sachs C, Persson HE, Hagenfeldt K. Menstruation-related periodic hypersomnia: a case study with successful treatment.
Neurology
 
1982
;
32
:
1376
–9.
Sadeghu M. Kleine–Levin syndrome: a report of three adolescent female patients.
Arch Iran Med
 
1999
;
2
:
1
–3.
Sagar RS, Khandelwal SK, Gupta S. Interepisodic morbidity in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1990
;
157
:
139
–41.
Salter MS, White PD. A variant of the Kleine–Levin syndrome precipitated by both Epstein–Barr and varicella–zoster virus infections.
Biol Psychiatry
 
1993
;
33
:
388
–90.
Savet JF, Robert H, Angeli C. A case of Kleine–Levin syndrome stabilized for over 1 year with carbamazepine.
Presse Med
 
1986
;
15
:
1281
.
Schlierf C. Report on a case of periodic hypersomnia (Kleine–Levin syndrome).
Nervenarzt
 
1975
;
46
:
317
–24.
Sethi S, Bhargava SC. Kleine–Levin syndrome and encephalitis.
Indian J Pediatr
 
2002
;
69
:
999
–1000.
Shukla G, Bajpai H, Mishra D. Kleine–Levin syndrome: a case-report from India.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1982
;
141
:
97
–104.
Smirne S, Castellotti V, Passerini D, Lizzi F. EEG study in a case of Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Riv Neurol
 
1970
;
40
:
357
–65.
Smolik P, Roth B. Kleine–Levin syndrome etiopathogenesis and treatment.
Acta Univ Carol Med Monogr
 
1988
;
128
:
5
–94.
Striano S, Bilo L, Meo R. An unusual case of Kleine–Levin syndrome associated with sleep terrors.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
 
1986
;
64
:
517
–20.
Takrani LB, Cronin D. Kleine–Levin syndrome in a female patient.
Can Psychiatr Assoc J
 
1976
;
21
:
315
–8.
Testa S, Opportuno A, Gallo P, Tavolato B. A case of multiple sclerosis with an onset mimicking the Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Ital J Neurol Sci
 
1987
;
8
:
151
–5.
Thacore VR, Ahmed M, Oswald I. The EEG in a case of periodic hypersomnia.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
 
1969
;
27
:
605
–6.
Turgman J, Braham J. Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Harefuah
 
1977
;
92
:
552
–4.
Visscher F, van der Horst AR, Smit LM. HLA-DR antigens in Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Ann Neurol
 
1990
;
28
:
195
.
Vlach V. Periodical somnolence, bulimia and mental changes (Kleine–Levin syndrome).
Cesk Neurol
 
1962
;
25
:
401
–5.
Wilder J. A case of atypical Kleine–Levin syndrome: 30 years' observation.
J Nerv Ment Dis
 
1972
;
154
:
69
–72.
Wilkus RJ, Chiles JA. Electrophysiological changes during episodes of the Kleine–Levin syndrome.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
 
1975
;
38
:
1225
–31.
Will RG, Young JP, Thomas DJ. Kleine–Levin syndrome: report of two cases with onset of symptoms precipitated by head trauma.
Br J Psychiatry
 
1988
;
152
:
410
–2.
Yassa R, Nair NP. The Kleine–Levine syndrome—a variant?
J Clin Psychiatry
 
1978
;
39
:
254
–9.
Zeitlhofer J, Mamoli B, Wolf C. Differential diagnostic problems and polygraphic studies in a case of Pickwickian syndrome.
Wien Klin Wochenschr
 
1982
;
94
:
434
–8.
Zhou Q. Kleine–Levin syndrome.
Chinese Clin Neurol
 
2004
;
12
:
423
–4.

Author notes

1Stanford University Center for Narcolepsy, 2Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Palo Alto, CA, 3Kleine–Levin Syndrome Foundation, Boston, MA, USA and 4Fédération des Pathologies du Sommeil, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France