Abstract

Adherence to social norms is compromised in a variety of neuropsychiatric conditions. Functional neuroimaging studies have investigated social norm compliance in healthy individuals, leading to the identification of a network of fronto-subcortical regions that underpins this ability. However, there is a lack of corroborative evidence from human lesion models investigating the structural anatomy of norm compliance across this fronto-subcortical network. To address this, we developed a neuroeconomic task to investigate social norm compliance in a neurodegenerative lesion model: behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia, a condition characterized by gross social dysfunction. The task assessed norm compliance across three behaviours that are well-studied in the neuroeconomics literature: fairness, prosocial and punishing behaviours. We administered our novel version of the Ultimatum Game in 22 patients with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and 22 age-matched controls, to assess how decision-making behaviour was modulated in response to (i) fairness of monetary offers; and (ii) social context of monetary offers designed to produce either prosocial or punishing behaviours. Voxel-based morphometry was used to characterize patterns of grey matter atrophy associated with task performance. Acceptance rates between patients and controls were equivalent when only fairness was manipulated. However, patients were impaired in modulating their decisions in response to social contextual information. Patients’ performance in the punishment condition was consistent with a reduced tendency to engage in punishment; this was associated with decreased grey matter volume in the anterior cingulate, orbitofrontal cortex, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and right inferior frontal gyrus. In the prosocial condition, patients’ performance suggested a reduced expression of prosocial behaviour, associated with decreased grey matter in the anterior insula, lateral orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate and dorsal striatum. Acceptance rates in the Ultimatum Game were also significantly related to impairments in the everyday expression of empathic concern. In conclusion, we demonstrate that compliance to basic social norms (fairness) can be maintained in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia; however, more complex normative behaviours (prosociality, punishment) that require integration of social contextual information are disrupted in association with atrophy in key fronto-striatal regions. These results suggest that the integration of social contextual information to guide normative behaviour is uniquely impaired in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia, and may explain other common features of the condition including gullibility and impaired empathy. Our findings also converge with previous functional neuroimaging investigations in healthy individuals and provide the first description of the structural anatomy of social norm compliance in a neurodegenerative lesion model.

 Adherence to social norms relies on fronto-subcortical networks. O'Callaghan et al. use a decision-making task to determine how social interaction breaks down in patients with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. They show that impairment correlates with grey matter loss in fronto-striatal brain regions.

Adherence to social norms relies on fronto-subcortical networks. O'Callaghan et al. use a decision-making task to determine how social interaction breaks down in patients with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. They show that impairment correlates with grey matter loss in fronto-striatal brain regions.

Introduction

Decisions in social contexts are complex and often require compromise between self-interest and consideration of others. Ever implicit in such decision-making is a regard for social norms; that collective sentiment of what constitutes appropriate behaviour, which is so fundamental to adaptive human interaction ( Buckholtz and Marois, 2012 ). Despite recent advances ( Spitzer et al. , 2007 ; Baumgartner et al. , 2011 ; Ruff et al. , 2013 ), little is known regarding the neurobiology of social norm compliance. Even more pressing is the need for a framework to account for dysfunctional social norm compliance, which underscores symptoms across a range of neuropsychiatric conditions.

Certain social normative behaviours are increasingly studied in the neuroeconomics literature. These include the tendency to engage in prosocial behaviours such as fairness or altruism, and the tendency to enact punishment upon others who violate social norms ( Buckholtz and Marois, 2012 ). Despite the diversity of social norms at play, insights from the field of neuroeconomics implicate a distinct network of fronto-striatal-insular regions that appears to mediate compliance to social norms ( Sanfey et al. , 2003 ; Spitzer et al. , 2007 ; Baumgartner et al. , 2011 ; Chang and Sanfey, 2013 ; Ruff et al. , 2013 ; Gabay et al. , 2014 ; Ruff and Fehr, 2014 ).

The behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia (FTD), a neurodegenerative condition with insidious, progressive change in personality and social interactions, represents the prototypical example of disordered social norm compliance. Patients commonly exhibit behavioural changes considered under this rubric, including loss of empathy and insight, disinhibited remarks or behaviour, egocentricity, impulsive spending or gambling, and gullibility ( Piguet et al. , 2011 ). Intriguingly, the earliest sites of pathology overlap with those regions implicated in social norm compliance, most notably in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, insula, amygdalae and striatum ( Broe et al. , 2003 ; Seeley et al. , 2008 ; O'Callaghan et al. , 2014 ; Bertoux et al. , 2015 ). Decision-making paradigms traditionally used in the literature, which typically involve monetary gambles under conditions of risk and ambiguity, are found to have limited utility in behavioural variant FTD ( Gleichgerrcht et al. , 2010 ; Bertoux et al. , 2013 ; Kloeters et al. , 2013 ). Convergent approaches incorporating measures of social processing and decision-making represent a promising avenue to better detect the complex social-contextual deficits that typify behavioural variant FTD ( Ibañez and Manes, 2012 ).

The Ultimatum Game, a paradigm drawn from the neuroeconomics literature, offers a means of gauging normative decision-making behaviours in a social context. The task requires participants to either accept or reject monetary offers, varying in their degree of ‘fairness’. A consistent observation is that healthy participants frequently reject unfair offers, in order to punish their opponent, even though this decision incurs a personal cost ( Güth et al. , 1982 ; Sanfey et al. , 2014 ). Such unfair offers are considered a violation of ‘fairness norms’ and therefore deserve sanctioning ( Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004 ).

Here, we sought to investigate social normative behaviour in behavioural variant FTD by introducing social contextual factors in the Ultimatum Game. In this novel manipulation, we included ‘social framing’ conditions intended to either induce participants to accept more offers due to compassion/desire to help (prosocial), or to incite the desire to punish via rejecting more offers (punishing). Fairness behaviour has deep developmental roots ( Fehr et al. , 2008 ; Castelli et al. , 2014 ), and examples of altered responses to fairness on the Ultimatum Game have been attributed to states of exaggerated emotional reactivity ( Koenigs and Tranel, 2007 ; Crockett et al. , 2008 ), which is not present in behavioural variant FTD. Because of this, we predicted that fairness behaviours may remain robust in behavioural variant FTD and that patients would demonstrate intact expression of fairness and thus perform similarly to healthy controls in the classic Ultimatum Game. However, in contrast, we predicted patients would have difficulty adapting their behaviour to engage in prosocial or punishing choice in the social framing conditions, as they require the integration of social contextual factors, an ability specifically known to be disrupted in behavioural variant FTD ( Ibañez and Manes, 2012 ). We anticipated that patients’ abnormal expression of social norms in the social framing conditions would correlate with grey matter loss in discrete regions within the fronto-subcortical network previously implicated in social norm compliance.

Materials and methods

Case selection

Twenty-two patients with behavioural variant FTD were recruited from the FRONTIER dementia clinic, at Neuroscience Research Australia. All patients met current consensus criteria for behavioural variant FTD ( Neary et al. , 1998 ; Rascovsky et al. , 2011 ). Twenty-two age- and education-matched healthy controls were selected from a volunteer panel. The Mini-Mental Status Examination was administered as a measure of general cognition. The Frontotemporal Dementia Rating Scale (FRS; Mioshi et al. , 2010 ) was used as a measure of clinical staging for the patients. In brief, the FRS is an informant-rated staging scheme validated for use in behavioural variant FTD, tapping into a variety of behavioural and functional symptoms. Lower percentage scores on the FRS indicate more severe impairment, with six stages ranging from very mild to profound . The study was approved by the local Ethics Committees and all participants provided informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. See Table 1 for demographic details and clinical characteristics.

Table 1

Scores on demographics, behavioural symptoms and background neuropsychology for patients with behavioural variant FTD and control subjects

Demographics, clinical characteristics and empathyControlBehavioural variant FTDP -values
n2222
Sex (M:F)11:1118:4
Age64.8 (11.1)64.8 (8.8)n.s.
Education13.8 (1.9)12.0 (1.8)n.s.
MMSE (max. 30)29.4 (1.1)26.3 (1.9)<0.001
Duration (years diagnosed)2.3 (2.0)
Behavioural symptoms
FRS staging % score 38.8 (17.2) §
CBI-R Total score (max. 180)73.1 (25.5)
Empathy item (max. 4)3.0 (1.5)
Neuropsychology
Digit span total20.5 (2.5)14.7 (3.5)<0.001
TMT-A (s)31.2 (14.2)52.8 (36.7)<0.05
TMT-B (s)53.2 (14.2)174.6 (97.5)<0.001
RAVLT (trials 1–5)54.2 (8.2)31.8 (10.2)<0.001
RAVLT delay11.0 (3.2)4.4 (3.7)<0.001
Rey Figure Copy34.2 (2.1)26.3 (8.1)<0.01
Rey Figure 3-minute delay17.9 (5.8)10.9 (7.3)<0.01
Demographics, clinical characteristics and empathyControlBehavioural variant FTDP -values
n2222
Sex (M:F)11:1118:4
Age64.8 (11.1)64.8 (8.8)n.s.
Education13.8 (1.9)12.0 (1.8)n.s.
MMSE (max. 30)29.4 (1.1)26.3 (1.9)<0.001
Duration (years diagnosed)2.3 (2.0)
Behavioural symptoms
FRS staging % score 38.8 (17.2) §
CBI-R Total score (max. 180)73.1 (25.5)
Empathy item (max. 4)3.0 (1.5)
Neuropsychology
Digit span total20.5 (2.5)14.7 (3.5)<0.001
TMT-A (s)31.2 (14.2)52.8 (36.7)<0.05
TMT-B (s)53.2 (14.2)174.6 (97.5)<0.001
RAVLT (trials 1–5)54.2 (8.2)31.8 (10.2)<0.001
RAVLT delay11.0 (3.2)4.4 (3.7)<0.001
Rey Figure Copy34.2 (2.1)26.3 (8.1)<0.01
Rey Figure 3-minute delay17.9 (5.8)10.9 (7.3)<0.01

Values are reported as mean (standard deviation).

n.s. = non-significant; § ‘Moderate’ stage.

MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; FRS = Frontotemporal Dementia Rating Scale; CBI-R = Cambridge Behavioural Inventory-Revised; TMT = Trail Making Test Parts A and B.

Table 1

Scores on demographics, behavioural symptoms and background neuropsychology for patients with behavioural variant FTD and control subjects

Demographics, clinical characteristics and empathyControlBehavioural variant FTDP -values
n2222
Sex (M:F)11:1118:4
Age64.8 (11.1)64.8 (8.8)n.s.
Education13.8 (1.9)12.0 (1.8)n.s.
MMSE (max. 30)29.4 (1.1)26.3 (1.9)<0.001
Duration (years diagnosed)2.3 (2.0)
Behavioural symptoms
FRS staging % score 38.8 (17.2) §
CBI-R Total score (max. 180)73.1 (25.5)
Empathy item (max. 4)3.0 (1.5)
Neuropsychology
Digit span total20.5 (2.5)14.7 (3.5)<0.001
TMT-A (s)31.2 (14.2)52.8 (36.7)<0.05
TMT-B (s)53.2 (14.2)174.6 (97.5)<0.001
RAVLT (trials 1–5)54.2 (8.2)31.8 (10.2)<0.001
RAVLT delay11.0 (3.2)4.4 (3.7)<0.001
Rey Figure Copy34.2 (2.1)26.3 (8.1)<0.01
Rey Figure 3-minute delay17.9 (5.8)10.9 (7.3)<0.01
Demographics, clinical characteristics and empathyControlBehavioural variant FTDP -values
n2222
Sex (M:F)11:1118:4
Age64.8 (11.1)64.8 (8.8)n.s.
Education13.8 (1.9)12.0 (1.8)n.s.
MMSE (max. 30)29.4 (1.1)26.3 (1.9)<0.001
Duration (years diagnosed)2.3 (2.0)
Behavioural symptoms
FRS staging % score 38.8 (17.2) §
CBI-R Total score (max. 180)73.1 (25.5)
Empathy item (max. 4)3.0 (1.5)
Neuropsychology
Digit span total20.5 (2.5)14.7 (3.5)<0.001
TMT-A (s)31.2 (14.2)52.8 (36.7)<0.05
TMT-B (s)53.2 (14.2)174.6 (97.5)<0.001
RAVLT (trials 1–5)54.2 (8.2)31.8 (10.2)<0.001
RAVLT delay11.0 (3.2)4.4 (3.7)<0.001
Rey Figure Copy34.2 (2.1)26.3 (8.1)<0.01
Rey Figure 3-minute delay17.9 (5.8)10.9 (7.3)<0.01

Values are reported as mean (standard deviation).

n.s. = non-significant; § ‘Moderate’ stage.

MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; FRS = Frontotemporal Dementia Rating Scale; CBI-R = Cambridge Behavioural Inventory-Revised; TMT = Trail Making Test Parts A and B.

Background neuropsychology

General neuropsychological tests to assess executive function and memory were administered to both patients and controls. Executive function measures included attention and working memory assessed via the digit span task (total score of digits repeated forwards and backwards; Wechsler, 1997 ). To assess psychomotor speed and attentional set-shifting, the Trail Making Test, parts A and B, were administered ( Strauss et al. , 2006 ). Verbal memory encoding and retrieval were assessed using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; ( Lezak et al. , 2004 ), sum of trials 1–5 and the delay score, respectively. Copying and short-term visual recall were assessed using the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure ( Meyers, 1995 ).

Capacity for empathy

The Cambridge Behavioural Inventory-Revised (CBI-R; Wear et al. , 2008 ) was used to assess behavioural disturbance in the patients. The CBI-R is a 45-item informant-rated questionnaire probing a variety of neuropsychiatric, cognitive and functional symptoms, rating their frequency of occurrence from 0 (never) to 4 (constantly). As such, higher CBI-R scores indicate greater behavioural dysfunction. To assess empathy specifically, we extracted scores from the item that best exemplifies deficits in this ability: ‘Appears indifferent to the worries and concerns of family members’. Importantly, this question addresses empathic concern—an aspect of empathy primarily affected in behavioural variant FTD ( Baez et al. , 2014 ).

Ultimatum Game

We created a modified Ultimatum Game with baseline and reappraisal versions, using the same monetary amounts and fair to unfair offer ratios that have been previously described ( Koenigs and Tranel, 2007 ). In both the baseline and reappraisal versions, participants acted in response to different proposers who offered to split a hypothetical $10 with them. The proposer–responder offers ranged from fair ($5–$5; $6–$4), to unfair ($7–$3; $8–$2; $9–$1). Based on previous findings ( Güth et al. , 1982 ), including those validated in neurological patients and in older adults ( Koenigs and Tranel, 2007 ), we operationalized ‘fair’ acceptance rates as the average of $5–$5 or $6–$4, and the ‘unfair’ as the average of $7–$3, $8–$2 and $9–$1 acceptance rates.

In the baseline condition, participants were informed they would play against 22 different people, each of whom had been given $10 to divide. It was explained that proposers were free to decide how to split the money, but participants could choose whether to accept the offer (resulting in a payout for both players) or reject the offer (resulting in $0 for both). An example of baseline trials is shown in Fig. 1 A. In each trial a black and white photograph of a neutral face, with the caption ‘[name] has made you an offer’ was presented on a computer screen for 3.5 s. This was followed by a decision screen where the offer was stated, e.g. ‘[name] gets $7, you get $3’, and a prompt to either ‘accept’ or ‘reject’. This decision screen was displayed until a response was made, followed by a feedback screen of ‘you get $3’ or ‘you both get $0’ (4 s) depending on the response made.

Example of trials in the Ultimatum Game. ( A ) An example of a trial in the baseline condition, where the participant has accepted the offer. ( B and C ) Example trials from the prosocial and punishing social framing conditions where the offers were accepted and rejected, respectively.
Figure 1

Example of trials in the Ultimatum Game. ( A ) An example of a trial in the baseline condition, where the participant has accepted the offer. ( B and C ) Example trials from the prosocial and punishing social framing conditions where the offers were accepted and rejected, respectively.

In the social framing version, participants were informed they would play against a set of 22 new people, each given $10 to divide, with the same contingences applying for accepting or rejecting offers. However, now they were provided with information about the proposers’ current circumstances. The prosocial condition framed proposers as poor, or ‘down on their luck’, and was designed to elicit a higher acceptance rate on the basis that participants should feel sorry for the proposer. The punishing condition framed proposers as rich to encourage higher rejection rates or punishing behaviour, as the offers (particularly the unfair ones) should evoke a heightened sense of unfairness. Social framing trials are exemplified in Fig. 1 B and C. As in the baseline condition, in each trial a black and white photograph of a neutral face was presented, followed by a description screen (4.5 s). Descriptions were restricted to brief and uncomplicated language. Examples of the prosocial condition included: ‘[name] lost his/her house in a fire’; ‘[name] is saving for his/her son’s operation’; and ‘[name] is homeless’, and examples of the punishing condition included: ‘[name] owns an international company’; ‘[name] just won the lottery’; and ‘[name] is a wealthy investment banker’. A decision screen with the offer followed, then a feedback screen. To ensure patients understood the terminology (i.e. that winning the lottery or being a wealthy investment banker would be associated with being rich; or being homeless would be associated with being poor etc.), a checklist was administered at the end of the experiment. All patients included in the study demonstrated intact understanding of the social framing terminology.

In both the baseline and social framing conditions the 22 trials comprised two of each fair offer and six of each unfair offer. To control for possible gender biases deriving from the stimulus set, within each condition half of the presented faces were female and half were male. In addition, the offer amounts were paired with proposers on a random cycle, to control for the possibility that features of a proposer (for example, physical attractiveness) might induce systematic response biases. The photos were of neutral expressions across all conditions, to further reduce the possibility of eliciting response biases on the basis on perceived friendliness/unfriendliness. For the social framing condition, an equal mix of prosocial and punishing descriptors made up the 22 trials, and these were presented in a randomized order. Each participant completed the baseline version first, followed by the social framing version. Participants were not awarded actual monetary payouts in relation to the task and were not compensated for their participation; they were, however, instructed to make their choices based on how they would act in a real-life situation.

For the Ultimatum Game, the outcome measures were percentage acceptance rates for fair and unfair offers in each of the conditions. Further to this, we created percentage scores to reflect ‘change from baseline’ in the social framing conditions, in order to see both the extent and direction that acceptance rates deviated from baseline in response to the social framing information. The extent of each participants ‘change from baseline’ scores were calculated by subtracting acceptance rates in the prosocial/punishing conditions from baseline acceptance rates, then taking the reciprocal of this to represent directionality of the change. These change scores were calculated separately for fair and unfair offers.

Behavioural analysis

Demographics and background neuropsychology variables were analysed by two-tailed independent samples t -tests, using SPSS v.22 (Chicago, IL, USA). For the Ultimatum Game, we analysed our results by estimating a logistic GEE (generalized estimating equations) model with an exchangeable working correlation structure, which accounts for within-subjects correlation across decisions. We did not use a repeated-measures ANOVA analysis on proportions (i.e. rejection rates) for two reasons: the ANOVA assumption of variance homogeneity is violated for categorical data, and ANOVA does not respect the upper and lower bounds of 0 and 1 for proportions ( Jaeger, 2008 ) for a more complete exposition of arguments favouring a logit specification over ANOVA for categorical variables). The binary dependent variable is the decision to accept or reject an offer, and the independent variables are a three-way factorial of the fairness level (fair versus unfair acceptance rates), group membership (control versus behavioural variant FTD) and the social framing condition (baseline versus punishing versus prosocial), as well as a set of control variables. Variables were introduced in the main analysis to control for the effects of gender, given previous evidence of gender effects in the Ultimatum Game ( Croson and Gneezy, 2009 ), and also to control for the impact of patients’ cognitive deficits on Ultimatum Game performance. We controlled for gender by including a main effect of gender, and two-way interactions of gender with condition, fairness, and group. We controlled for cognitive ability using neuropsychological variables covering three cognitive domains [i.e. episodic memory as measured by the RAVLT delay score; speeded set-shifting as indexed by the Trail Making Test part B (TMT-B); attention/working memory as assessed by Digit Span total]. Consequently, all of the results presented in the main text were controlled for the possible effects of these variables. All results marked as ‘ post hoc comparisons’ were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Sidak correction. Note, 95% confidence intervals of effects will be presented throughout in […]. GEE analyses were conducted using the Stata 13 software package (Stata Corporation).

Imaging acquisition

Whole-brain T 1 images were acquired using 3 T Philips MRI scanners with standard quadrature head coil (8 channels). The 3D T-weighted sequences were acquired as follows: coronal orientation, matrix 256 × 256, 200 slices, 1 × 1 mm 2 in-plane resolution, slice thickness 1 mm, echo time/repetition time = 2.6/5.8 ms.

Voxel-based morphometry analysis

Three dimensional T 1 -weighted sequences were analysed with FSL-VBM, a voxel-based morphometry analysis ( Ashburner and Friston, 2000 ; Good et al. , 2001 ), part of the FSL software package http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslvbm/index.html ( Smith et al. , 2004 ). First, tissue segmentation was carried out using FMRIB’s Automatic Segmentation Tool (FAST) ( Zhang et al. , 2001 ) from brain-extracted images. The resulting grey matter partial volume maps were then aligned to the Montreal Neurological Institute standard space (MNI152) using the non-linear registration approach (FNIRT; Andersson et al. , 2007 a , b ), which uses a b-spline representation of the registration warp field ( Rueckert et al. , 1999 ). Registered partial volume maps were then modulated (to correct for local expansion or contraction) by dividing them by the Jacobian of the warp field. The modulated images were then smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a standard deviation of 3 mm (full-width at half-maximum = 8 mm). Based on previous studies that defined neural correlates of the Ultimatum Game ( Sanfey et al. , 2003 ) and social norm processing ( Spitzer et al. , 2007 ) across various prefrontal, striatal and limbic regions, we created a region of interest mask using the Harvard-Oxford cortical and subcortical structural atlases. The following bilateral atlas regions were included in the mask: frontal pole, frontal orbital cortex, subcallosal cortex, frontal medial cortex, superior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, cingulate gyrus (anterior division), paracingulate gyrus, caudate, putamen, nucleus accumbens, insula cortex and amygdala.

Group differences in grey matter intensity between patients and controls, within the fronto-subcortical mask, were compared using a voxelwise general linear model. Significant clusters were formed by using the threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) method ( Smith and Nichols, 2009 ). The TFCE method is a cluster-based thresholding method that does not require the setting of an arbitrary cluster forming threshold (e.g. t,z). Instead, it takes a raw statistics image and produces an output image in which the voxel-wise values represent the amount of cluster-like local spatial support. The TFCE image is then turned into voxel-wise P -values via permutation testing. We used a permutation-based non-parametric testing with 5000 permutations ( Nichols and Holmes, 2002 ). Differences in grey matter intensity between patients and controls were assessed using t -tests, tested for significance at P < .05, corrected for multiple comparisons via family-wise error (FWE) correction across space.

Following this, correlations between Ultimatum Game performance and grey matter intensity were conducted. Both patients and controls were included in the analysis, to add greater variance in the behavioural scores thereby increasing the study’s statistical power to detect brain-behaviour relationships ( Sollberger et al. , 2009 ; Irish et al. , 2014 ). The ‘change from baseline’ scores for unfair offers in both the positive and negative social framing conditions were entered as covariates in separate general linear model matrices. For additional statistical power, a covariate only statistical model with either a positive or negative t-contrast was used. These contrasts were applied to test the association between the degree of patients’ abnormal behavioural performance in the social framing condition (i.e. if acceptance rates were lower in the prosocial condition or higher in the punishing condition) and grey matter loss. Therefore, a positive t-contrast was applied to ‘change from baseline’ in the prosocial condition, in order to index an association between decreasing grey matter volume and extent of lowered acceptance rates. A negative t-contrast was applied to ‘change from baseline’ scores in the punishing condition, which indexed an association between decreasing grey matter volume and the extent of higher acceptance rates. Positive and negative contrasts were tested with 5000 permutations and reported at a significance level of P < 0.001 uncorrected and at a cluster threshold of >35 contiguous voxels.

Results

Demographics and background neuropsychology

As shown in Table 1 , patients and controls were not significantly different for age and education levels ( t -values −0.02 and 2.0, P -values 0.985 and 0.061, respectively). Patients had significantly lower Mini-Mental Status Examination scores ( t = 6.1, P < 0.001). In terms of background neuropsychology, patients had significantly slower psychomotor speed on TMT-A ( t = −2.2, P < 0.05). Patients also had worse working memory/attention, speeded set-shifting and visuospatial copying (digit span: t = 5.6, P < 0.001; TMT-B: t = −4.9, P < 0.001; Rey Complex Figure copy: t = 3.7, P < 0.01). Patients’ scores were significantly reduced across verbal and non-verbal memory encoding and retrieval (RAVLT 1–5: t = 6.5, P < 0.001; RAVLT delay: t = 5.5, P < 0.001; Rey Complex Figure 3 minute delay: t = 3.2, P < 0.01).

Baseline acceptance rates in the Ultimatum Game. Percentage of offers accepted in the baseline condition for fair versus unfair offer amounts. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. bvFTD = behavioural variant FTD.
Figure 2

Baseline acceptance rates in the Ultimatum Game. Percentage of offers accepted in the baseline condition for fair versus unfair offer amounts. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. bvFTD = behavioural variant FTD.

Social framing acceptance rates in the Ultimatum Game. Percentage of fair versus unfair offers accepted in the prosocial and punishing social framing conditions. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. bvFTD = behavioural variant FTD.
Figure 3

Social framing acceptance rates in the Ultimatum Game. Percentage of fair versus unfair offers accepted in the prosocial and punishing social framing conditions. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. bvFTD = behavioural variant FTD.

Ultimatum Game baseline condition

Acceptance rates in the baseline condition are shown in Fig. 2 . The group × fairness level interaction was not significant [χ 2 (1) = 0.03, P = 0.870]. The main effect of group was not significant [χ 2 (1) = 0.69, P = 0.405], but there was a significant main effect for fairness level [χ 2 (1) = 53.83, P < 0.0001). The acceptance rates for both control subjects and patients with behavioural variant FTD fell significantly in the unfair condition compared to the fair condition by 35.5% (19.6–51.4%) and 37.4% (23.1–51.7%), respectively. The absence of a significant interaction between fairness and group implies that the acceptance rates of the two groups were not significantly different within both the fair and unfair condition. Consequently, both patients and controls exhibited a normative response to the fairness manipulation, that is, accepting significantly fewer unfair offers. See Supplementary Fig. 1A , for a graphical representation of acceptance rates at each fairness level.

Social framing conditions

This section compares acceptance rates across the two social framing conditions (punishing versus prosocial), as illustrated in Fig. 3 . The overall three-way interaction of fairness level, social framing and group was not significant [χ 2 (1) = 2.63, P = 0.105]. The fairness by group interaction was not significant [χ 2 (1) = 0.99, P = 0.319], indicating that response rates in both controls and patients were modulated to a similar degree by fairness level—a similar pattern to that seen in the baseline condition. By contrast, social framing interacted with fairness [χ 2 (1) = 12.96, P < 0.001]. More importantly, there was also a significant social framing by group interaction [χ 2 (1) = 8.75, P < 0.01], such that the social framing condition modulated the responses of both groups differentially.

For the social framing by fairness interaction, post hoc comparisons revealed that for unfair offers the increase in the acceptance rate comparing the punishing to the prosocial conditions, 29.3% points (21.1–37.5%), was significant [χ 2 (1) = 64.05, P < 0.0001]. However, acceptance rates for fair offers were not significantly different across the punishing and prosocial conditions [χ 2 (1) = 2.80, P = 0.179].

Regarding the social framing by group interaction, post hoc comparisons showed that in the punishing condition, acceptance rates for controls and patients were not significantly different [χ 2 (1) = 0.14, P = 0.916]. However, in the prosocial condition patients accepted significantly less offers than controls [χ 2 (1) = 13.72, P < 0.001]. Controls demonstrated significantly lower acceptance rates [a difference of 37.2% (23.5–50.1%)] in the punishing versus prosocial condition [χ 2 (1) = 37.09, P < 0.0001]. Patient acceptance rates were also significantly lower in the punishing versus prosocial conditions, 14.9% difference (7.3–22.5%) [χ 2 (1) = 19.32, P < 0.0001]. Importantly, the effect size of the change in patients’ acceptance rates (across conditions) is significantly less than that of the controls 22.2% (7.5–37.0%) [χ 2 (1) = 8.75, P < 0.01]. Hence, patients were significantly less influenced by condition than controls indicating a muted response to the prosocial framing. See Supplementary Fig. 1B and Supplementary Data for acceptance rates at each offer level of the social framing conditions.

Comparisons with the baseline condition

Comparisons with baseline are shown in Fig. 4 . The overall three-way interaction of fairness level, social framing and group was not significant [χ 2 (2) = 3.37, P = 0.186]. The fairness × group interaction was not significant [χ 2 (1) =0.45, P = 0.5], indicating that acceptance rate changes from the baseline in both controls and patients were modulated to a similar degree by fairness level. By contrast, social framing interacted with fairness [χ 2 (2) = 26.15, P < 0.001]. Also, there was a significant social framing by group interaction [χ 2 (2) = 9.34, P < 0.01], indicating that the social framing condition modulated the change in acceptance rates from the baseline for both groups differentially.

Change from baseline acceptance rates in the social framing conditions. Panels show the change in acceptance rates from baseline in the social framing conditions, represented separately as fair ( A ) versus unfair ( B ) offer amounts. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 4

Change from baseline acceptance rates in the social framing conditions. Panels show the change in acceptance rates from baseline in the social framing conditions, represented separately as fair ( A ) versus unfair ( B ) offer amounts. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Post hoc comparisons revealed the following findings. For fair offers, in both the prosocial or punishing conditions neither control nor patient acceptance rates differed significantly from baseline ( P -values ranging from 0.93 to 1.00) ( Fig. 4 A). For unfair offers in the punishing condition, patients accepted on average 11% more than their baseline levels, although this difference was not significant [χ 2 (1) = 2.33, P = 0.663]. This contrasts with the control group whose acceptance rates for unfair offers in the punishing condition were virtually identical to the baseline rates, a difference of only 0.06% [χ 2 (1) = 0.00, P = 1.00] ( Fig. 4 B). For unfair offers in the prosocial condition, patients’ acceptance rates were significantly higher than their baseline levels by 28.5% [χ 2 (1) = 15.57, P < 0.001]. Controls’ unfair acceptance rates were also significantly higher than their baseline levels by 43.1% [χ 2 (1) = 44.99, P < 0.0001] ( Fig. 4 B). For the unfair condition, we compared the difference between acceptance rates in the punishing condition versus acceptance rates in the prosocial condition. Both groups accepted significantly more offers in the prosocial condition, compared to the punishing condition, with controls accepting 43% more (27.0–59.0%) [χ 2 (1) = 36.26, P < 0.0001], but patients accepting only 17.1% more (8.1–26.0%) [χ 2 (1) = 18.43, P < 0.0001]. The magnitude of this change differed significantly between the groups [difference: 25.9% (8.8–43.0%); χ 2 (1) = 8.81, P < 0.01], consistent with the patients displaying a divergent response to social framing, characterized by a tendency for higher acceptance rates in the punishing condition and a muted response to the prosocial condition.

Relationship to empathy

To investigate the effect of empathy on acceptance rates in the patients, we estimated a GEE logistic model using a three-way factorial design of the fairness level, social framing condition and the level of empathy impairment (minimal versus severe). We categorized patients based on their score on the CBI-R empathy item (ranging from 0 to 4, higher scores indicating more significant impairment). Minimal impairment was defined as values ranging from 0 to 3 and severe impairment as values of 4. A total of nine patients were in the minimally impaired range (three patients were rated 0, one patient scored 1, two patients had a score of 2, and three had a score of 3), the remaining 13 patients were in the severely impaired range, with scores of 4. The three-way interaction between empathy, fairness, and social framing condition was not significant [χ 2 (1) = 0.78, P = 0.377]. We found significant main effects of empathy [χ 2 (1) = 5.03, P < 0.05], fairness [χ 2 (1) =22.89, P < 0.001] and social framing [χ 2 (1) = 9.88, P < 0.01]. Two-way interactions between empathy and social framing, and fairness and social framing, were not significant [χ 2 (1) = 1.21, P = 0.271 and χ 2 (1) = 0.75, P = 0.377, respectively]. However, we found a significant interaction between empathy and fairness [χ 2 (1) = 4.83, P < 0.05].

Post hoc comparisons within the significant empathy and fairness interaction revealed that for unfair offers, those with severely impaired empathy had significantly lower acceptance rates than those with minimal impairment [39.0% versus 61.3% accepted; χ 2 (1) = 6.46, P < 0.05]. However, no difference was found between severely impaired and minimally impaired subjects for fair offers [82.2% versus 74.4% accepted; χ 2 (1) = 0.52, P = 0.718]. Together indicating that reduced empathy was specifically associated with lower acceptance rates for unfair offers.

Ultimatum Game: control variables

Here we report analysis of the gender and cognitive control variables that were included in the main analysis. For the individual cognitive variables, no effect of TMT-B or RAVLT delay was found on acceptance rates (coefficient = 0.002, z = 0.10, P = 0.918 and coefficient = −0.026, z = −0.77, P = 0.442, respectively); however, there was a significant main effect for digit span total (coefficient = 0.173, z = 3.45, P < 0.01), indicating that the higher the digit span of a subject, the higher their acceptance rate across conditions. The main effect of gender was significant [χ 2 (1) = 5.93, P < 0.05], as was the interaction with fairness [χ 2 (1) = 5.16, P < 0.05]. Post hoc comparisons reveal that for unfair offers males had significantly higher acceptance rates (62.6%) than females (45.7%) [χ 2 (1) = 6.99, P < 0.05]. No significant difference was found in acceptances rates for fair offers [χ 2 (1) = 0.06, P = 0.805]. The interactions of gender with condition and group were not statistically significant [χ 2 (2) = 2.66, P = 0.265 and χ 2 (1) = 1.13, P = 0.289, respectively]. The lack of significant interaction between gender and condition suggests that a gender effect is unlikely to account for specific deficits on the social aspect of the tasks exhibited by patients with behavioural variant FTD. However, future studies with equal gender distributions will be important to explore potential within-group variations in performance on the Ultimatum Game in behavioural variant FTD.

Voxel-based morphometry analysis

Atrophy pattern in behavioural variant FTD group

The behavioural variant FTD group was initially contrasted with controls to reveal overall patterns of grey matter intensity decrease in the fronto-subcortical mask. Patients showed characteristic patterns of atrophy throughout the fronto-subcortical regions of interest. For details see Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Fig. 2 .

Ultimatum Game

Regions of decreased grey matter intensity associated with higher acceptance rates for unfair offers in the punishing condition are shown in Table 2 and Fig. 5 A. These regions included the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, right inferior frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate and paracingulate cortices and medial orbitofrontal cortex and subgenual cingulate. The regions of decreased grey matter intensity associated with lower acceptance rates in the prosocial condition are shown in Table 2 and Fig. 5 B. These regions included a large cluster encompassing the left dorsal putamen and left anterior insular, other regions included the right dorsal posterior putamen and caudate body, and also the left lateral orbitofrontal cortex. See Supplementary Table 2 for reports of within group correlations between grey matter intensity and behavioural scores.

Voxel-based morphometry correlates for change from baseline scores in the social framing conditions. Region of interest voxel-based morphometry (VBM) results showing areas of significant grey matter intensity decrease correlating with ( A ) Punishing: higher acceptance rates for unfair offers in the punishing condition (red–yellow), showing significant clusters in (i) and (ii) orbitofrontal/subcallosal cortices; (iii) anterior cingulate/paracingulate cortices; (iv) right inferior frontal gyrus; and ( B ) Prosocial: lower acceptance rates for unfair offers in the prosocial condition (blue–light blue), showing significant clusters in (i) left dorsal putamen; (ii) right dorsal posterior putamen; (iii) left anterior insula; and (iv) left lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Results uncorrected at P < 0.001 and at a cluster threshold of >35 contiguous voxels.
Figure 5

Voxel-based morphometry correlates for change from baseline scores in the social framing conditions. Region of interest voxel-based morphometry (VBM) results showing areas of significant grey matter intensity decrease correlating with ( A ) Punishing: higher acceptance rates for unfair offers in the punishing condition (red–yellow), showing significant clusters in (i) and (ii) orbitofrontal/subcallosal cortices; (iii) anterior cingulate/paracingulate cortices; (iv) right inferior frontal gyrus; and ( B ) Prosocial: lower acceptance rates for unfair offers in the prosocial condition (blue–light blue), showing significant clusters in (i) left dorsal putamen; (ii) right dorsal posterior putamen; (iii) left anterior insula; and (iv) left lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Results uncorrected at P < 0.001 and at a cluster threshold of >35 contiguous voxels.

Table 2

Region of interest voxel-based morphometry results showing areas of significant grey matter intensity decrease correlating with higher acceptance rates for unfair offers in the punishing condition, and lower acceptance rates for unfair offers in the prosocial condition

RegionsHemisphere (L/R/B) MNI coordinates for voxel of maximal intensity
Number of voxels
xyz
Change from baseline: punishing condition
Orbitofrontal/subcallosal corticiesB1022−16445
Middle frontal gyrus/frontal poleL−483626318
Anterior cingulate/paracingulate corticesB24026221
Superior frontal gyrus/frontal poleB6524068
Middle frontal gyrusR3606049
Superior frontal gyrusB−4385646
Inferior frontal gyrusR60182241
Cingulate cortexB−1024036
Change from baseline: prosocial condition
Dorsal putamen/anterior insularL−26122445
Dorsal posterior putamenR28−81478
Lateral orbitofrontal cortexL−4428−272
Frontal medial cortex/frontal poleB856−1643
Caudate bodyR18−42038
RegionsHemisphere (L/R/B) MNI coordinates for voxel of maximal intensity
Number of voxels
xyz
Change from baseline: punishing condition
Orbitofrontal/subcallosal corticiesB1022−16445
Middle frontal gyrus/frontal poleL−483626318
Anterior cingulate/paracingulate corticesB24026221
Superior frontal gyrus/frontal poleB6524068
Middle frontal gyrusR3606049
Superior frontal gyrusB−4385646
Inferior frontal gyrusR60182241
Cingulate cortexB−1024036
Change from baseline: prosocial condition
Dorsal putamen/anterior insularL−26122445
Dorsal posterior putamenR28−81478
Lateral orbitofrontal cortexL−4428−272
Frontal medial cortex/frontal poleB856−1643
Caudate bodyR18−42038

Results uncorrected at P < 0.001 and at a cluster threshold of >35 contiguous voxels and reported at t > 3.3.

R = right; L = left; B = bilateral.

Table 2

Region of interest voxel-based morphometry results showing areas of significant grey matter intensity decrease correlating with higher acceptance rates for unfair offers in the punishing condition, and lower acceptance rates for unfair offers in the prosocial condition

RegionsHemisphere (L/R/B) MNI coordinates for voxel of maximal intensity
Number of voxels
xyz
Change from baseline: punishing condition
Orbitofrontal/subcallosal corticiesB1022−16445
Middle frontal gyrus/frontal poleL−483626318
Anterior cingulate/paracingulate corticesB24026221
Superior frontal gyrus/frontal poleB6524068
Middle frontal gyrusR3606049
Superior frontal gyrusB−4385646
Inferior frontal gyrusR60182241
Cingulate cortexB−1024036
Change from baseline: prosocial condition
Dorsal putamen/anterior insularL−26122445
Dorsal posterior putamenR28−81478
Lateral orbitofrontal cortexL−4428−272
Frontal medial cortex/frontal poleB856−1643
Caudate bodyR18−42038
RegionsHemisphere (L/R/B) MNI coordinates for voxel of maximal intensity
Number of voxels
xyz
Change from baseline: punishing condition
Orbitofrontal/subcallosal corticiesB1022−16445
Middle frontal gyrus/frontal poleL−483626318
Anterior cingulate/paracingulate corticesB24026221
Superior frontal gyrus/frontal poleB6524068
Middle frontal gyrusR3606049
Superior frontal gyrusB−4385646
Inferior frontal gyrusR60182241
Cingulate cortexB−1024036
Change from baseline: prosocial condition
Dorsal putamen/anterior insularL−26122445
Dorsal posterior putamenR28−81478
Lateral orbitofrontal cortexL−4428−272
Frontal medial cortex/frontal poleB856−1643
Caudate bodyR18−42038

Results uncorrected at P < 0.001 and at a cluster threshold of >35 contiguous voxels and reported at t > 3.3.

R = right; L = left; B = bilateral.

Discussion

We present a novel neuroeconomic task to investigate normative social decision-making behaviour in a neurodegenerative lesion model characterised by gross social dysfunction (behavioural variant FTD). For the first time, we describe the decision-making behaviour in behavioural variant FTD across three social normative scenarios: fairness, punishment and prosocial. Behaviourally, we demonstrate intact responses to fairness in the patients. In contrast, they show muted expression of punishing and prosocial behaviour, associated with discrete regions of fronto-striatal atrophy. Our findings suggest that whilst expression of basic normative behaviour may remain intact in behavioural variant FTD, more complex normative behaviours that rely on the integration of social contextual information are compromised.

Equivalent responses to fairness between patients and controls may, at first glance, seem difficult to reconcile, considering that core emotion processing regions known to underpin fairness behaviour are compromised in behavioural variant FTD. Previous investigations in patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesion, or during dietary serotonin depletion, indicate that emotion regulatory mechanisms interact with perceptions of unfairness. Accordingly, exaggerated emotional reactions lead to elevated rejection rates ( Koenigs and Tranel, 2007 ; Crockett et al. , 2008 , 2013 ; Shamay-Tsoory et al. , 2012 ). In contrast, the well-described blunting of emotional reactivity in behavioural variant FTD ( Sturm et al. , 2006 ) can explain why we do not see those same elevated rejection rates as patients with discrete prefrontal lesions, who often manifest exaggerated anger and irritability in frustrating situations ( Koenigs and Tranel, 2007 ). An interaction between emotional blunting and fairness behaviour has not been established in the literature. Our findings raise the possibility that fairness behaviour may not be similarly moderated by reduced emotional reactivity as it is by exaggerated emotional states. This supports that conclusion that perception of fairness is a dissociable construct from emotional state, and that it is maintained in behavioural variant FTD.

In contrast, patients differed from controls in terms of their engagement in either punishing or prosocial behaviour. These deviations from normative behaviour were associated with specific regions of fronto-striatal grey matter atrophy. In the punishing condition (i.e. framing proposers as rich to provoke an increased sense of unfairness, and encourage higher rejection rates), patients with behavioural variant FTD showed a different pattern to controls, by a tendency to accept more than their baseline levels. This contrasted with controls’ acceptance rates, which were more in keeping with their baseline levels—suggesting that controls’ responses were not modulated by information framing proposers as rich. The extent that patients’ acceptance rates were elevated relative to their baseline levels was correlated with atrophy particularly in ventromedial prefrontal cortex regions (medial orbitofrontal/subgenual cortex, mid/anterior cingulate) but also in the right inferior frontal gyrus and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Although the punishing condition did not induce punishment behaviour in controls, behavioural variant FTD patients’ tendency to accept more in this condition suggests a potential ‘misuse’ of social contextual information, whereby potentially negative social contextual information is not exploited to guide economic decisions. Such a finding is consistent with the commonly noticed financial gullibility of patients with behavioural variant FTD. Maladaptive financial decision-making in behavioural variant FTD is pervasive in both everyday life and experimental contexts ( Manes et al. , 2010 , 2011 ; Chiong et al. , 2014 ; Perry et al. , 2015 ). Extravagant spending, economic negligence and financial vulnerability can emerge long before a behavioural variant FTD diagnosis is achieved. A potentially compromised ability to integrate negative social cues with economic decisions warrants further investigation in behavioural variant FTD, as it may emerge as a critical area for early diagnostic assessment.

Our imaging findings for the punishing condition reveal a putative neural network that could underpin patients’ inability to effectively use negative social contextual information. The extent to which patients were less likely to engage in punishing behaviour was mediated by atrophy in regions at the intersection of emotion- and value-based social decision-making, namely the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and mid/anterior cingulate. This is consistent with the involvement of these regions in evaluating both reward and punishment, and integrating value with social and emotional information to inform subsequent behaviour ( Rushworth et al. , 2007 ; Etkin et al. , 2011 ; Apps et al. , 2013 ). The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, in particular, emerges as an important integrative hub for social behaviour, given its role in person perception and mentalizing ( Amodio and Frith, 2006 ).

We also show that the right inferior frontal gyrus, and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex atrophy related to a reduced tendency to engage in punishing behaviour. Bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation has been implicated in social norm compliance ( Spitzer et al. , 2007 ), and non-invasive brain stimulation to disrupt the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex alters social norm compliance in economic decision-making ( Knoch et al. , 2006 ; Ruff et al. , 2013 ). One study using the Ultimatum Game demonstrated a significant decline in rejection rates of unfair offers after transcranial magnetic stimulation to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which induced decreased activity of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and posterior ventromedial cortex, and connectivity between them ( Baumgartner et al. , 2011 ). Our results corroborate that combined ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex atrophy is associated with an unwillingness to engage in punishing behaviour. In the context of the Ultimatum Game, rejecting unfair offers entails a more immediate inhibition of self-interest, as monetary gain is foregone in order to penalize the proposer’s violation of the fairness norm. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has been linked to the ability to override self-interest via functional imaging and electrical stimulation studies ( Rilling and Sanfey, 2011 ), using a structural lesion model we highlight that the right inferior frontal gyrus may be a critical locus involved in this process. The right inferior frontal gyrus is implicated in various forms of inhibitory control ( Aron, 2011 ). Inhibitory dysfunction is well described in behavioural variant FTD ( O'Callaghan et al. , 2013 a ) and has been linked to orbitofrontal and inferior frontal cortex abnormalities ( Peters et al. , 2006 ; Hornberger et al. , 2011 ; O'Callaghan et al. , 2013 b ; Hughes et al. , 2015 ). Together, the structural neural correlates we describe here converge across circuitry involved in inhibition and emotion/reward processing, confirming a role for this network in mediating alterations to normative social function in behavioural variant FTD.

In terms of the prosocial condition (i.e. to frame proposers as being ‘in need’ to encourage higher acceptance rates), patients showed a tendency toward prosocial behaviour, albeit to a lesser extent than controls. The extent to which patients deviated from normative responses in this condition, by accepting less than their baseline levels, was associated with atrophy in dorsal striatal regions, left anterior insula and left lateral orbitofrontal cortex. A proximate mechanism for prosocial behaviour is thought to be the subjective reward derived from helping another ( Fehr and Fischbacher, 2003 ). In line with our finding that dorsal striatum atrophy mediated the muted prosocial response, functional activation studies implicate the striatum in social reward processing ( Fehr and Camerer, 2007 ; Bhanji and Delgado, 2014 ), although the exact subdivisions associated with social rewards have not been established. Currently, both dorsal and ventral striatum have been linked to social reward valuation and learning, vicarious social reward, and signalling reward inequality ( Báez-Mendoza and Schultz, 2013 ). However, convergent evidence from non-social paradigms implicates the dorsal striatum in signalling reward- and motivation-based information that drives subsequent action selection ( O'Doherty, 2004 ; O'Doherty et al. , 2004 ; Balleine et al. , 2007 ). Taken together, the dorsal striatum may play a unique role in computing social reward and biasing action-output geared to seek rewarding social interactions.

Activation in the anterior insula has consistently been linked with Ultimatum Game performance in healthy people ( Sanfey et al. , 2003 ; Tabibnia et al. , 2008 ; Civai et al. , 2012 ; Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al. , 2013 ). A strong association between the anterior insula and negative emotional states ( Damasio et al. , 2000 ) was taken as evidence that this region mediated the negative emotional reactions that led to rejecting unfair offers. However, an alternative interpretation is that the insula has a broader role in both detecting inequality and motivating decisions to restore inequality ( Rilling and Sanfey, 2011 ; Gabay et al. , 2014 ). Our findings show that anterior insula damage may not necessarily affect normative responses to fairness, but that it is related to a reduced expression of prosocial behaviour, consistent with its hypothesized broader role. In keeping with our results, both the left lateral orbitofrontal cortex and right insula have been implicated in norm-abiding social behaviour ( Spitzer et al. , 2007 ).

The expression of normative social behaviours relevant to explore in behavioural variant FTD, as the human tendency to engage in fair, prosocial behaviour offers a window into one of the cardinal symptoms of the disease—reduced empathy. Our findings show that patients with lower levels of everyday empathic concern were more likely to reject unfair offers. Rather than being consistent with an elevated emotional response to unfairness or insensitivity to reward, we would suggest that this finding supports a diminished response to normative social expectations. This is consistent with a recent study illustrating that, in healthy individuals, trait levels of empathic concern predicts the tendency to engage in altruistic behaviour during a social interaction paradigm ( FeldmanHall et al. , 2015 ). Objective assessment of empathy continues to pose a challenge in behavioural variant FTD; however, our findings suggest that normative social behaviours on the Ultimatum Game may provide a useful surrogate for everyday empathy. As our current results are based on a single, clinical assessment of empathy, it will be important for future studies to undertake a more in-depth assessment of empathy. Given that empathy is a complex, multifaceted process that also draws upon other abilities that can be affected by dementia including language, motivation and mentalization ( Decety et al. , 2012 ), future studies should explore the association between such abilities, different aspects of empathy, and how they relate to the expression of normative behaviour in neuropsychiatric conditions.

Considering our findings across the three scenarios of social norm compliance, expression of normative behaviour in the patients was not uniformly impaired. Engaging purely in fairness-based behaviour on the Ultimatum Game does not involve complex social computations. In contrast, engaging in prosocial or punishing behaviour relies on the effective integration of social contextual information to guide behaviour. Our finding that normative behaviour in the patients was disrupted when additional social processing was required is consistent with a recent hypothesis proposing that a range of symptoms in behavioural variant FTD may be underscored by a generalized deficit in the ability to effectively integrate social context and behaviour ( Ibañez and Manes, 2012 ).

A potential limitation of our study design is that we did not include a control condition in the Ultimatum Game that incorporated additional information that was non-social, thereby being unable to directly distinguish whether the effects seen in the reappraisal conditions stemmed from social norm deficits, or simply difficulty in incorporating extra information. However, the inclusion of cognitive variables in our main analysis allowed us to control for the effects of cognitive dysfunction. Our results indicated that generalized cognitive dysfunction did not mediate patients’ divergent performance in the social framing conditions—emphasizing that our results are consistent with a specific deficit in integrating social context into decision-making. Nevertheless, future studies in patients with behavioural variant FTD, and indeed in other neurodegenerative groups, which are matched for gender to control for potential heterogeneity within the patient sample, are necessary to replicate and extend the results we describe here.

In conclusion, we have developed a novel neuroeconomic task to provide insights into complex social dysfunction in a neurodegenerative lesion model (behavioural variant FTD). In doing so, we have identified discrete deficits in patients’ ability to integrate social contextual information to guide normative decision-making behaviour, associated with abnormalities in key fronto-striatal regions. This ‘social norm compliance’ network represents an important target for future research into disordered norm compliance in behavioural variant FTD. From a wider theoretical standpoint, these findings speak to on-going appeals that norm-based decision-making research be extended to clinical populations ( Ruff et al. , 2013 ; Sanfey et al. , 2014 ), in an effort to determine causal mechanisms of social norm compliance, and its relevance to neuropsychiatric symptomatology across conditions.

Funding

C.O. is supported by an Alzheimer’s Australia Dementia Research Foundation PhD scholarship. M.B. is supported by a Marie Sklodowska-Curie Intra European Fellowship. J.M.S. is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council CJ Martin Fellowship. L.S. is supported by the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation (Research Fellowship for Experienced Researchers) and a University of New South Wales Vice-Chancellor’s Postdoctoral Research Fellowship. M.I., J.R.H. and M.H. are supported by the Australian Research Council (DE130100463 to M.I., FF0776229 to J.R.H. and DP110104202 to M.H.). M.H. is also supported by Alzheimer’s Research UK and the Newton Trust. This work was supported by funding to Forefront, a collaborative research group dedicated to the study of frontotemporal dementia and motor neurone disease, from the National Health and Medical research Council of Australia program grant (1037746) and the Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders Memory Node (CE110001021).

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain online.

Abbreviations

    Abbreviations
     
  • FTD

    frontotemporal dementia

  •  
  • RAVLT

    Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

References

Amodio
DM
Frith
CD
.
Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
2006
;
7
:
268
77
.

Andersson
JLR
Jenkinson
M
Smith
S
.
Non-linear optimisation
.
FMRIB technical report TR07JA1. Available from: www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep
;
2007a
.

Andersson
JLR
Jenkinson
M
Smith
S
.
Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation. FMRIB technical report TR07JA2. from, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep
;
2007b
.

Apps
MA
Lockwood
PL
Balsters
JH
.
The role of the midcingulate cortex in monitoring others' decisions
.
Front Neurosci
2013
;
7
:
251
.

Aron
AR
.
From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses
.
Biol Psychiatry
2011
;
69
:
e55
68
.

Ashburner
J
Friston
KJ
.
Voxel-based morphometry—the methods
.
Neuroimage
2000
;
11
(
6 Pt 1
):
805
21
.

Baez
S
Manes
F
Huepe
D
Torralva
T
Fiorentino
N
Richter
F
et al. .
Primary empathy deficits in frontotemporal dementia
.
Front Aging Neurosci
2014
;
6
:
262
.

Báez-Mendoza
R
Schultz
W
.
The role of the striatum in social behavior
.
Front Neurosci
2013
;
7
:
233
.

Balleine
BW
Delgado
MR
Hikosaka
O
.
The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making
.
J Neurosci
2007
;
27
:
8161
5
.

Baumgartner
T
Knoch
D
Hotz
P
Eisenegger
C
Fehr
E
.
Dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex orchestrate normative choice
.
Nat Neurosci
2011
;
14
:
1468
74
.

Bertoux
M
Funkiewiez
A
O’Callaghan
C
Dubois
B
Hornberger
M
.
Sensitivity and specificity of ventromedial prefrontal cortex tests in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Alzheimers Dement
2013
;
9
(
5 Suppl
):
S84
94
.

Bertoux
M
O'Callaghan
C
Flanagan
E
Hodges
JR
Hornberger
M
.
Fronto-striatal atrophy in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia & Alzheimer’s disease
.
Front Neurol
2015
;
6
:
147
.

Bhanji
JP
Delgado
MR
.
The social brain and reward: social information processing in the human striatum
.
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci
2014
;
5
:
61
73
.

Broe
M
Hodges
JR
Schofield
E
Shepherd
CE
Kril
JJ
Halliday
GM
.
Staging disease severity in pathologically confirmed cases of frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
2003
;
60
:
1005
11
.

Buckholtz
JW
Marois
R
.
The roots of modern justice: cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcement
.
Nat Neurosci
2012
;
15
:
655
61
.

Castelli
I
Massaro
D
Bicchieri
C
Chavez
A
Marchetti
A
.
Fairness norms and theory of mind in an ultimatum game: judgments, offers, and decisions in school-aged children
.
PLoS One
2014
;
9
:
e105024
.

Chang
LJ
Sanfey
AG
.
Great expectations: neural computations underlying the use of social norms in decision-making
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
2013
;
8
:
277
84
.

Chiong
W
Hsu
M
Wudka
D
Miller
BL
Rosen
HJ
.
Financial errors in dementia: Testing a neuroeconomic conceptual framework
.
Neurocase
2014
;
20
:
389
96
.

Civai
C
Crescentini
C
Rustichini
A
Rumiati
RI
.
Equality versus self-interest in the brain: differential roles of anterior insula and medial prefrontal cortex
.
Neuroimage
2012
;
62
:
102
12
.

Corradi-Dell'Acqua
C
Civai
C
Rumiati
RI
Fink
GR
.
Disentangling self-and fairness-related neural mechanisms involved in the ultimatum game: an fMRI study
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
2013
;
8
:
424
31
.

Crockett
MJ
Apergis-Schoute
A
Herrmann
B
Lieberman
MD
Müller
U
Robbins
TW
et al. .
Serotonin modulates striatal responses to fairness and retaliation in humans
.
J Neurosci
2013
;
33
:
3505
13
.

Crockett
MJ
Clark
L
Tabibnia
G
Lieberman
MD
Robbins
TW
.
Serotonin modulates behavioral reactions to unfairness
.
Science
2008
;
320
:
1739
.

Croson
R
Gneezy
U
.
Gender differences in preferences
.
J Econ Lit
2009
;
47
:
448
74
.

Damasio
AR
Grabowski
TJ
Bechara
A
Damasio
H
Ponto
LL
Parvizi
J
et al. .
Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions
.
Nat Neurosci
2000
;
3
:
1049
56
.

Decety
J
Norman
GJ
Berntson
GG
Cacioppo
JT
.
A neurobehavioral evolutionary perspective on the mechanisms underlying empathy
.
Prog Neurobiol
2012
;
98
:
38
48
.

Etkin
A
Egner
T
Kalisch
R
.
Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex
.
Trends Cogn Sci
2011
;
15
:
85
93
.

Fehr
E
Bernhard
H
Rockenbach
B
.
Egalitarianism in young children
.
Nature
2008
;
454
:
1079
83
.

Fehr
E
Camerer
CF
.
Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences
.
Trends Cogn Sci
2007
;
11
:
419
27
.

Fehr
E
Fischbacher
U
.
The nature of human altruism
.
Nature
2003
;
425
:
785
91
.

Fehr
E
Fischbacher
U
.
Third-party punishment and social norms
.
Evol Hum Behav
2004
;
25
:
63
87
.

FeldmanHall
O
Dalgleish
T
Evans
D
Mobbs
D
.
Empathic concern drives costly altruism
.
Neuroimage
2015
;
105
:
347
56
.

Gabay
AS
Radua
J
Kempton
MJ
Mehta
MA
.
The Ultimatum Game and the brain: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
2014
;
47
:
549
58
.

Gleichgerrcht
E
Ibáñez
A
Roca
M
Torralva
T
Manes
F
.
Decision-making cognition in neurodegenerative diseases
.
Nat Rev Neurol
2010
;
6
:
611
23
.

Good
CD
Johnsrude
IS
Ashburner
J
Henson
RN
Friston
KJ
Frackowiak
RS
.
A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains
.
Neuroimage
2001
;
14
(
1 Pt 1
):
21
36
.

Güth
W
Schmittberger
R
Schwarze
B
.
An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining
.
J Econ Behav Organ
1982
;
3
:
367
88
.

Hornberger
M
Geng
J
Hodges
JR
.
Convergent grey and white matter evidence of orbitofrontal cortex changes related to disinhibition in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
2011
;
134
:
2502
12
.

Hughes
LE
Rittman
T
Regenthal
R
Robbins
TW
Rowe
JB
.
Improving response inhibition systems in frontotemporal dementia with citalopram
.
Brain
2015
.
Advance Access published on May 22, 2015, doi:

Ibañez
A
Manes
F
.
Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
2012
;
78
:
1354
62
.

Irish
M
Hodges
JR
Piguet
O
.
Right anterior temporal lobe dysfunction underlies theory of mind impairments in semantic dementia
.
Brain
2014
;
137
:
1241
53
.

Jaeger
TF
.
Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models
.
J Mem Lang
2008
;
59
:
434
46
.

Kloeters
S
Bertoux
M
O'Callaghan
C
Hodges
JR
Hornberger
M
.
Money for nothing — Atrophy correlates of gambling decision making in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease
.
Neuroimage Clin
2013
;
2
:
263
72
.

Knoch
D
Pascual-Leone
A
Meyer
K
Treyer
V
Fehr
E
.
Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex
.
Science
2006
;
314
:
829
32
.

Koenigs
M
Tranel
D
.
Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: evidence from the ultimatum game
.
J Neurosci
2007
;
27
:
951
6
.

Lezak
MD
Howieson
DB
Loring
DW
.
Neuropsychological assessment
. 4th edn .
New York
:
Oxford University Press
;
2004
.

Manes
F
Torralva
T
Ibáñez
A
Roca
M
Bekinschtein
T
Gleichgerrcht
E
.
Decision-making in frontotemporal dementia: clinical, theoretical and legal implications
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
2011
;
32
:
11
7
.

Manes
F
Torralva
T
Roca
M
Gleichgerrcht
E
Bekinschtein
TA
Hodges
JR
.
Frontotemporal dementia presenting as pathological gambling
.
Nat Rev Neurol
2010
;
6
:
347
52
.

Meyers
J
.
The Meyers scoring system for the Rey complex figure and the recognition trial: professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources
;
1995
.

Mioshi
E
Hsieh
S
Savage
S
Hornberger
M
Hodges
J
.
Clinical staging and disease progression in frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
2010
;
74
:
1591
7
.

Neary
D
Snowden
JS
Gustafson
L
Passant
U
Stuss
D
Black
S
et al. .
Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria
.
Neurology
1998
;
51
:
1546
54
.

Nichols
TE
Holmes
AP
.
Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples
.
Hum Brain Mapp
2002
;
15
:
1
25
.

O'Callaghan
C
Bertoux
M
Hornberger
M
.
Beyond and below the cortex: the contribution of striatal dysfunction to cognition and behaviour in neurodegeneration
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
2014
;
85
:
371
8
.

O'Callaghan
C
Hodges
JR
Hornberger
M
.
Inhibitory dysfunction in frontotemporal dementia: a review
.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
2013a
;
27
:
102
8
.

O'Callaghan
C
Naismith
SL
Hodges
JR
Lewis
SJG
Hornberger
M
.
Fronto-striatal atrophy correlates of inhibitory dysfunction in Parkinson's disease versus behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Cortex
2013b
;
49
:
1833
43
.

O'Doherty
J
.
Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
2004
;
14
:
769
76
.

O'Doherty
J
Dayan
P
Schultz
J
Deichmann
R
Friston
K
Dolan
RJ
.
Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning
.
Science
2004
;
304
:
452
4
.

Perry
DC
Sturm
VE
Wood
KA
Miller
BL
Kramer
JH
.
Divergent processing of monetary and social reward in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease
.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Dsord
2015
;
29
:
161
4
.

Peters
F
Perani
D
Herholz
K
Holthoff
V
Beuthien-Baumann
B
Sorbi
S
et al. .
Orbitofrontal dysfunction related to both apathy and disinhibition in frontotemporal dementia
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
2006
;
21
:
373
9
.

Piguet
O
Hornberger
M
Mioshi
E
Hodges
JR
.
Behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: diagnosis, clinical staging, and management
.
Lancet Neurol
2011
;
10
:
162
72
.

Rascovsky
K
Hodges
JR
Knopman
D
Mendez
MF
Kramer
JH
Neuhaus
J
et al. .
Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
2011
;
134
:
2456
77
.

Rilling
JK
Sanfey
AG
.
The neuroscience of social decision-making
.
Annu Rev Psychol
2011
;
62
:
23
48
.

Rueckert
D
Sonoda
LI
Hayes
C
Hill
DL
Leach
MO
Hawkes
DJ
.
Nonrigid registration using free-form deformations: application to breast MR images
.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging
1999
;
18
:
712
21
.

Ruff
C
Ugazio
G
Fehr
E
.
Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulation
.
Science
2013
;
342
:
482
4
.

Ruff
CC
Fehr
E
.
The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
2014
;
15
:
549
62
.

Rushworth
M
Behrens
T
Rudebeck
P
Walton
M
.
Contrasting roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in decisions and social behaviour
.
Trends Cogn Sci
2007
;
11
:
168
76
.

Sanfey
AG
Rilling
JK
Aronson
JA
Nystrom
LE
Cohen
JD
.
The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game
.
Science
2003
;
300
:
1755
8
.

Sanfey
AG
Stallen
M
Chang
LJ
.
Norms and expectations in social decision-making
.
Trends Cogn Sci
2014
;
18
:
172
4
.

Seeley
WW
Crawford
R
Rascovsky
K
Kramer
JH
Weiner
M
Miller
BL
et al. .
Frontal paralimbic network atrophy in very mild behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Arch Neurol
2008
;
65
:
2249
55
.

Shamay-Tsoory
SG
Suleiman
R
Aharon-Peretz
J
Gohary
R
Hirschberger
G
.
Sensitivity to fairness and intentions of others in the ultimatum game in patients with ventromedial prefontal lesions
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc
2012
;
18
:
952
61
.

Smith
SM
Jenkinson
M
Woolrich
MW
Beckmann
CF
Behrens
TE
Johansen-Berg
H
et al. .
Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL
.
Neuroimage
2004
;
23
(
Suppl 1
):
S208
19
.

Smith
SM
Nichols
TE
.
Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference
.
Neuroimage
2009
;
44
:
83
98
.

Sollberger
M
Stanley
CM
Wilson
SM
Gyurak
A
Beckman
V
Growdon
M
et al. .
Neural basis of interpersonal traits in neurodegenerative diseases
.
Neuropsychologia
2009
;
47
:
2812
27
.

Spitzer
M
Fischbacher
U
Herrnberger
B
Grön
G
Fehr
E
.
The neural signature of social norm compliance
.
Neuron
2007
;
56
:
185
96
.

Strauss
E
Sherman
EM
Spreen
O
.
A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary
.
Oxford University Press, USA
;
2006
.

Sturm
VE
Rosen
HJ
Allison
S
Miller
BL
Levenson
RW
.
Self-conscious emotion deficits in frontotemporal lobar degeneration
.
Brain
2006
;
129
:
2508
16
.

Tabibnia
G
Satpute
AB
Lieberman
MD
.
The sunny side of fairness preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry)
.
Psychol Sci
2008
;
19
:
339
47
.

Wear
HJ
Wedderburn
CJ
Mioshi
E
Williams-Gray
CH
Mason
SL
Barker
RA
et al. .
The Cambridge Behavioral Inventory revised
.
Dement Neuropsychol
2008
;
2
:
102
7
.

Wechsler
D
.
Wechsler adult intelligence scale-III
.
San Antonio
:
The Psychological Corporation
;
1997
.

Zhang
Y
Brady
M
Smith
S
.
Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm
.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging
2001
;
20
:
45
57
.

Supplementary data