Abstract

Physical exercise can convey a protective effect against cognitive decline in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. While the long-term health-promoting and protective effects of exercise are encouraging, it’s potential to induce neuronal and vascular plasticity in the ageing brain is still poorly understood. It remains unclear whether exercise slows the trajectory of normal ageing by modifying vascular and metabolic risk factors and/or consistently boosts brain function by inducing structural and neurochemical changes in the hippocampus and related medial temporal lobe circuitry—brain areas that are important for learning and memory. Hence, it remains to be established to what extent exercise interventions in old age can improve brain plasticity above and beyond preservation of function. Existing data suggest that exercise trials aiming for improvement and preservation may require different outcome measures and that the balance between the two may depend on exercise intensity and duration, the presence of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease pathology, vascular and metabolic risk factors and genetic variability.

 Physical activity can offer protection against cognitive decline and neurodegenerative diseases, but whether it slows the trajectory of normal ageing or boosts plasticity above and beyond preservation of function is unclear. Duzel et al. examine the evidence that exercise improves hippocampal plasticity in ageing and discuss avenues for future research.

Physical activity can offer protection against cognitive decline and neurodegenerative diseases, but whether it slows the trajectory of normal ageing or boosts plasticity above and beyond preservation of function is unclear. Duzel et al. examine the evidence that exercise improves hippocampal plasticity in ageing and discuss avenues for future research.

Introduction

Physical inactivity is an important risk factor for cognitive decline in ageing and for Alzheimer’s disease ( Norton et al. , 2014 ). Conversely, exercise can convey a protective effect ( Ahlskog et al. , 2011 ; Geda et al. , 2012 ; Wirth et al. , 2014 ; Ngandu et al. , 2015 ; Prakash et al. , 2015 ; Tolppanen et al. , 2015 ) even if initiated after midlife ( Tolppanen et al. , 2015 ). In this article we summarize current knowledge regarding exercise-induced hippocampal neural plasticity in animals and humans with a focus on old age. Animal research of normal ageing and models of Alzheimer’s disease generally support the notion that physical activity can prevent loss of neuroplasticity and maintain memory function. However, randomized controlled trials in older humans indicate that memory improvements after exercise interventions are highly variable. Understanding the possible sources of this variability will be important for tailored clinical prescriptions of exercise interventions, for preventing side-effects and treatment failures, and enhancing efficacy.

We outline how research in this field can benefit from recent insights into the functional anatomical and cognitive organization of hippocampal circuits ( Fig. 1 ). This brain region is critical for episodic memory, the ability to memorize and remember unique events and experiences (for review see Eichenbaum et al. , 2007 ). Together with its medial temporal lobe and subcortical circuits ( Fig. 1 A and B) the hippocampus is compromised early in ageing and neurodegenerative conditions, most notably in Alzheimer’s disease and fronto-temporal dementias ( Tan et al. , 2014 ; Braak and Del Tredici, 2015 ). We also consider how exercise may interact with preclinical Alzheimer’s disease pathology, vascular and metabolic risk factors and genetic variability. Finally, we highlight questions that can be addressed in future studies.

Brain regions and networks that are affected by exercise. ( A ) Subfields of the hippocampus (HC) and adjacent medial temporal lobe. The entorhinal cortex (EC) is a major gateway for hippocampus (for review see  van Strien et al. , 2009  ). The dentate gyrus (DG) seems to be important for pattern separation of novel information (  Leutgeb et al. , 2007  ; Neunuebel and Knierim, 2014 ), whereas CA3 (not visible in this section) is implicated in pattern completion. Pattern separation dissociates different memories to non-overlapping neural codes so that they are not confused with each other even if similar. Pattern completion, on the other hand, associates different memories to overlapping and linked neural codes such that a full episodic experience can be remembered from a partial memory ( Neunuebel and Knierim, 2014 ). CA1 probably remaps memory representations back to their cortical topography ( Kumaran and McClelland, 2012 ). ( B ) The entorhinal cortex receives inputs about object representations from perirhinal (PRC) and space/scene representations from parahippocampal cortices (PHC) (  Witter et al. , 2014  ). In humans, the anterior-lateral (al) entorhinal cortex is functionally associated with the perirhinal cortex (  Maass et al. , 2015 a ) and object processing (  Navarro Schroder et al. , 2015  ) whereas the posterior-medial (pm) entorhinal cortex is functionally associated with the parahippocampal cortex (  Maass et al. , 2015 a ) and scene processing (  Navarro Schroder et al. , 2015  ). The mammillary bodies (MB) are a major subcortical hub for the hippocampus. Trans-EC = transentorhinal cortex, a transition zone between lateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex affected early in Alzheimer’s disease by tau pathology. MB = mammillary bodies. Atrophy of the mammillary bodies can be seen in ∼60% of patients with Alzheimer’s disease (  Hornberger et al. , 2012  ) and correlates with impaired recollection (  Tsivilis et al. , 2008  ;  Vann et al. , 2009  ). In animals running can enhance the connectivity between the mammillary bodies and new neurons in the dentate gyrus (  Vivar et al. , 2015  ). ( C ) Running increases adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus and improves discrimination between similar stimuli (such as the two brown conical shapes on either side of the mouse). In the background is a photomicrograph of a coronal section through the mouse dentate gyrus double-labelled with the neuronal nuclei marker NeuN (red) and bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU, green), a thymidine analogue that labels dividing cells. Cells that show an overlap of red and green labels are considered to be newly born neurons in the adult mouse brain. ( D ) Voxel-based multiple regression analyses between changes in hippocampal regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) changes after exercise in old age and changes in hippocampal grey matter density (with 7 T MRI, adapted from  Maass et al. , 2015 b ). Sub = subiculum.
Figure 1

Brain regions and networks that are affected by exercise. ( A ) Subfields of the hippocampus (HC) and adjacent medial temporal lobe. The entorhinal cortex (EC) is a major gateway for hippocampus (for review see van Strien et al. , 2009 ). The dentate gyrus (DG) seems to be important for pattern separation of novel information ( Leutgeb et al. , 2007 ; Neunuebel and Knierim, 2014 ), whereas CA3 (not visible in this section) is implicated in pattern completion. Pattern separation dissociates different memories to non-overlapping neural codes so that they are not confused with each other even if similar. Pattern completion, on the other hand, associates different memories to overlapping and linked neural codes such that a full episodic experience can be remembered from a partial memory ( Neunuebel and Knierim, 2014 ). CA1 probably remaps memory representations back to their cortical topography ( Kumaran and McClelland, 2012 ). ( B ) The entorhinal cortex receives inputs about object representations from perirhinal (PRC) and space/scene representations from parahippocampal cortices (PHC) ( Witter et al. , 2014 ). In humans, the anterior-lateral (al) entorhinal cortex is functionally associated with the perirhinal cortex ( Maass et al. , 2015 a ) and object processing ( Navarro Schroder et al. , 2015 ) whereas the posterior-medial (pm) entorhinal cortex is functionally associated with the parahippocampal cortex ( Maass et al. , 2015 a ) and scene processing ( Navarro Schroder et al. , 2015 ). The mammillary bodies (MB) are a major subcortical hub for the hippocampus. Trans-EC = transentorhinal cortex, a transition zone between lateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex affected early in Alzheimer’s disease by tau pathology. MB = mammillary bodies. Atrophy of the mammillary bodies can be seen in ∼60% of patients with Alzheimer’s disease ( Hornberger et al. , 2012 ) and correlates with impaired recollection ( Tsivilis et al. , 2008 ; Vann et al. , 2009 ). In animals running can enhance the connectivity between the mammillary bodies and new neurons in the dentate gyrus ( Vivar et al. , 2015 ). ( C ) Running increases adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus and improves discrimination between similar stimuli (such as the two brown conical shapes on either side of the mouse). In the background is a photomicrograph of a coronal section through the mouse dentate gyrus double-labelled with the neuronal nuclei marker NeuN (red) and bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU, green), a thymidine analogue that labels dividing cells. Cells that show an overlap of red and green labels are considered to be newly born neurons in the adult mouse brain. ( D ) Voxel-based multiple regression analyses between changes in hippocampal regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) changes after exercise in old age and changes in hippocampal grey matter density (with 7 T MRI, adapted from Maass et al. , 2015 b ). Sub = subiculum.

Neural plasticity after exercise in animals

Voluntary wheel running upregulates expression of BDNF, IGF1 and VEGF (encoded by VEGFA ) and modulates plasticity in the hippocampus and cortex ( Box 2 ) (for reviews see Cotman et al. , 2007 ; Vivar et al. , 2013 ; Prakash et al. , 2015 ; Sleiman and Chao, 2015 ). BDNF is a major regulator of synaptic plasticity ( Park and Poo, 2013 ) and lowering BDNF levels results in dysfunction, such as decreased synaptic plasticity not only in the hippocampus ( Korte et al. , 1995 ; Patterson et al. , 1996 ) but also other brain regions that are affected during ageing such as cortex, the extra-hippocampal limbic system and the striatum ( Rauskolb et al. , 2010 ). In mice lacking BDNF in the postnatal brain, survival of hippocampal neurons is not affected ( Rauskolb et al. , 2010 ). Thus exercise-induced upregulation of BDNF expression in hippocampal and cortical neurons ( Neeper et al. , 1995 ; 1996 ; Hopkins and Bucci, 2010 ) may promote synaptic remodelling and morphological complexity rather than cell survival.

Box 1 Preserving and improving brain function

Several studies provided cross-sectional support for the long-term preserving benefits of a physically lifestyle for the ageing brain ( Colcombe et al. , 2003 ; Erickson and Kramer, 2009 ; Honea et al. , 2009 ; Flöel et al. , 2010 ; Bugg and Head, 2011 ; Ho et al. , 2011 ; Weinstein et al. , 2012 ; Benedict et al. , 2013 ).

•  What is the optimal exercise regime?

Preliminary data in humans and evidence from animal studies suggest that improving brain function would require 3–6 months of moderate to high intensity training. In contrast, preservation of function can be best demonstrated with prolonged regimes where mild-to-moderate levels are more practical. A combination of both approaches seems feasible but has not yet been tested.

Exercise regimeDuration
Preserving function Mild-to-moderate intensity, 50–70% of max. cardiac output, ≥3 × weeks, 30–40-min sessions ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ) ≥12 months
Improving function Moderate-to-high >75% of max. cardiac output, ≥3 × weeks, 30–40-min sessions with high intensity intervals of 4 × 5 min ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ) 3 to 6 months
Combined approachLong-term mild-to-moderate intensity training with brief intervals (for instance 1 week every 4 weeks) of moderate-to-high intensity≥12 months
Exercise regimeDuration
Preserving function Mild-to-moderate intensity, 50–70% of max. cardiac output, ≥3 × weeks, 30–40-min sessions ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ) ≥12 months
Improving function Moderate-to-high >75% of max. cardiac output, ≥3 × weeks, 30–40-min sessions with high intensity intervals of 4 × 5 min ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ) 3 to 6 months
Combined approachLong-term mild-to-moderate intensity training with brief intervals (for instance 1 week every 4 weeks) of moderate-to-high intensity≥12 months
Exercise regimeDuration
Preserving function Mild-to-moderate intensity, 50–70% of max. cardiac output, ≥3 × weeks, 30–40-min sessions ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ) ≥12 months
Improving function Moderate-to-high >75% of max. cardiac output, ≥3 × weeks, 30–40-min sessions with high intensity intervals of 4 × 5 min ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ) 3 to 6 months
Combined approachLong-term mild-to-moderate intensity training with brief intervals (for instance 1 week every 4 weeks) of moderate-to-high intensity≥12 months
Exercise regimeDuration
Preserving function Mild-to-moderate intensity, 50–70% of max. cardiac output, ≥3 × weeks, 30–40-min sessions ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ) ≥12 months
Improving function Moderate-to-high >75% of max. cardiac output, ≥3 × weeks, 30–40-min sessions with high intensity intervals of 4 × 5 min ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ) 3 to 6 months
Combined approachLong-term mild-to-moderate intensity training with brief intervals (for instance 1 week every 4 weeks) of moderate-to-high intensity≥12 months

•  What are the optimal cognitive outcome markers?

It is unclear to what extent the memory effects of exercise in old age depend on modality (verbal, objects, spatial), retrieval demands (recall versus recognition) and delay (early versus late) and to what extent tests of spatial navigation are sensitive (analogous to spatial navigation in rodents; Fig. 2 ). Systematic comparisons of memory tasks that tap into pattern separation and pattern completion have not yet been conducted. The consequences on cognitive performance would be important; whereas pattern separation would improve the accuracy and precision of visual memory particularly in tests of recognition memory and visual discrimination, pattern completion would improve tests of recollection and recall. The following patterns of cognitive outcomes can be hypothesized:

  • Improving function in moderate-to-high intensity interventions:

    • (i) Induction of circuit-specific plasticity improves circuit-specific memory processes such as pattern separation.

    •  (ii) Improving insulin resistance and thereby brain glucose metabolism improves function of different circuits and can therefore affect memory tasks that rely on distributed networks such as verbal delayed recall

  • Preserving function in prolonged mild-to-moderate intensity intervention:

    • (i) Modifying risk factors of cognitive decline helps preserving brain function and this is associated with stable maintenance of memory tasks that rely on distributed networks

•  Intermediate outcomes measures to detect adverse effects

For a timely identification of adverse effects of moderate-to-intensive exercise (drop in memory performance, drop in hippocampal perfusion) it seems necessary to use intermediate (i.e. 1–3 months) outcome markers, such as cognitive tests (i.e. those that tap into pattern separation) and non-invasive magnetic resonance-based perfusion measures such as arterial spin labelling (ASL). Perfusion measures immediately after acute exercise may yield information about vascular reactivity ( Lucas et al. , 2015 ). In one study ( Ruscheweyh et al. , 2011 ), a substantial proportion of older adults showed cortical volume loss after 6 months of moderate exercise training despite of a positive correlation between fitness change and volume.

The inconsistent findings for growth factor levels suggest that they may not be reliable to monitor efficacy. In individuals who react adversely to moderate-to-intense exercise it could be advisable to focus on preservation of function and continue with mild exercise in combination with other lifestyle modifications such as dietary and cognitive interventions ( Ngandu et al. , 2015 ).

•  Recommended intensity of exercise in old age

The World Health Organization ( WHO, 2012 ) recommends vigorous exercise levels in old age (75 min per week) that are similar to those levels with which some older adults show hippocampal perfusion and volume decreases ( Maass et al. , 2014 ). With the advance of neuroimaging methodology, it seems necessary to accumulate more data on potential physiological side effects of exercise on brain structure and function and adjust these recommendations accordingly.

•  Standardizing randomized controlled proof-of-concept exercise trials

Prescription of exercise interventions in individuals at risk of cognitive decline will require a better understanding of the efficacy and risk profiles of different exercise regimes. To achieve this, it seems important that the field agrees on standardized core protocols for imaging, cognition and biomarker sampling. For instance, the Rey-Complex Figure recall and recognition test ( Fig. 2 ) has been used in both cross-sectional (indexing preservation) ( Brown et al. , 2012 ) and interventional ( Maass et al. , 2014 ; 2015 a ) studies.

Box 2 The role of the muscle–brain axis

Substantial knowledge has been accumulated about the changes that exercise evokes in the brain in rodents and humans. However, the systemic, metabolic peripheral triggers that elicit these brain processes are virtually unknown. Recent research indicates that blood-borne systemic factors from young animals can counteract the age-related decline of adult neurogenesis and brain function ( Villeda et al. , 2014 ). On activation by exercise, skeletal muscle may release such factors into the circulation to communicate with the brain. In addition, pharmacological activation and transgenic overexpression of muscle energy metabolism pathways can to some extent mimic effects of exercise on the brain ( Kobilo et al. , 2011 ; Wrann et al. , 2013 ; Agudelo et al. , 2014 ; Moon and van Praag, 2014 ; Sleiman and Chao, 2015 ). As not everyone can exercise due to disease circumstances, or ageing senescence-related frailty, identification of peripheral factors and mechanisms that elicit the effects of exercise on the brain may lead to therapeutics that maintain and improve cognition.

•  IGF1 (a Janus-faced molecule)

Growth factors in the periphery and brain have been closely linked to neural plasticity. These include the neurotrophin family ( Dekkers et al. , 2013 ; Park and Poo, 2013 ), with brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), the neurocytokine family with ciliary neurotrophic factor (CNTF) and leukaemia inhibitory factor (LIF) ( Sendtner, 2014 ) as well as pluripotent growth factors such as insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF1) and the vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) family, hypoxia-inducible proteins promoting the formation and growth of blood vessels and neurogenesis ( Greenberg and Jin, 2005 ). Reduced levels of IGF1 in the ageing brain have been proposed to contribute to age-associated decline in cognitive functions ( Aleman and Torres-Aleman, 2009 ) and circulating IGF1 levels in healthy older adults show a modest positive correlation with hippocampal volumes and verbal free recall performance ( Maass et al. , 2015 b ). During physical exercise, IGF1 levels in skeletal muscle are rapidly upregulated and peak transiently at ∼5–10 min of training ( Berg and Bang, 2004 ). Circulating IGF1 can be transported through the blood–brain barrier into the brain ( Pardridge, 1993 ), and increased levels of circulating IGF1 also result in increased IGF1 levels in the brain ( Carro et al. , 2000 ).

Plasticity promoting effects

Neutralizing IGF-I antibodies block the effect of physical exercise on upregulation of hippocampal BDNF expression ( Chen and Russo-Neustadt, 2007 ) and adult neurogenesis ( Trejo et al. , 2001 ) in rodents. However, other mechanisms than IGF1 such as altered blood flow could also contribute to this effect ( Davila et al. , 2007 ). In aged rodents, circulating IGF1 levels decrease ( Breese et al. , 1991 ; Niblock et al. , 1998 ). In humans, basal circulating levels of IGF1 are lower in older individuals with high fitness than in those with lower fitness, but the transient IGF1 increases after exercise rise to significant levels only in those individuals with higher levels of fitness ( Amir et al. , 2007 ). Nevertheless, progressive muscle training in aged humans has a significant effect on IGF1 upregulation in muscle that corresponds to musculoskeletal remodeling ( Singh et al. , 1999 ).

Negative effects

IGF1 also seems to play a role in metabolism related to protein toxicity, and depletion of IGF1 apparently has a protective role ( Cohen et al. , 2009 ; Gontier et al. , 2015 ) in mouse models of Alzheimer's disease. These data suggest that IGF1 cannot simply be considered as a mediator of neuroprotective effects of physical exercise on brain function. In fact, lower levels of IGF1 convey longevity ( Longo et al. , 2015 ). Thus future studies have to define the conditions under which IGF1 can mediate neuroprotective effects of physical exercise or prevent acceleration of neurodegeneration during ageing.

•  Are there exercise-mimetics?

Muscle energy metabolism is regulated by AMP-Kinase ( Hardie, 2004 ). AMPK lies at the core of complex interconnected energy-sensing networks that include many other transcriptional co-activators such as peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor gamma coactivator-1 alpha (PGC-1α and sirtuin 1). This network is activated by a decrease in the ATP/AMP ratio within cells and increases catabolism, while reducing anabolic processes. Interestingly, over-expression of PGC-1α in mouse muscle affects the kynurenine pathway and protects these mice from stress induced reduction of synaptic plasticity proteins in the brain, as well as from exhibiting depression-like behaviors ( Agudelo et al. , 2014 ). Overexpression of PGC-1α in muscle also has been reported to lead to increased production of Fibronectin type III domain containing 5 (FNDC5), a myokine that is released during exercise. Enzymatic cleavage of FNDC5 generates a peptide called irisin, which may enter the brain and induce hippocampal BDNF gene expression, and that is upregulated by an exercise intervention in humans ( Wrann et al. , 2013 ; Sleiman and Chao, 2015 ; Wrann, 2015 ). Support for the idea that muscle energy metabolism affects brain function ( Kobilo et al. , 2011 ) comes from studies showing that pharmacological activation of AMPK with the agonist 5-aminoimidazole-4-carboxamide 1-β-D-ribofuranoside (AICAR) can mimic effects of exercise on endurance ( Narkar et al. , 2008 ), cognition, adult neurogenesis and hippocampal BDNF levels ( Kobilo et al. , 2011 ; Guerrieri and van Praag, 2015 ). It is important to note that the beneficial effects of AICAR on the brain depend on the duration of administration ( Kobilo et al. , 2011 ). Prolonged treatment (2 weeks) resulted in increased hippocampal and cortical expression of apoptotic genes, elevated markers of oxidative stress and inflammation (Guerrieri and van Praag, 2015), suggesting it may prove challenging to design pharmacological interventions that mimic exercise and are safe for humans.

Running increases dendritic complexity and the number of dendritic spines in the dentate gyrus ( Eadie et al. , 2005 ), CA1 and entorhinal cortex ( Stranahan et al. , 2007 ; Siette et al. , 2013 ; for review see Opendak and Gould, 2015 ). Aerobic exercise also increases resting state perfusion in the hippocampus in rodents and young/middle-aged humans ( Pereira et al. , 2007 ), which could be mediated by exercise-induced angiogenesis. Animal studies suggest that angiogenesis is closely linked to adult neurogenesis ( Palmer et al. , 2000 ; Louissaint et al. , 2002 ; Pereira et al. , 2007 ). Running-induced adult hippocampal neurogenesis is positively correlated with increased synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and pattern separation in adult animals. In addition, exercise can reverse declining neurogenesis and memory function in aged rodents ( van Praag, 2008 ; Marlatt et al. , 2010 ; Vivar et al. , 2013 ; Opendak and Gould, 2015 ), ( Figs 1 C and 2 ).

Effects of exercise on spatial memory, pattern separation and hippocampal circuitry. ( A–D ) Examples of tasks that tap into pattern separation and are influenced by exercise in animals ( A ,  Creer et al. , 2010  and B ,  Bolz et al. , 2015  ) and old ( C , Maass et al., 2015c ) and young ( D , Dery et al., 2015 ) human adults. ( E ) Running reduces latency and path-length in the Morris water maze in aged male mice. ( F , Vivar et al., 2015 ) Horizontal sections modified from the mouse brain atlas ( Paxinos and Franklin, 2007 ) under sedentary ( top sections) and running ( bottom sections) conditions. Running increased innervation (red) from both caudomedial entorhinal cortex (CEnt) and lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC), proportionate to the increase in adult hippocampal neurogenesis ( Vivar et al., 2015 ). MS = medial septum; OB = olfactory bulb; CB = cerebellum. ( G ) A hypothetical relationship between exercise duration/intensity and improvement and preservation of cognitive function.
Figure 2

Effects of exercise on spatial memory, pattern separation and hippocampal circuitry. ( A–D ) Examples of tasks that tap into pattern separation and are influenced by exercise in animals ( A , Creer et al. , 2010 and B , Bolz et al. , 2015 ) and old ( C , Maass et al., 2015c ) and young ( D , Dery et al., 2015 ) human adults. ( E ) Running reduces latency and path-length in the Morris water maze in aged male mice. ( F , Vivar et al., 2015 ) Horizontal sections modified from the mouse brain atlas ( Paxinos and Franklin, 2007 ) under sedentary ( top sections) and running ( bottom sections) conditions. Running increased innervation (red) from both caudomedial entorhinal cortex (CEnt) and lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC), proportionate to the increase in adult hippocampal neurogenesis ( Vivar et al., 2015 ). MS = medial septum; OB = olfactory bulb; CB = cerebellum. ( G ) A hypothetical relationship between exercise duration/intensity and improvement and preservation of cognitive function.

It is important to consider that new hippocampal neurons are a functional and integral part of brain circuitry rather than just a local hippocampal phenomenon. Under sedentary conditions, substantial input to new neurons originates from perirhinal and lateral entorhinal cortex ( Vivar et al. , 2012 ), areas that are considered important for object and object-in-context information processing ( Knierim et al. , 2014 ). Lesion of this projection to new neurons results in deficient pattern separation ( Vivar et al. , 2012 ). Running increases fine discrimination ( Creer et al. , 2010 ; Bolz et al. , 2015 ) and results in enhanced short-term plasticity and increased input from lateral entorhinal cortex, as well as recruitment of caudo-medial entorhinal cortex to the circuitry of new neurons ( Vivar et al. , 2015 ), ( Fig. 2 ). Consistently, recent research in humans revealed a positive association between entorhinal cortex grey matter volume, cardiovascular fitness and memory function in young adults ( Whiteman et al. , 2015 ).

Neurovascular plasticity and neurotrophins in humans

Human post-mortem studies using environmentally mediated 14 C labelling support earlier research ( Eriksson et al. , 1998 ) that hippocampal neurogenesis continuously occurs until very advanced age ( Spalding et al. , 2013 ; Bergmann et al. , 2015 ). In the absence of in vivo imaging markers for human neurogenesis, exercise studies have used indirect indices of plasticity including morphometric measures such as magnetic resonance volumetry of grey matter and diffusion tensor imaging of white matter, functional and perfusion imaging of the hippocampus (for a review see Thomas et al. , 2012 ), as well as PET (for a review of PET studies see Boecker and Drzezga, 2015 ).

Although several randomized controlled exercise interventions have been conducted in old age ( Colcombe et al. , 2004 ; Lautenschlager et al. , 2008 ; Muscari et al. , 2010 ; Anderson-Hanley et al. , 2012 ; Barnes et al. , 2013 ; Suzuki et al. , 2013 ) these have not assessed the relationship between improvements in cardiovascular fitness and morphometric, functional and perfusion-based brain changes, and memory performance. Also, in some of these studies outcome measures were primarily cognitive scores used in clinical Alzheimer’s disease trials [Mini-Mental State Examination ( Muscari et al. , 2010 ); Alzheimer Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS-Cog; Lautenschlager et al. , 2008 ; Suzuki et al. , 2013 ), Cognitive Battery of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer Disease words recall ( Lautenschlager et al. , 2008 ; Suzuki et al. , 2013 )] and hence not designed to be sensitive to specific plasticity-related mechanisms.

Few intervention trials ( Colcombe et al. , 2006 ; Erickson et al. , 2011 ; Ruscheweyh et al. , 2011 ; Dery et al. , 2013 ; Maass et al. , 2015 b , c ) did relate fitness changes to hippocampal and/or serum biomarkers of plasticity in a way that allowed more direct contact with the animal physiology of exercise-induced plasticity. Mild-to-moderate exercise over a period of 1 year appears to prevent hippocampal volume atrophy ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ). Another study ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ) of older individuals using very high-resolution (0.6 mm isotropic) structural imaging with high field MRI (7 T) did not observe a volume difference between moderate-to-intensive exercisers and controls over a period of 3 months. However, both studies found that hippocampal volume changes were correlated with cardio-vascular fitness changes and with changes in memory scores. Another study of mild-to-moderate exercise ( Ruscheweyh et al. , 2011 ) did not observe fitness-related hippocampal changes after 6 months. A recent meta-analysis of exercise-related white matter changes (white matter volume, lesions, microstructure) as measured with MRI showed that there are modest beneficial effects, which are not consistent across studies ( Sexton et al. , 2015 ).

Gadolinium contrast imaging in humans revealed hippocampal perfusion changes after exercise in young ( Pereira et al. , 2007 ) and older adults ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ). In the older adults, again the exercise group did not show an improvement of perfusion but instead a correlation between changes in fitness levels and perfusion changes, hippocampal head volumes, and grey matter density ( Fig. 1 D) within the hippocampus.

It is unclear how exercise-induced changes in levels of growth and neurotrophic factors relate to volumetric and perfusion changes. In a 1-year intervention study ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ) changes in serum BDNF were correlated with increases in hippocampal volume (exercise group only) and functional connectivity between the bilateral parahippocampal and bilateral middle temporal gyrus ( Voss et al. , 2013 ). Another study showed that aerobic exercise did not elevate serum BDNF levels, whereas exhaustive physical exercise only induced BDNF serum changes that lasted for a few minutes ( Berg and Bang, 2004 ; Rojas Vega et al. , 2006 ). In healthy older humans there was no increase in the serum concentration of BDNF, VEGF or IGF1 ( Box 2 ) by moderate-to-intensive exercise for 3 months ( Maass et al. , 2015 b ) or mild-to-moderate exercise for a year ( Voss et al. , 2013 ). One obvious limitation of such studies is that serum sampling pre- and post-intervention may miss the dynamic time course of growth factor upregulation, which, as outlined above, might be fast and transient ( Berg and Bang, 2004 ; Rojas Vega et al. , 2006 ).

The cognitive effects of exercise in animals and humans

Hippocampal–medial temporal lobe memory circuits ( Fig. 1 ) partly segregate modalities such as objects and scenes, and memory processes such as pattern separation and pattern completion ( Treves et al. , 2008 ; Yassa and Stark, 2011 ; Kumaran and McClelland, 2012 ). Converging evidence suggests that running-induced or transgenic upregulation of adult neurogenesis is associated with improved pattern separation ( Creer et al. , 2010 ; Sahay et al. , 2011 ; Bolz et al. , 2015 ) ( Fig. 2 A–D). Quantitative analysis of afferent innervation of new neurons under running conditions suggests there is a shift in network balance that may benefit pattern separation processes ( Vivar et al. , 2015 ). In particular, exercise increased adult neurogenesis more than the expansion of afferent inputs. This may allow for a larger spread of incoming data over individual new neurons, and benefit sparse encoding.

In older humans, memory measures are not consistently improved in randomized controlled trials of physical exercise due to considerable interindividual variability ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ; Maass et al. , 2015 c ). However, rather than being random, this variability appears to be linked to neurovascular plasticity. After 3 months of exercise, changes in complex object recognition memory, a task that poses high demands on pattern separation ( Fig. 2 D), were correlated with changes in fitness levels, gadolinium-contrast magnetic resonance–perfusion in the hippocampus and hippocampal head volumes ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ). Another study in healthy older adults that did not assess pattern separation ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ) found that changes in fitness and hippocampal volumes were correlated with performance in a short-delay spatial working memory task.

Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease

A substantial proportion of older adults who perform normally on standard neuropsychological assessments of dementia show Alzheimer’s disease pathology and this is referred to as the preclinical phase of Alzheimer’s disease ( Reiman et al. , 2016 ). Preclinical cerebral tau deposition starts in the transentorhinal cortex ( Fig. 1 A and B) and propagates to the dentate gyrus, CA1 and other medial temporal lobe regions (Gallagher and Koh, 2011; Braak and Del Tredici, 2015 ) although there is a controversy as to whether this pathology is part of a normal ageing process ( Duyckaerts et al. , 2015 ; Jellinger et al. , 2015 ). Preclinical amyloid deposition is primarily found in extra-medial temporal lobe regions such as the retrosplenial region ( Buckner et al. , 2005 ; Bateman et al. , 2012 ).

There is some evidence that physical activity levels may modify disease pathology in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Mouse models suggest that exercise may improve degradation and clearance of amyloid-β ( Adlard et al. , 2005 ). In humans, habitual physical activity levels, as measured by self-report questionnaires, are associated with lower brain amyloid load ( Brown et al. , 2013 ; Wirth et al. , 2014 ) lower insulin, triglyceride, amyloid-β 1–42/1–40 levels ( Brown et al. , 2013 ), and higher brain glucose metabolism, immediate recall and visuospatial ability ( Vidoni et al. , 2012 ; Okonkwo et al. , 2014 ). Equally important is the possibility that preclinical Alzheimer’s disease pathology modifies the effects of exercise. Amyloid-β deposition may lead to insulin resistance in the brain and therefore cause metabolic dysfunction ( Bomfim et al. , 2012 ).

Aside from these direct interactions, exercise also has the potential to influence the modifiable risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease ( Norton et al. , 2014 ; Cooper et al. , 2015 ), such as insulin-resistance ( Yarchoan and Arnold, 2014 ). Insulin-resistance in middle-aged to older adults is associated with reduced glucose metabolism in the medial temporal lobe and this association is even stronger than with APOE4 status ( Willette et al. , 2015 ), which is a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease and also associated with reduced brain glucose metabolism ( Jagust et al. , 2012 ). Exercise can improve insulin sensitivity ( Lucas et al. , 2015 ) and could thereby potentially contribute indirectly to improving memory function.

Can the effects of exercise be enhanced?

One possibility to enhance efficacy is to combine physical activity with cognitive stimulation (i.e. Anderson-Hanley et al. , 2012 ; Barnes et al. , 2013 ). Just like exercise ( Erickson et al. , 2011 ), spatial navigation training in old age can prevent the hippocampus from shrinkage ( Lovden et al. , 2012 ) and also has a modest preserving effect on the retrosplenial region ( Wenger et al. , 2012 ). A combination of exercise and cognitive enrichment in mice increases protective effects against synaptotoxicity of amyloid-β protein in the hippocampus ( Li et al. , 2013 ).

Pharmacological and dietary enhancements are other possibilities. The dietary flavanol epicatechin may enhance effects of running on retention of spatial memory in young rodents ( van Praag, 2009 ) and may protect against Alzheimer's disease ( Cox et al. , 2015 ). Three months intake of a high flavanol drink in older humans enhanced reaction times in pattern separation but was independent of exercise ( Brickman et al. , 2014 ). The flavanol epicatechin also has anxiolytic properties in rodents ( Stringer et al. , 2015 ). Exercise has antidepressant effects ( Ernst et al. , 2006 ; Ota and Duman, 2013 ) but serotonergic antidepressants do not seem to enhance the efficacy of exercise interventions in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease ( Marlatt et al. , 2013 ). Another possibility is that the administration of ‘exercise-mimetics’ that activate energy metabolism pathways in muscle affect brain function ( Box 2 ). Furthermore, the importance of glucose metabolism in ageing and dementia ( Wirth et al. , 2014 ) and the empirical observation that the insulin sensitizer pioglutazone is protective against Alzheimer’s disease ( Heneka et al. , 2015 ) raises the possibility that a combination of exercise and insulin sensitization may be effective ( Yarchoan and Arnold, 2014 ). This approach may be particularly relevant for older adults with metabolic syndrome, a major risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease ( Norton et al. , 2014 ). More fundamentally, future studies will need to address the question whether exercise interventions should complement disease-modifying treatments of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease which target amyloid ( Sperling et al. , 2014 ) or, in future, tau deposition.

Interindividual variability: side effects and intermediate outcome markers

A number of vascular risk factors could compromise vascular plasticity after exercise, including metabolic syndrome, insulin resistance, hypertension, dyslipidaemia and recurrent strokes ( Iadecola, 2013 ). Arteriogenesis and angiogenesis are stimulated not only by growth factors (i.e. VEGF) but also by mechanical factors such as fluid shear stress, which can be compromised by arteriosclerosis, and cerebral amyloid angiopathy ( Charidimou et al. , 2012 ; Iadecola, 2013 ; Schmidt et al. , 2013 ). These factors can also limit adequate perfusion of the hippocampus during acute exercise ( Schmidt et al. , 2013 ). In the Maass et al. (2015 c ) study reduced perfusion after exercise was observed in some individuals (particularly those older than 70) despite the fact that vascular risk factors were excluded. Indeed, more subtle factors could also compromise vascular plasticity. For instance, in ageing and MCI, leakage of gadolinium-contrast agent into CA1 and dentate gyrus suggests damage to the hippocampal blood–brain barrier that may be caused by pericyte injury ( Iadecola, 2015 ; Montagne et al. , 2015 ). Finally, stress can suppress adult neurogenesis in animal models ( Opendak and Gould, 2015 ), and elevated cortisol has a negative effect on verbal recall in old age ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ). However, in one study, cortisol levels were unrelated to the vascular and cognitive benefits of exercise in old age ( Maass et al. , 2015 c ).

Until the sources of outcome variability ( Freund et al. , 2013 ) are better understood, individuals at risk for adverse effects can be identified and criteria for either terminating or adapting intensity ( Box 1 ) are developed, it seems premature to unrestrictedly prescribe moderate-to-intensive exercise interventions in old age. Moderate-to-intense exercise may have detrimental effects particularly in the presence of cerebrovascular and metabolic risk factors ( Lucas et al. , 2015 ) when there is increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease ( Schmidt et al. , 2013 ; Norton et al. , 2014 ). Empirical data from the ‘million women’ study indicate that moderate activity was associated with lower risks for cerebrovascular events (haemorrhagic and ischaemic) than strenuous physical activity ( Armstrong et al. , 2015 ).

Genetic factors may be another source of variability ( Podewils et al. , 2005 ) pertaining to risk genes for Alzheimer’s disease such as APOE4 ( Brown et al. , 2013 ; Jansen et al. , 2015 ) and genes that modify plasticity such as BDNF ( Egan et al. , 2003 ; Rauskolb et al. , 2010 ). In a self-report study of habitual physical activity, APOE4 carrying older adults in the highest tertile of physical activity had lower brain amyloid burden than those in the lowest exercise tertile ( Brown et al. , 2013 ) while in non-carriers there was no difference.

Future outlook

It is now feasible to study the impact of exercise interventions on various aspects of vascular plasticity, connectivity, amyloid load, glucose metabolism and linking these effects to circuit-specific cognitive functions ( Box 1 and Fig. 1 B). Advances in brain imaging also allow investigating the effects of exercise on the function and structure of entorhinal cortex subregions ( Maass et al. , 2014 , 2015 a ).

In reviewing and discussing the effects of exercise, we see the need to highlight a distinction between preservation (or maintenance) and improvement of function ( Box 1 and Fig. 2 G). Improvement of function refers to plasticity in specific memory circuits (i.e. the dentate gyrus and entorhinal cortex) along with increased hippocampal perfusion and volume. Preservation, on the other hand, refers to the possibility that exercise interventions, particularly if they are prolonged, modify risk factors for cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease, such as metabolic and vascular risk factors and amyloid deposition.

Although we do not yet understand the time courses of plasticity and risk modification this distinction may improve the selection of outcome measures in exercise interventions and thereby the consistency of results. Clinical outcome measures such as ADAS-Cog (for an overview see Andrieu et al. , 2015 ) tax widely distributed neural networks and may be suited in prevention trials aimed at preserving brain function. In contrast, they are likely to provide negative results in brief exercise interventions where the primary goal is to induce plasticity.

As adult neurogenesis declines with ageing ( Seki and Arai, 1995 ) and mobility may be hampered by frailty, it is important to explore the possibility of pharmacologically enhancing or substituting the effects of exercise ( Box 2 ). Of equal importance is a better understanding of side effects of exercise, which may not be necessarily predicted by the presence of vascular risk factors. A concerted effort in which clinical prevention trials are complemented by trials designed to induce plasticity may offer the fastest way towards an optimal utilization and prescription of exercise interventions in old age and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease, both as stand-alone intervention and as an add-on strategy to disease modification and prevention. To facilitate this research, there is an urgent need for the field to adjust and standardize protocols and outcome measures ( Box 1 ).

Acknowledgements

We are most grateful to David Berron, Linda R. Kitabayashi and Galit Benoni for help with image preparation.

Funding

This work was supported in part by the National Institute on Aging, Intramural Research Program (DFG, TRR58, A09) and the BMBF through the EnergI consortium and the Deutsche Forschungs-Gemeinschaft (SFB 779 TP A7).

References

Adlard
PA
Perreau
VM
Cotman
CW
.
The exercise-induced expression of BDNF within the hippocampus varies across life-span
.
Neurobiol Aging
2005
;
26
:
511
20
.

Agudelo
LZ
Femenia
T
Orhan
F
Porsmyr-Palmertz
M
Goiny
M
Martinez-Redondo
V
et al. .
Skeletal muscle PGC-1alpha1 modulates kynurenine metabolism and mediates resilience to stress-induced depression
.
Cell
2014
;
159
:
33
45
.

Ahlskog
JE
Geda
YE
Graff-Radford
NR
Petersen
RC
.
Physical exercise as a preventive or disease-modifying treatment of dementia and brain aging
.
Mayo Clin Proc
2011
;
86
:
876
84
.

Aleman
A
Torres-Aleman
I
.
Circulating insulin-like growth factor I and cognitive function: neuromodulation throughout the lifespan
.
Progress in neurobiology
2009
;
89
:
256
65
.

Amir
R
Ben-Sira
D
Sagiv
M
.
Igf-I and fgf-2 responses to wingate anaerobic test in older men
.
J Sports Sci Med
2007
;
6
:
227
32
.

Anderson-Hanley
C
Arciero
PJ
Brickman
AM
.
Exergaming and older adult cognition: a cluster randomized clinical trial
.
Am J Prev Med
2012
;
42
:
109
19
.

Andrieu
S
Coley
N
Lovestone
S
Aisen
PS
Vellas
B
.
Prevention of sporadic Alzheimer's disease: lessons learned from clinical trials and future directions
.
Lancet Neurol
2015
;
14
:
926
44
.

Armstrong
ME
Green
J
Reeves
GK
Beral
V
Cairns
BJ
Million Women Study
C
.
Frequent physical activity may not reduce vascular disease risk as much as moderate activity: large prospective study of women in the United Kingdom
.
Circulation
2015
;
131
:
721
9
.

Barnes
DE
Santos-Modesitt
W
Poelke
G
Kramer
AF
Castro
C
Middleton
LE
et al. .
The Mental Activity and eXercise (MAX) trial: a randomized controlled trial to enhance cognitive function in older adults
.
JAMA Intern Med
2013
;
173
:
797
804
.

Bateman
RJ
Xiong
C
Benzinger
TL
Fagan
AM
Goate
A
Fox
NC
et al. .
Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer's disease
.
N Engl J Med
2012
;
367
:
795
804
.

Benedict
C
Brooks
SJ
Kullberg
J
Nordenskjold
R
Burgos
J
Le Greves
M
et al. .
Association between physical activity and brain health in older adults
.
Neurobiol Aging
2013
;
34
:
83
90
.

Berg
U
Bang
P
.
Exercise and circulating insulin-like growth factor I
.
Hormone research
2004
;
62
(
Suppl 1
):
50
8
.

Bergmann
O
Spalding
KL
Frisen
J
.
Adult neurogenesis in humans
.
Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol
2015
;
7
:
a018994
.

Boecker
H
Drzezga
A
A perspective on the future role of brain pet imaging in exercise science
.
Neuroimage
2015
.

Bolz
L
Heigele
S
Bischofberger
J
Running improves pattern separation during novel object recognition
.
Brain Plasticity
2015
.

Bomfim
TR
Forny-Germano
L
Sathler
LB
Brito-Moreira
J
Houzel
JC
Decker
H
et al. .
An anti-diabetes agent protects the mouse brain from defective insulin signaling caused by Alzheimer's disease- associated Abeta oligomers
.
J Clin Invest
2012
;
122
:
1339
53
.

Braak
H
Del Tredici
K
.
The preclinical phase of the pathological process underlying sporadic Alzheimer's disease
.
Brain
2015
;
138
(
Pt 10
):
2814
33
.

Breese
CR
Ingram
RL
Sonntag
WE
.
Influence of age and long-term dietary restriction on plasma insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1), IGF-1 gene expression, and IGF-1 binding proteins
.
J Gerontol
1991
;
46
:
B180
7
.

Brickman
AM
Khan
UA
Provenzano
FA
Yeung
LK
Suzuki
W
Schroeter
H
et al. .
Enhancing dentate gyrus function with dietary flavanols improves cognition in older adults
.
Nat Neurosci
2014
;
17
:
1798
803
.

Brown
BM
Peiffer
JJ
Sohrabi
HR
Mondal
A
Gupta
VB
Rainey-Smith
SR
et al. .
Intense physical activity is associated with cognitive performance in the elderly
.
Transl Psychiatry
2012
;
2
:
e191
.

Brown
BM
Peiffer
JJ
Taddei
K
Lui
JK
Laws
SM
Gupta
VB
et al. .
Physical activity and amyloid-beta plasma and brain levels: results from the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle Study of Ageing
.
Mol Psychiatry
2013
;
18
:
875
81
.

Buckner
RL
Snyder
AZ
Shannon
BJ
LaRossa
G
Sachs
R
Fotenos
AF
et al. .
Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer's disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory
.
J Neurosci
2005
;
25
:
7709
17
.

Bugg
JM
Head
D
.
Exercise moderates age-related atrophy of the medial temporal lobe
.
Neurobiol Aging
2011
;
32
:
506
14
.

Carro
E
Nunez
A
Busiguina
S
Torres-Aleman
I
.
Circulating insulin-like growth factor I mediates effects of exercise on the brain
.
J Neurosci
2000
;
20
:
2926
33
.

Charidimou
A
Gang
Q
Werring
DJ
.
Sporadic cerebral amyloid angiopathy revisited: recent insights into pathophysiology and clinical spectrum
.
J Neurol
2012
;
83
:
124
37
.

Chen
MJ
Russo-Neustadt
AA
.
Running exercise- and antidepressant-induced increases in growth and survival-associated signaling molecules are IGF-dependent
.
Growth Factors
2007
;
25
:
118
31
.

Cohen
E
Paulsson
JF
Blinder
P
Burstyn-Cohen
T
Du
D
Estepa
G
et al. .
Reduced IGF-1 signaling delays age-associated proteotoxicity in mice
.
Cell
2009
;
139
:
1157
69
.

Colcombe
SJ
Erickson
KI
Raz
N
Webb
AG
Cohen
NJ
McAuley
E
et al. .
Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2003
;
58
:
176
80
.

Colcombe
SJ
Erickson
KI
Scalf
PE
Kim
JS
Prakash
R
McAuley
E
et al. .
Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
2006
;
61
:
1166
70
.

Colcombe
SJ
Kramer
AF
Erickson
KI
Scalf
P
McAuley
E
Cohen
NJ
et al. .
Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2004
;
101
:
3316
21
.

Cooper
C
Sommerlad
A
Lyketsos
CG
Livingston
G
.
Modifiable predictors of dementia in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Am J Psychiatry
2015
;
172
:
323
34
.

Cotman
CW
Berchtold
NC
Christie
LA
.
Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation
.
Trends Neurosci
2007
;
30
:
464
72
.

Cox
CJ
Choudhry
F
Peacey
E
Perkinton
MS
Richardson
JC
Howlett
DR
et al. .
Dietary (-)-epicatechin as a potent inhibitor of βγ-secretase amyloid precursor protein processing
.
Neurobiol Aging
2015
;
36
:
178
87
.

Creer
DJ
Romberg
C
Saksida
LM
van Praag
H
Bussey
TJ
.
Running enhances spatial pattern separation in mice
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2010
;
107
:
2367
72
.

Davila
D
Piriz
J
Trejo
JL
Nunez
A
Torres-Aleman
I
.
Insulin and insulin-like growth factor I signalling in neurons
.
Front Biosci
2007
;
12
:
3194
202
.

Dekkers
MP
Nikoletopoulou
V
Barde
YA
.
Cell biology in neuroscience: death of developing neurons: new insights and implications for connectivity
.
J Cell Biol
2013
;
203
:
385
93
.

Dery
N
Pilgrim
M
Gibala
M
Gillen
J
Wojtowicz
JM
Macqueen
G
et al. .
Adult hippocampal neurogenesis reduces memory interference in humans: opposing effects of aerobic exercise and depression
.
Front Neurosci
2013
;
7
:
66
.

Duyckaerts
C
Braak
H
Brion
JP
Buee
L
Del Tredici
K
Goedert
M
et al. .
PART is part of Alzheimer disease
.
Acta Neuropathol
2015
;
129
:
749
56
.

Eadie
BD
Redila
VA
Christie
BR
.
Voluntary exercise alters the cytoarchitecture of the adult dentate gyrus by increasing cellular proliferation, dendritic complexity, and spine density
.
J Comp Neurol
2005
;
486
:
39
47
.

Egan
MF
Kojima
M
Callicott
JH
Goldberg
TE
Kolachana
BS
Bertolino
A
et al. .
The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function
.
Cell
2003
;
112
:
257
69
.

Eichenbaum
H
Yonelinas
AP
Ranganath
C
.
The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
2007
;
30
:
123
52
.

Erickson
KI
Kramer
AF
.
Aerobic exercise effects on cognitive and neural plasticity in older adults
.
Br J Sports Med
2009
;
43
:
22
4
.

Erickson
KI
Voss
MW
Prakash
RS
Basak
C
Szabo
A
Chaddock
L
et al. .
Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2011
;
108
:
3017
22
.

Eriksson
PS
Perfilieva
E
Björk-Eriksson
T
Alborn
AM
Nordborg
C
Peterson
DA
et al. .
Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus
.
Nat Med
1998
;
4
:
1313
17
.

Ernst
C
Olson
AK
Pinel
JP
Lam
RW
Christie
BR
.
Antidepressant effects of exercise: evidence for an adult-neurogenesis hypothesis?
J Psychiatry Neurosci
2006
;
31
:
84
92
.

Flöel
A
Ruscheweyh
R
Krüger
K
Willemer
C
Winter
B
Völker
K
et al. .
Physical activity and memory functions: are neurotrophins and cerebral gray matter volume the missing link?
Neuroimage
2010
;
49
:
2756
63
.

Freund
J
Brandmaier
AM
Lewejohann
L
Kirste
I
Kritzler
M
Krüger
A
et al. .
Emergence of individuality in genetically identical mice
.
Science (New York, NY)
2013
;
340
:
756
9
.

Gallagher
M
Koh
MT
.
Episodic memory on the path to Alzheimer's disease
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
2011
;
21
:
929
34
.

Geda
YE
Silber
TC
Roberts
RO
Knopman
DS
Christianson
TJ
Pankratz
VS
et al. .
Computer activities, physical exercise, aging, and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study
.
Mayo Clin Proc
2012
;
87
:
437
42
.

Gontier
G
George
C
Chaker
Z
Holzenberger
M
Aïd
S
.
Blocking IGF signaling in adult neurons alleviates alzheimer's disease pathology through amyloid-β clearance
.
J Neurosci
2015
;
35
:
11500
13
.

Greenberg
DA
Jin
K
.
From angiogenesis to neuropathology
.
Nature
2005
;
438
:
954
9
.

Guerrieri
D
van Praag
H
.
Exercise-mimetic AICAR transiently benefits brain function
.
Oncotarget
2015
;
6
:
18293
313
.

Hardie
DG
.
The AMP-activated protein kinase pathway–new players upstream and downstream
.
J Cell Sci
2004
;
117
(
Pt 23
):
5479
87
.

Heneka
MT
Fink
A
Doblhammer
G
.
Effect of pioglitazone medication on the incidence of dementia
.
Ann Neurol
2015
;
78
:
284
94
.

Ho
AJ
Raji
CA
Becker
JT
Lopez
OL
Kuller
LH
Hua
X
et al. .
The effects of physical activity, education, and body mass index on the aging brain
.
Hum Brain Mapp
2011
;
32
:
1371
82
.

Honea
RA
Thomas
GP
Harsha
A
Anderson
HS
Donnelly
JE
Brooks
WM
et al. .
Cardiorespiratory fitness and preserved medial temporal lobe volume in Alzheimer disease
.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
2009
;
23
:
188
97
.

Hopkins
ME
Bucci
DJ
.
BDNF expression in perirhinal cortex is associated with exercise-induced improvement in object recognition memory
.
Neurobiol Learn Mem
2010
;
94
:
278
84
.

Hornberger
M
Wong
S
Tan
R
Irish
M
Piguet
O
Kril
J
et al. .
In vivo and post-mortem memory circuit integrity in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease
.
Brain
2012
;
135
(
Pt 10
):
3015
25
.

Iadecola
C
.
The pathobiology of vascular dementia
.
Neuron
2013
;
80
:
844
66
.

Iadecola
C
.
Dangerous leaks: blood-brain barrier woes in the aging hippocampus
.
Neuron
2015
;
85
:
231
3
.

Jagust
WJ
Landau
SM
Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging
I
.
Apolipoprotein E, not fibrillar beta-amyloid, reduces cerebral glucose metabolism in normal aging
.
J Neurosci
2012
;
32
:
18227
33
.

Jansen
WJ
Ossenkoppele
R
Knol
DL
Tijms
BM
Scheltens
P
Verhey
FR
et al. .
Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: a meta-analysis
.
JAMA
2015
;
313
:
1924
38
.

Jellinger
KA
Alafuzoff
I
Attems
J
Beach
TG
Cairns
NJ
Crary
JF
et al. .
PART, a distinct tauopathy, different from classical sporadic Alzheimer disease
.
Acta Neuropathol
2015
;
129
:
757
62
.

Knierim
JJ
Neunuebel
JP
Deshmukh
SS
.
Functional correlates of the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex: objects, path integration and local-global reference frames
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
2014
;
369
:
20130369
.

Kobilo
T
Yuan
C
van Praag
H
.
Endurance factors improve hippocampal neurogenesis and spatial memory in mice
.
Learn Mem
2011
;
18
:
103
7
.

Korte
M
Carroll
P
Wolf
E
Brem
G
Thoenen
H
Bonhoeffer
T
.
Hippocampal long-term potentiation is impaired in mice lacking brain-derived neurotrophic factor
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1995
;
92
:
8856
60
.

Kumaran
D
McClelland
JL
.
Generalization through the recurrent interaction of episodic memories: a model of the hippocampal system
.
Psychol Rev
2012
;
119
:
573
616
.

Leutgeb
JK
Leutgeb
S
Moser
MB
Moser
EI
.
Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus
.
Science
2007
;
315
:
961
966
.

Lautenschlager
NT
Cox
KL
Flicker
L
Foster
JK
Bockxmeer
FM
Xiao
J
et al. .
Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial
.
JAMA
2008
;
300
:
1027
37
.

Leutgeb
JK
Leutgeb
S
Moser
MB
Moser
EI
.
Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus
.
Science
2007
;
315
:
961
6
.

Li
S
Jin
M
Zhang
D
Yang
T
Koeglsperger
T
Fu
H
et al. .
Environmental novelty activates beta2-adrenergic signaling to prevent the impairment of hippocampal LTP by Abeta oligomers
.
Neuron
2013
;
77
:
929
41
.

Longo
VD
Antebi
A
Bartke
A
Barzilai
N
Brown-Borg
HM
Caruso
C
et al. .
Interventions to slow aging in humans: are we ready?
Aging Cell
2015
;
14
:
497
510
.

Louissaint
A
Jr
Rao
S
Leventhal
C
Goldman
SA
.
Coordinated interaction of neurogenesis and angiogenesis in the adult songbird brain
.
Neuron
2002
;
34
:
945
60
.

Lovden
M
Schaefer
S
Noack
H
Bodammer
NC
Kuhn
S
Heinze
HJ
et al. .
Spatial navigation training protects the hippocampus against age-related changes during early and late adulthood
.
Neurobiol Aging
2012
;
33
:
620 e9
e22
.

Lucas
SJ
Cotter
JD
Brassard
P
Bailey
DM
.
High-intensity interval exercise and cerebrovascular health: curiosity, cause, and consequence
.
J Cerebr Blood Flow Metab
2015
;
35
:
902
11
.

Maass
A
Berron
D
Libby
LA
Ranganath
C
Duzel
E
Functional subregions of the human entorhinal cortex
.
Elife
2015a
;
4
.

Maass
A
Duzel
S
Goerke
M
Becke
A
Sobieray
U
Neumann
K
et al. .
Relationship between peripheral IGF-1, VEGF and BDNF levels and exercise-related changes in memory, hippocampal perfusion and volumes in older adults
.
Neuroimage
2015b
.

Maass
A
Duzel
S
Goerke
M
Becke
A
Sobieray
U
Neumann
K
et al. .
Vascular hippocampal plasticity after aerobic exercise in older adults
.
Mol Psychiatry
2015c
;
20
:
585
93
.

Maass
A
Schutze
H
Speck
O
Yonelinas
A
Tempelmann
C
Heinze
HJ
et al. .
Laminar activity in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex related to novelty and episodic encoding
.
Nat Commun
2014
;
5
:
5547
.

Marlatt
MW
Lucassen
PJ
van Praag
H
.
Comparison of neurogenic effects of fluoxetine, duloxetine and running in mice
.
Brain Res
2010
;
1341
:
93
9
.

Marlatt
MW
Potter
MC
Bayer
TA
van Praag
H
Lucassen
PJ
.
Prolonged running, not fluoxetine treatment, increases neurogenesis, but does not alter neuropathology, in the 3xTg mouse model of Alzheimer's disease
.
Curr Top Behav Neurosci
2013
;
15
:
313
40
.

Montagne
A
Barnes
SR
Sweeney
MD
Halliday
MR
Sagare
AP
Zhao
Z
et al. .
Blood-brain barrier breakdown in the aging human hippocampus
.
Neuron
2015
;
85
:
296
302
.

Moon
HY
van Praag
H
.
Muscle over mind
.
Cell Metab
2014
;
20
:
560
2
.

Muscari
A
Giannoni
C
Pierpaoli
L
.
Chronic endurance exercise training prevents aging-related cognitive decline in healthy older adults: a randomized controlled trial
.
J Geriatr Psychiatry
2010
;
25
:
1055
64
.

Narkar
VA
Downes
M
Yu
RT
Embler
E
Wang
YX
Banayo
E
et al. .
AMPK and PPARdelta agonists are exercise mimetics
.
Cell
2008
;
134
:
405
15
.

Navarro Schroder
T
Haak
KV
Zaragoza Jimenez
NI
Beckmann
CF
Doeller
CF
.
Functional topography of the human entorhinal cortex
.
Elife
2015
;
4
.

Neeper
SA
Gomez-Pinilla
F
Choi
J
Cotman
C
.
Exercise and brain neurotrophins
.
Nature
1995
;
373
:
109
.

Neeper
SA
Gomez-Pinilla
F
Choi
J
Cotman
CW
.
Physical activity increases mRNA for brain-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth factor in rat brain
.
Brain Res
1996
;
726
:
49
56
.

Neunuebel
JP
Knierim
JJ
.
CA3 retrieves coherent representations from degraded input: direct evidence for CA3 pattern completion and dentate gyrus pattern separation
.
Neuron
2014
;
81
:
416
27
.

Ngandu
T
Lehtisalo
J
Solomon
A
Levälahti
E
Ahtiluoto
S
Antikainen
R
et al. .
A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial
.
Lancet
2015
.

Niblock
MM
Brunso-Bechtold
JK
Lynch
CD
Ingram
RL
McShane
T
Sonntag
WE
.
Distribution and levels of insulin-like growth factor I mRNA across the life span in the Brown Norway x Fischer 344 rat brain
.
Brain Res
1998
;
804
:
79
86
.

Norton
S
Matthews
FE
Barnes
DE
Yaffe
K
Brayne
C
.
Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer's disease: an analysis of population-based data
.
Lancet Neurol
2014
;
13
:
788
94
.

Okonkwo
OC
Schultz
SA
Oh
JM
Larson
J
Edwards
D
Cook
D
et al. .
Physical activity attenuates age-related biomarker alterations in preclinical AD
.
Neurology
2014
;
83
:
1753
60
.

Opendak
M
Gould
E
.
Adult neurogenesis: a substrate for experience-dependent change
.
Trends Cogn Sci
2015
;
19
:
151
61
.

Ota
KT
Duman
RS
.
Environmental and pharmacological modulations of cellular plasticity: role in the pathophysiology and treatment of depression
.
Neurobiol Dis
2013
;
57
:
28
37
.

Palmer
TD
Willhoite
AR
Gage
FH
.
Vascular niche for adult hippocampal neurogenesis
.
J Comp Neurol
2000
;
425
:
479
94
.

Pardridge
WM
.
Transport of insulin-related peptides and glucose across the blood-brain barrier
.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
1993
;
692
:
126
37
.

Park
H
Poo
MM
.
Neurotrophin regulation of neural circuit development and function
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
2013
;
14
:
7
23
.

Patterson
SL
Abel
T
Deuel
TA
Martin
KC
Rose
JC
Kandel
ER
.
Recombinant BDNF rescues deficits in basal synaptic transmission and hippocampal LTP in BDNF knockout mice
.
Neuron
1996
;
16
:
1137
45
.

Paxinos
G
Franklin
K
.
Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates
,
2007
.
Academic Press
.

Pereira
AC
Huddleston
DE
Brickman
AM
Sosunov
AA
Hen
R
McKhann
GM
et al. .
An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2007
;
104
:
5638
43
.

Podewils
LJ
Guallar
E
Kuller
LH
Fried
LP
Lopez
OL
Carlson
M
et al. .
Physical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study
.
Am J Epidemiol
2005
;
161
:
639
51
.

Prakash
RS
Voss
MW
Erickson
KI
Kramer
AF
.
Physical activity and cognitive vitality
.
Annu Rev Psychol
2015
;
66
:
769
97
.

Rauskolb
S
Zagrebelsky
M
Dreznjak
A
Deogracias
R
Matsumoto
T
Wiese
S
et al. .
Global deprivation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the CNS reveals an area-specific requirement for dendritic growth
.
J Neurosci
2010
;
30
:
1739
49
.

Reiman
EM
Langbaum
JB
Tariot
PN
Lopera
F
Bateman
RJ
Morris
JC
et al. .
CAP-advancing the evaluation of preclinical Alzheimer disease treatments
.
Nat Rev Neurol
2016
;
12
:
56
61
.

Rojas Vega
S
Struder
HK
Vera Wahrmann
B
Schmidt
A
Bloch
W
Hollmann
W
.
Acute BDNF and cortisol response to low intensity exercise and following ramp incremental exercise to exhaustion in humans
.
Brain Res
2006
;
1121
:
59
65
.

Ruscheweyh
R
Willemer
C
Kruger
K
Duning
T
Warnecke
T
Sommer
J
et al. .
Physical activity and memory functions: an interventional study
.
Neurobiol Aging
2011
;
32
:
1304
19
.

Sahay
A
Scobie
KN
Hill
AS
O'Carroll
CM
Kheirbek
MA
Burghardt
NS
et al. .
Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation
.
Nature
2011
;
472
:
466
70
.

Schmidt
W
Endres
M
Dimeo
F
Jungehulsing
GJ
.
Train the vessel, gain the brain: physical activity and vessel function and the impact on stroke prevention and outcome in cerebrovascular disease
.
Cerebrovasc Dis
2013
;
35
:
303
12
.

Seki
T
Arai
Y
.
Age-related production of new granule cells in the adult dentate gyrus
.
Neuroreport
1995
;
6
:
2479
82
.

Sendtner
M
Motoneuron disease
. In:
Handbook of experimental pharmacology
,
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
,
Vol. 220
.
2014
, p.
411
41
.

Sexton
C
Betts
JF
Demnitz
N
Dawes
H
Ebmeier
KP
JohansenBerg
H
.
A systematic review of MRI studies examining the relationship between physical fitness and activity and the white matter of the ageing brain
.
Neuroimage
2015
.

Siette
J
Westbrook
RF
Cotman
C
Sidhu
K
Zhu
W
Sachdev
P
et al. .
Age-specific effects of voluntary exercise on memory and the older brain
.
Biol Psychiatry
2013
;
73
:
435
42
.

Singh
MA
Ding
W
Manfredi
TJ
Solares
GS
O'Neill
EF
Clements
KM
et al. .
Insulin-like growth factor I in skeletal muscle after weight-lifting exercise in frail elders
.
Am J Physiol
1999
;
277(1 Pt 1)
:
E135
43
.

Sleiman
SF
Chao
MV
.
Downstream consequences of exercise through the action of BDNF
.
Brain Plasticity
2015
:
143
8
.

Spalding
KL
Bergmann
O
Alkass
K
Bernard
S
Salehpour
M
Huttner
HB
et al. .
Dynamics of hippocampal neurogenesis in adult humans
.
Cell
2013
;
153
:
1219
27
.

Sperling
RA
Rentz
DM
Johnson
KA
Karlawish
J
Donohue
M
Salmon
DP
et al. .
The A4 study: stopping AD before symptoms begin?
Sci Transl Med
2014
;
6
:
228fs13
.

Stranahan
AM
Khalil
D
Gould
E
.
Running induces widespread structural alterations in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex
.
Hippocampus
2007
;
17
:
1017
22
.

Stringer
TP
Guerrieri
D
Vivar
C
van Praag
H
.
Plant-derived flavanol (-)epicatechin mitigates anxiety in association with elevated hippocampal monoamine and BDNF levels, but does not influence pattern separation in mice
.
Transl Psychiatry
2015
;
5
:
e493
.

Suzuki
T
Shimada
H
Makizako
H
Doi
T
Yoshida
D
A randomized controlled trial of multicomponent exercise in older adults with mild cognitive impairment
.
PloS One
2013
;
8
;
e61483
.

Tan
RH
Wong
S
Kril
JJ
Piguet
O
Hornberger
M
Hodges
JR
et al. .
Beyond the temporal pole: limbic memory circuit in the semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia
.
Brain
2014
;
137
(
Pt 7
):
2065
76
.

Thomas
AG
Dennis
A
Bandettini
PA
Johansen-Berg
H
.
The effects of aerobic activity on brain structure
.
Front Psychol
2012
;
3
:
86
.

Tolppanen
A-MM
Solomon
A
Kulmala
J
Kåreholt
I
Ngandu
T
Rusanen
M
et al. .
Leisure-time physical activity from mid- to late life, body mass index, and risk of dementia
.
Alzheimers Dement
2015
;
11
:
434
443000000
.

Trejo
JL
Carro
E
Torres-Aleman
I
.
Circulating insulin-like growth factor I mediates exercise-induced increases in the number of new neurons in the adult hippocampus
.
J Neurosci
2001
;
21
:
1628
34
.

Treves
A
Tashiro
A
Witter
MP
Moser
EI
.
What is the mammalian dentate gyrus good for?
Neuroscience
2008
;
154
:
1155
72
.

Tsivilis
D
Vann
SD
Denby
C
Roberts
N
Mayes
AR
Montaldi
D
et al. .
A disproportionate role for the fornix and mammillary bodies in recall versus recognition memory
.
Nat Neurosci
2008
;
11
:
834
42
.

van Praag
H
.
Neurogenesis and exercise: past and future directions
.
Neuromolecular Med
2008
;
10
:
128
40
.

van Praag
H
.
Exercise and the brain: something to chew on
.
Trends Neurosci
2009
;
32
:
283
90
.

van Strien
NM
Cappaert
NL
Witter
MP
.
The anatomy of memory: an interactive overview of the parahippocampal-hippocampal network
.
Nature reviews Neuroscience
2009
;
106
:
272
82
.

Vann
SD
Tsivilis
D
Denby
CE
Quamme
JR
Yonelinas
AP
Aggleton
JP
et al. .
Impaired recollection but spared familiarity in patients with extended hippocampal system damage revealed by 3 convergent methods
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2009
;
106
:
5442
7
.

Vidoni
ED
Van Sciver
A
Johnson
DK
He
J
Honea
R
Haines
B
et al. .
A community-based approach to trials of aerobic exercise in aging and Alzheimer's disease
.
Contemp Clin Trials
2012
;
33
:
1105
16
.

Villeda
SA
Plambeck
KE
Middeldorp
J
Castellano
JM
Mosher
KI
Luo
J
et al. .
Young blood reverses age-related impairments in cognitive function and synaptic plasticity in mice
.
Nat Med
2014
;
20
:
659
63
.

Vivar
C
Peterson
DA
van Praag
H
.
Running rewires the neuronal network of adult-born dentate granule cells
.
Neuroimage
2015
.

Vivar
C
Potter
MC
Choi
J
Lee
JY
Stringer
TP
Callaway
EM
et al. .
Monosynaptic inputs to new neurons in the dentate gyrus
.
Nat Commun
2012
;
3
:
1107
.

Vivar
C
Potter
MC
van Praag
H
.
All about running: synaptic plasticity, growth factors and adult hippocampal neurogenesis
.
Curr Top Behav Neurosci
2013
;
15
:
189
210
.

Voss
MW
Erickson
KI
Prakash
RS
Chaddock
L
Kim
JS
Alves
H
et al. .
Neurobiological markers of exercise-related brain plasticity in older adults
.
Brain Behav Immun
2013
;
28
:
90
9
.

Weinstein
AM
Voss
MW
Prakash
RS
Chaddock
L
Szabo
A
White
SM
et al. .
The association between aerobic fitness and executive function is mediated by prefrontal cortex volume
.
Brain Behav Immun
2012
;
26
:
811
9
.

Wenger
E
Schaefer
S
Noack
H
Kuhn
S
Martensson
J
Heinze
HJ
et al. .
Cortical thickness changes following spatial navigation training in adulthood and aging
.
Neuroimage
2012
;
59
:
3389
97
.

WHO
.
Global recommendations on physical activity for health
.
WHO
, editor.
Geneva
:
WHO Press
,
2012
.

Whiteman
AS
Young
DE
Budson
AE
Stern
CE
Schon
K
.
Entorhinal volume, aerobic fitness, and recognition memory in healthy young adults: A voxel-based morphometry study
.
NeuroImage
2015
;
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.049
.

Willette
AA
Bendlin
BB
Starks
EJ
Birdsill
AC
Johnson
SC
Christian
BT
et al. .
Association of insulin resistance with cerebral glucose uptake in late middle-aged adults at risk for Alzheimer disease
.
JAMA Neurol
2015
;
72
:
1013
20
.

Wirth
M
Haase
CM
Villeneuve
S
Vogel
J
Jagust
WJ
.
Neuroprotective pathways: lifestyle activity, brain pathology, and cognition in cognitively normal older adults
.
Neurobiol Aging
2014
;
35
:
1873
82
.

Witter
MP
Canto
CB
Couey
JJ
Koganezawa
N
O'Reilly
KC
.
Architecture of spatial circuits in the hippocampal region
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
2014
;
369
:
20120515
.

Wrann
CD
.
FNDC5/Irisin – their role in the nervous system and as a mediator for beneficial effects of exercise on the brain
.
Brain Plasticity
2015
;
1
:
55
61
.

Wrann
CD
White
JP
Salogiannnis
J
Laznik-Bogoslavski
D
Wu
J
Ma
D
et al. .
Exercise induces hippocampal BDNF through a PGC-1alpha/FNDC5 pathway
.
Cell Metab
2013
;
18
:
649
59
.

Yarchoan
M
Arnold
SE
.
Repurposing diabetes drugs for brain insulin resistance in Alzheimer disease
.
Diabetes
2014
;
63
:
2253
61
.

Yassa
MA
Stark
CE
.
Pattern separation in the hippocampus
.
Trends Neurosci
2011
;
34
:
515
25
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ ), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]