Abstract

Behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia is characterized by a number of ostensibly disparate clinical features, which have largely been considered independently. This update proposes an integrated conceptual framework for these symptoms, by bringing together findings from animal studies, functional neuroimaging and behavioural neurology. The combined evidence indicates that many of the clinical symptoms––such as altered eating behaviour; overspending and susceptibility to scams; reduced empathy and socially inappropriate behaviour; apathy and stereotyped/ritualistic behaviour––can be conceptualized as a common underlying deficiency in goal-directed behaviour and the concomitant emergence of habits. This view is supported by similarities between the characteristic patterns of frontostriatal and insular atrophy in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia and the circuitry of homologous brain regions responsible for goal-directed and habitual behaviour in animals. Appreciating the impact of disturbance in goal-directed behaviour provides a new, integrated understanding of the common mechanisms underpinning prototypical clinical symptoms in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia. Furthermore, by drawing parallels between animal and clinical research, this translational approach has important implications for the development and evaluation of novel therapeutic treatments, from animal models through to behavioural interventions and clinical trials in humans.

Introduction

Frontotemporal dementia refers to a heterogeneous group of neurodegenerative disorders primarily affecting the frontal and temporal lobes of the brain. While three main clinical variants of frontotemporal dementia are recognized, including two language variants (semantic dementia and progressive non-fluent aphasia) and the behavioural variant, the current Update focuses on the latter. Clinical features of behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) have largely been considered independently. This is not surprising, given that symptoms such as increased preference for sweet foods, reduced empathy towards family members and stereotypical/ritualistic behaviour appear, superficially at least, to have little in common. Consequently, research into the symptoms of bvFTD has lacked an integrated behavioural framework, despite the shared involvement of frontostriatal-insular brain regions. In this Update, we bring together evidence from animal studies, functional neuroimaging and behavioural neurology to provide support for a new way of conceptualizing the behavioural changes in bvFTD as due to a disturbance in the capacity for goal-directed behaviour and the concomitant emergence of habits. As such, we highlight the intersection of animal and clinical research in studying goal-directed behaviour and outline a path for future research to systematically investigate goal-directed behaviour in this neurodegenerative syndrome.

Goal-directed behaviour in animals

The capacity for goal-directed behaviour allows us and other animals to control our environment in order to achieve our basic needs and desires. Theories of goal-directed behaviour emphasize knowledge of the ‘causal relationship’ between an action and its consequences, and the ‘desirability’ of those consequences—referred to as ‘belief plus desire’ (Goldman, 1970). This ‘belief plus desire’ account has led to two criteria that can be applied across species, which must be met for behaviour to be considered goal-directed (Balleine and Dickinson, 1998). From this perspective, goal-directed behaviours must be sensitive to manipulations of both (i) the action-outcome contingency; and (ii) the value of the outcome.

Behavioural paradigms used in animal studies. During instrumental training, animals are trained on two action-outcome associations. Following (A) contingency degradation or (B) outcome devaluation procedures, the animal’s performance on a choice test (where frequencies of A1 and A2 are measured) indicates whether their actions are controlled by ‘belief’ or ‘desire’, respectively. A1 = action 1 (left lever), A2 = action 2 (right lever), O1 = outcome 1 (sucrose), O2 = outcome 2 (food pellet).
Figure 1

Behavioural paradigms used in animal studies. During instrumental training, animals are trained on two action-outcome associations. Following (A) contingency degradation or (B) outcome devaluation procedures, the animal’s performance on a choice test (where frequencies of A1 and A2 are measured) indicates whether their actions are controlled by ‘belief’ or ‘desire’, respectively. A1 = action 1 (left lever), A2 = action 2 (right lever), O1 = outcome 1 (sucrose), O2 = outcome 2 (food pellet).

Laboratory-based tests have been developed to reliably detect these features of goal-directed behaviour in rodents, with the two most common being tests of (i) contingency degradation; and (ii) outcome devaluation (Fig. 1 and Box 1). In contingency degradation tests (Fig. 1A), the instrumental performance of both rats and mice is sensitive to selective changes in the causal relation between their actions and their consequences i.e. changes in ‘belief’. In contrast, the outcome devaluation test (Fig. 1B) is designed to measure changes in ‘desire’, and demonstrates that rats and mice are sensitive to selective changes in the value of the outcome. At the neural level, the contingency degradation and outcome devaluation tests have been used to demonstrate that the acquisition and ongoing performance of goal-directed behaviour involves frontostriatal and insular regions.

In rodents, encoding of action-outcome associations occurs in the posterior dorsomedial striatum (cf. Balleine and O'Doherty, 2010) (Fig. 2A). During acquisition, this encoding requires direct cortical inputs to the posterior dorsomedial striatum from the prelimbic prefrontal cortex (Balleine and Dickinson, 1998) and limbic inputs from the basolateral amygdala (Balleine et al., 2003). Once goal-directed actions have been acquired, however, prelimbic prefrontal cortex input to the posterior dorsomedial striatum is no longer required (Ostlund and Balleine, 2005). The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is necessary to recall the outcome associated with a specific action (Ostlund and Balleine, 2007; Bradfield et al., 2015). Interestingly, changes in the value of outcomes involve the basolateral amygdala (Wassum et al., 2009), with this information subsequently encoded in the anterior insular cortex (Balleine and Dickinson, 2000; Parkes and Balleine, 2013). Finally, the insular cortex inputs to the nucleus accumbens for subsequent value-related changes in instrumental performance (Parkes et al., 2015).

Animal and human homologies in goal-directed and habitual behaviour. Brain regions involved in goal-directed (dark blue) and habitual (light blue) behaviour in rats (A) and in humans (B). Areas shaded in red stripes depict the typical pattern of brain atrophy in bvFTD. BLA = basolateral amygdala; DL Str = dorsolateral striatum; DM Str = dorsomedial striatum; IC = insula cortex; NAcc = nucleus accumbens; PL = prelimbic prefrontal cortex.
Figure 2

Animal and human homologies in goal-directed and habitual behaviour. Brain regions involved in goal-directed (dark blue) and habitual (light blue) behaviour in rats (A) and in humans (B). Areas shaded in red stripes depict the typical pattern of brain atrophy in bvFTD. BLA = basolateral amygdala; DL Str = dorsolateral striatum; DM Str = dorsomedial striatum; IC = insula cortex; NAcc = nucleus accumbens; PL = prelimbic prefrontal cortex.

Box 1 Paradigms to assess goal-directed behaviour and habitual behaviour in animals

Contingency degradation test (Fig. 1A): Animals are trained on two action-outcome associations (i.e. A1→O1, A2→O2), after which the relationship between one action and its consequence is selectively degraded, such that performing the action or not, leads to equal probability of the outcome being obtained. As they learn that the specific action (A1) is no longer causal, both rats (cf. Balleine and Dickinson, 1998) and mice (Wiltgen et al., 2007) selectively reduce their performance of action 1 (A1), without changing their performance of action 2 (A2). This demonstrates that animals have learned the specific action-outcome associations.

Outcome devaluation test (Fig. 1B): Animals are trained on two action-outcome associations (i.e. A1→O1, A2→O2), after which the value of one of the two outcomes (e.g. O1) is altered––either via ‘sensory specific satiety’ (e.g. by allowing the animal to consume the food outcome until satiated) or via a ‘conditioned taste aversion treatment’ (e.g. by pairing consumption of the food outcome with an injection of lithium chloride, causing the animal to feel nauseous) (for a review see Balleine, 2009). Both approaches lead to a subsequent selective reduction in the performance of the outcome’s associated action (A1) relative to the other action (A2) (Balleine, 2001; Carvalho et al., 2002). This demonstrates that animals have integrated knowledge of the action-outcome associations with knowledge of the altered value of the outcomes to modify their subsequent behaviour.

Overtraining: Overtraining paradigms have been used to change goal-directed actions into habitual actions. Here, a rat learns that A1→O1, and is subsequently allowed to continue performing A1 for O1 for many days without changes in the action-outcome contingency. As a result, the performance of A1 then becomes habitual, no longer controlled by ‘belief’ or ‘desire’ for O1, which may be assessed by contingency degradation and outcome devaluation tests, respectively (Balleine and O'Doherty, 2010; Dezfouli and Balleine, 2012).

Once goal-directed actions have been established, their continued invariant use often leads to habitual behaviour, such that the action is performed without reference to its outcome (Box 1). At the neural level, habits appear to be mediated by striatal regions that are distinct from those involved in goal-directed actions. Whereas the initial acquisition of action-outcome associations is encoded by the medial prefrontal cortex and dorsomedial striatum, the development of habits depends on a region of the dorsolateral striatum (Yin et al., 2004). During the course of overtraining, critical changes in dopamine D2 receptor activity in the dorsolateral striatum are associated with habits, and reflect changes in postsynaptic plasticity (Shan et al., 2015).

Importantly, it appears that goal-directed and habitual actions form a continuum: as the capacity for goal-directed control diminishes, habits emerge (cf. Balleine et al., 2009). Should conditions change suddenly and unexpectedly, however, the goal-directed circuitry can re-engage and inhibit habitual behaviour. Notably, habits can emerge not only as a consequence of overtraining, but also as a consequence of damage to the neural system that mediates goal-directed action (Ostlund and Balleine, 2008). As such, habits are actions that continue to be performed despite no longer being associated with favourable outcomes. In this case, the habits that emerge are clearly maladaptive; they are no longer subject to regulation by the goal-directed system and often continue (i.e. perseverate) in situations where such performance is not optimal (Corbit et al., 2014; Furlong et al., 2015). In sum, a large body of work using animal models has led to the development of paradigms that reliably assess goal-directed and habitual behaviour and their associated neural correlates.

Goal-directed behaviour in humans

Findings from functional neuroimaging studies in humans complement the animal literature, highlighting the importance of regions such as the OFC, insular cortex, amygdala and striatum in coding and maintaining reward representations that guide flexible, goal-directed behaviour (O'Doherty, 2004; Balleine and O'Doherty, 2010) (Fig. 2B). These regions have been compared in terms of their connectivity and function in a variety of species, including rodents, primates and humans. Although there have been controversies in this literature (cf. Wise, 2008), there is a developing consensus that regions of interest in goal-directed and habitual behaviour are homologous across species. While a full review of this literature is beyond the scope of this Update, evidence indicates that the rodent dorsomedial and dorsolateral striatum are homologous to human caudate and putamen, respectively (Balleine and O'Doherty, 2010; Woolley et al., 2013; Heilbronner et al., 2016); that rodent medial and ventrolateral OFC regions are homologous to the visceromotor and sensory OFC networks described in primates and humans, respectively (Öngür and Price, 2000; Heilbronner et al., 2016); and that the anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortices and the cortico-striatal emotional network are all also homologous across species (Schwarz et al., 2013; Woolley et al., 2013; Vogt and Paxinos, 2014; Heilbronner et al., 2016).

Contingency degradation tests, which vary the causal relationship between button presses and monetary reward over time, have been used in healthy adults (Tanaka et al., 2008; Liljeholm et al., 2011). These studies showed greater activity in the medial OFC and dorsomedial striatum (anterior caudate nucleus) when consequences are highly contingent on specific actions compared to when the action-outcome contingency is low. In addition, outcome devaluation procedures have demonstrated increased OFC activity in healthy adults during the selection of valued compared to devalued actions (Valentin et al., 2007), and impaired value-directed choice in patients with focal lesions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, encompassing the OFC (Reber et al., 2017).

Regions involved in habitual behaviour in humans have also been identified. In a functional MRI study, we have previously shown differential effects of training duration on sensitivity to outcome value (Tricomi et al., 2009). Some participants were given relatively little training to press buttons to earn snack foods, whereas others were overtrained. Behaviourally, the less trained participants showed a typical outcome devaluation effect, whereby sensitivity to devaluation was associated with activity in the medial prefrontal cortex and caudate nucleus. In contrast, the performance of overtrained participants was insensitive to outcome devaluation and activity in the right posterior putamen was predictive of overtraining-induced performance. These areas are homologous to those that mediate goal-directed and habitual action in rodents.

Of relevance here, in healthy humans, the application of the abovementioned contingency degradation and outcome devaluation paradigms (Valentin et al., 2007; Klossek et al., 2008; Tanaka et al., 2008; Tricomi et al., 2009; Liljeholm et al., 2011; Friedel et al., 2014; Eder and Dignath, 2015) and other related behavioural paradigms (de Wit et al., 2009, 2014; Eryilmaz et al., 2017) has demonstrated how tests of goal-directed and habitual actions can be translated from animal models to human behaviour. While such paradigms have been used in clinical settings to investigate drug addiction (Hogarth and Chase, 2011), obsessive compulsive disorder (Gillan et al., 2011), autism spectrum disorder (Geurts and de Wit, 2014; Alvares et al., 2016), schizophrenia (Morris et al., 2015) and Parkinson’s disease (de Wit et al., 2011), this translational approach is yet to be applied in frontotemporal dementia.

The clinical phenotype of behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia

The behavioural variant is the most common clinical variant of frontotemporal dementia (Coyle-Gilchrist et al., 2016). Patients with bvFTD present with progressive deterioration in social behaviour and personal conduct. Clinical diagnostic criteria for bvFTD (Rascovsky et al., 2011) describe six core symptoms, including: (i) behavioural disinhibition (e.g. socially inappropriate behaviour, loss of manners or decorum and impulsive behaviour); (ii) early apathy or inertia; (iii) early loss of sympathy or empathy; (iv) early perseverative, stereotyped or ritualistic behaviour; (v) hyperorality and dietary changes; and (vi) executive dysfunction. At least three of these six features are necessary for diagnosis, though the presence and severity of each symptom may vary across patients and disease stages (Banks and Weintraub, 2008; Mioshi et al., 2010). Considerable pathological heterogeneity also exists, such that those presenting with bvFTD have an almost equal chance of having underlying tau or TAR DNA-binding protein 43 (TDP-43) pathology, with a small proportion of cases having fused in sarcoma (FUS) pathology (Chare et al., 2014; Mackenzie and Neumann, 2016). However, a number of patients presenting with clinical features of bvFTD reportedly show underlying Alzheimer’s disease pathology (Forman et al., 2006; Chare et al., 2014). While up to 40% of patients have a positive family history of dementia, pathogenic gene mutations––including microtubule-associated protein tau (MAPT), granulin (GRN) and C9orf72––account for only 10–20% of cases (Wood et al., 2013; Po et al., 2014). Despite this pathological and genetic heterogeneity, however, anatomical profiles of brain atrophy appear to be relatively similar across tau and TDP-43 subtypes of FTD (Harper et al., 2017). Typically, atrophy is present in the frontal and/or anterior temporal cortices (Rascovsky et al., 2011), as well as anterior insular (Seeley, 2010) and striatal regions (Halabi et al., 2013). Of relevance here, these brain regions overlap with those implicated in goal-directed behaviour in rodent studies (Fig. 2B). Here, we propose that many of the clinical symptoms of bvFTD can be conceptualized as a deficiency in goal-directed behaviour, with a concomitant increase in habitual behaviour (Fig. 3).

New framework for conceptualizing symptoms of bvFTD in terms of goal-directed and habitual behaviour. Goal-directed behaviours must satisfy the ‘belief plus desire’ criteria, whereas habitual behaviours are insensitive to changes in action-outcome contingency and/or changes in outcome value. Altered goal-directed behaviour for primary and secondary rewards may underlie symptoms such as overeating, hypo- or hypersexuality, overspending or risky financial decision making, reduced empathy, socially-appropriate behaviour and apathy. As the capacity for goal-directed control of behaviour diminishes, habits emerge and may manifest as stereotypical or perseverative behaviours.
Figure 3

New framework for conceptualizing symptoms of bvFTD in terms of goal-directed and habitual behaviour. Goal-directed behaviours must satisfy the ‘belief plus desire’ criteria, whereas habitual behaviours are insensitive to changes in action-outcome contingency and/or changes in outcome value. Altered goal-directed behaviour for primary and secondary rewards may underlie symptoms such as overeating, hypo- or hypersexuality, overspending or risky financial decision making, reduced empathy, socially-appropriate behaviour and apathy. As the capacity for goal-directed control of behaviour diminishes, habits emerge and may manifest as stereotypical or perseverative behaviours.

Goal-directed behaviour in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia

Disturbance in goal-directed behaviour in bvFTD may be due to altered sensitivity to reward or deficits in the ability to update actions according to changes in reward value. In bvFTD patients, increased pursuit of primary rewards––including food, drugs and sex––is related to atrophy in the putamen, globus pallidus, insular cortex and thalamus, which are typically implicated in reward-processing (Perry et al., 2014). Similarly, symptoms of overeating are associated with atrophy in right orbitofrontal-insular-striatal regions (Whitwell et al., 2007; Woolley et al., 2007). A wider network of regions has been implicated in increased caloric intake and preference for sweet foods, including the cingulate cortices, thalamus and cerebellum for the former, and bilateral OFC and right insular-striatal regions for the latter (Ahmed et al., 2016). Despite this involvement of OFC regions, which have been implicated in failures of inhibitory control (Hornberger et al., 2010), overeating does not appear to be simply related to disinhibition (Whitwell et al., 2007; Ahmed et al., 2016). In the context of goal-directed behaviour, overeating may reflect insensitivity to the decreasing reward value of food or an inability to flexibly integrate updated reward values with goal-directed actions. Furthermore, while decreased sex drive is commonly reported in bvFTD, a subset of patients present with hypersexuality and aberrant/unusual sexual behaviours (Mendez and Shapira, 2013; Ahmed et al., 2015). Although the causes for altered pursuit of sex in bvFTD are likely multifactorial, involving endocrine dysfunction as well as reduced empathy and increased apathy (Ahmed et al., 2015), the potential contribution of altered sensitivity to rewards or changes in rewards has not been explored.

Given the overlap in brain networks involved in primary and secondary rewards (Sescousse et al., 2013), it is unsurprising that patients with bvFTD also show changes in their pursuit of secondary rewards, such as money and social acceptance. Increased financial risk-taking behaviour is well documented using monetary reward-related decision-making tasks (e.g. Iowa Gambling Task, Cambridge Gambling Task), where patients tend to choose risky options that lead to monetary losses (Rahman et al., 1999; Torralva et al., 2009; Kloeters et al., 2013). However, as failure on these gambling tasks can also reflect broader executive deficits (Perry and Kramer, 2013), targeted measures of monetary reward sensitivity are necessary. In terms of everyday behaviour, patients with bvFTD often spend excessively (Chiong et al., 2014) and are highly susceptible to financial exploitation (Wong et al., 2017). Although cognitive factors including impulsivity, disinhibition, and executive dysfunction may contribute to overspending or risky financial behaviour, such behaviours may also be due to reduced sensitivity to the negative reward value of financial losses, or an inability to flexibly integrate changes in reward value with actions. As such, experimental paradigms that enable specific assessment of components of goal-directed behaviour will help disentangle the reward-related versus cognitive contributions to poor financial decision-making in bvFTD.

Social cognitive dysfunction is well established in bvFTD, with widespread impairments in theory of mind, emotion recognition, empathy and complex social reasoning (Lough et al., 2006; Bertoux et al., 2012; Kumfor and Piguet, 2012; Kumfor et al., 2013; Dermody et al., 2016; Melloni et al., 2016; O'Callaghan et al., 2016). Considering the multi-faceted nature of social cognition, it is plausible that social cognitive deficits in bvFTD are influenced by altered processing of social rewards, although this has been relatively unexplored. Using an Incentive Delay paradigm that required patients to respond to a target cue in order to gain or avoid social or monetary rewards or punishment, Perry et al. (2015) demonstrated greater sensitivity to monetary compared to social rewards. BvFTD patients also show reduced sensitivity to negative social outcomes in social scenarios (e.g. cutting in line at a movie theatre) (Grossman et al., 2010) and reduced prosocial behaviour (e.g. sharing resources when personal gain is not at stake, or giving money to less-fortunate others) (O'Callaghan et al., 2016; Sturm et al., 2017). Moreover, we recently demonstrated that although bvFTD patients may learn and remember aspects of socially rewarding/punishing information (e.g. whether a person reciprocates or violates trust), they do not appear to apply this knowledge to modulate their behaviour (Wong et al., 2017). Of relevance here, failure to modify behaviour within specific social contexts has been proposed to contribute to social cognitive deficits in bvFTD (Ibanez and Manes, 2012). Whether this extends to encompass a failure to modify behaviour in relation to social rewards, however, remains to be established. As such, bvFTD patients may fail to modulate behaviour in order to avoid negative social consequences, or may not be motivated by rewarding social outcomes—an area which warrants further investigation.

Collectively, altered sensitivity to primary and/or secondary rewards, or deficits in the ability to modify behaviour in accordance with changes in reward value, may contribute to reduced motivation in bvFTD. Although apathy is one of the most prevalent and disabling non-cognitive symptoms in bvFTD (Merrilees et al., 2013), limited research has systematically explored this symptom in light of current theories of goal-directed behaviour. One study used the Philadelphia Apathy Computerised Test (PACT), a computerized reaction time task, to examine initiation, planning and motivation (Massimo, 2015). Impaired performance across all three components of the task was associated with widespread OFC, lateral prefrontal cortex and cingulate atrophy. However, the PACT does not allow assessment of the ‘belief plus desire’ criterion for goal-directed behaviour. Similarly, although a recent study by Perry et al. (2017) demonstrated reduced subjective and physiological responses to aversive olfactory stimuli, their behavioural paradigm did not distinguish between pursuit of rewards in valued versus devalued conditions. Thus, the relationship between goal-directed behaviour and symptoms of apathy requires further elucidation.

While many symptoms of bvFTD may be underpinned by deficiencies in goal-directed behaviour, we propose that a concomitant increase in habitual behaviour may underlie symptoms of perseverative, stereotyped or compulsive/ritualistic behaviours (Rascovsky et al., 2011). Examples of such behaviour include preoccupation with counting and/or clock watching, restricted interests in leisure activities or hobbies and rigid adherence to routines or food preferences (Nyatsanza et al., 2003). While it has been suggested that stereotypical behaviour, overeating and disinhibition are underpinned by changes to the same OFC-insular-amygdala-striatal network, (Ames et al., 1994; Nyatsanza et al., 2003), this requires confirmation through structural and functional neuroimaging studies that can distinguish between goal-directed and habitual behaviour. Specifically, insensitivity to the ‘belief plus desire’ criteria may give rise to habits that continue in circumstances where such behaviours are not optimal. While this is yet to be examined in bvFTD, evidence in autism spectrum disorder, which is also characterized by stereotypical or restricted patterns of behaviour, supports this hypothesis (Alvares et al., 2016). In this study, individuals with autism spectrum disorder showed impaired performance on an outcome devaluation test, such that they continued to perform the action associated with the devalued outcome. The authors suggested that symptoms of rigidity and stereotypic behaviour may be underpinned by reduced flexibility in integrating outcome values with actions, thereby rendering actions more habitual. In the same vein, it is possible that reduced flexibility in updating actions in accordance with value-related information may also contribute to symptoms of perseverative, stereotyped or compulsive/ritualistic behaviour in bvFTD, a hypothesis that warrants future investigation.

Challenges and future directions

A well established body of evidence from the animal literature has developed targeted experimental paradigms and identified the neural circuitry that mediates specific components of goal-directed behaviour. The translation of this approach to investigating the component processes and neural bases of symptoms of abnormal behaviour in bvFTD offers a new perspective on these symptoms (Fig. 3), yet a number of key issues are yet to be systematically explored.

Whether bvFTD patients are sensitive to changes in the relationship between actions and their outcomes is yet to be established. Current evidence indicates that bvFTD patients perform poorly on tests of reversal learning, where two previously learned action-outcome contingencies are reversed, such that the previously rewarded action is now punished, and vice versa (Rahman et al., 1999; Bertoux et al., 2013). Nonetheless, reversal learning paradigms do not distinguish between inflexibility in modulating actions in relation to the ‘value’ of their outcomes versus general cognitive inflexibility. As such, it is unclear whether similar deficits would be observed if only one of two previously learned action-outcome contingencies were selectively degraded, rather than reversed. Adapting a contingency degradation paradigm would, therefore, clarify whether bvFTD patients are able to selectively reduce performance of an action when they learn that it is no longer causal. Similarly, the extent that bvFTD patients are sensitive to changes in the value of outcomes remains to be established through using an outcome devaluation paradigm.

Whether changes in goal-directed behaviour in bvFTD vary according to sensitivity to different types of reward (e.g. primary versus secondary) is also only beginning to be explored. Specifically, the question of why patients may become hypersensitive to some rewards, but hyposensitive to others, warrants investigation. For example, while the majority of patients show symptoms of overeating, this may not necessarily occur in 100% of cases, nor does increased sex drive or increased propensity to make financial decisions. Indeed, some patients show more rigid eating behaviour (Shinagawa et al., 2009; Ahmed et al., 2016), hyposexuality (Ahmed et al., 2015) or minimal deficits in financial decision-making (Chiong et al., 2014). Such changes are likely to be relative, rather than an absolute preference for primary versus secondary rewards, and may reflect the clinical and pathological heterogeneity of bvFTD, or an exacerbation of pre-existing individual differences in sensitivity to different types of reward. Assessing the relative value of different reward types, and their impact on goal-directed behaviour in these patients, via translation of animal paradigms, will help inform the development of therapeutic interventions that target reward-related symptoms in bvFTD, and give important insights into how primary versus secondary rewards are represented at a neurobiological level. From a theoretical standpoint, future studies that contrast goal-directed and habitual behaviour across different neurodegenerative and neuropsychiatric disorders associated with frontostriatal deficits may also help identify transdiagnostic intervention approaches.

The evidence reviewed above provides support for a new conceptualization of the apparently diverse symptoms of bvFTD as a failure in a common underlying mechanism subserving the capacity for goal-directed behaviour. This conceptualization represents a new opportunity for developing treatments to address these challenging symptoms and for informing appropriate paradigms for animal disease models of bvFTD. First, by understanding the common mechanisms that underlie different symptoms, pharmacological interventions may be trialled and in turn appropriately evaluated. Of relevance here, modulation of striatal dopaminergic pathways has proved effective in Parkinson’s disease (for a review, see Chaudhuri et al., 2006). In bvFTD, striatal atrophy (Halabi et al., 2013) and dopamine deficiencies (Rinne et al., 2002) have been reported. Preliminary results suggest that dopamine modulation may benefit symptoms of apathy, disinhibition and risk-taking behaviour (Rahman et al., 2006; Huey et al., 2008; Lin et al., 2016). Nonetheless, larger randomized, placebo-controlled studies will be necessary to determine the efficacy of dopamine therapy for treatment of behavioural symptoms in bvFTD (for a review, see Tsai and Boxer, 2014). Moreover, behavioural interventions that apply principles of goal-directed behaviour and differential reinforcement, such as the Antecedent-Behaviour-Consequence (ABC) model of behaviour management (Logsdon et al., 2007), have been proposed for management of behavioural symptoms in bvFTD (Merrilees, 2007), but require validation through large-scale controlled studies. Secondly, whereas behavioural tests for animal models of Alzheimer’s disease have used hippocampally-dependent learning and memory tests, such tests are unlikely to be suitable for animal models of bvFTD (Roberson, 2012; Ahmed et al., 2017). The incorporation of behavioural tests of goal-directed behaviour, such as the contingency degradation and outcome devaluation tests, to clarify whether bvFTD patients are sensitive to the two criteria for goal-directed action [i.e. (i) sensitivity to action-outcome contingency; and (ii) sensitivity to changes in outcome value] may therefore prove fruitful in evaluating the efficacy of disease-modifying pharmacological treatments. This has important implications for animal drug trials, given the need for appropriate behavioural tests in enabling direct comparisons between animal models and clinical investigations of this disease. The translational approach outlined here, therefore represents an innovative research avenue to develop novel therapeutic interventions addressing the common factor of goal-directed behaviour and to assess the efficacy of such treatments simultaneously, from animal models through to clinical trials in humans.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Prof John R. Hodges, who provided helpful comments on a previous version of the manuscript, and Heidi Cartwright, who helped create the figures.

Funding

This work was supported in part by funding to ForeFront, a collaborative research group dedicated to the study of frontotemporal dementia and motor neuron disease, from the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) (APP1037746) and the Australian Research Council (ARC) Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders Memory Program (CE11000102). In addition, this study was supported by NHMRC Project Grant (APP1121791). F.K. is supported by an NHMRC-ARC Dementia Research Development Fellowship (APP1097026). B.W.B. is a NHMRC Senior Principal Research Fellow (GNT1079561).

Abbreviations

    Abbreviations
     
  • bvFTD

    behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia

  •  
  • OFC

    orbitofrontal cortex

References

Ahmed
RM
,
Irish
M
,
Henning
E
,
Dermody
N
,
Bartley
L
,
Kiernan
MC
et al. .
Assessment of eating behavior disturbance and associated neural networks in frontotemporal dementia
.
JAMA Neurol
2016
;
73
:
282
9
.

Ahmed
RM
,
Irish
M
,
van Eersel
J
,
Ittner
A
,
Ke
YD
,
Volkerling
A
et al. .
Mouse models of frontotemporal dementia: a comparison of phenotypes with clinical symptomatology
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
2017
;
74
:
126
38
.

Ahmed
RM
,
Kaizik
C
,
Irish
M
,
Mioshi
E
,
Dermody
N
,
Kiernan
MC
et al. .
Characterizing sexual behavior in frontotemporal dementia
.
J Alzheimers Dis
2015
;
46
:
677
86
.

Alvares
GA
,
Balleine
BW
,
Whittle
L
,
Guastella
AJ
.
Reduced goal-directed action control in autism spectrum disorder
.
Autism Res
2016
;
9
:
1285
93
.

Ames
D
,
Cummings
JL
,
Wirshing
WC
,
Quinn
B
,
Mahler
M
.
Repetitive and compulsive behavior in frontal-lobe degenerations
.
J Neuropsychiatry
1994
;
6
:
100
13
.

Balleine
BW
.
Incentive processes in instrumental conditioning
. In:
Mowrer
R
,
Klein
S
, editors.
Handbook of contemporary learning theories
.
Mahwah, NJ
:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
;
2001
. p.
307
66
.

Balleine
BW
.
Taste, disgust and value: taste aversion learning and outcome encoding in instrumental conditioning
. In:
Reilly
S
,
Schachtman
TR
, editors.
Conditioned taste aversion
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
;
2009
. p.
262
80
.

Balleine
BW
,
Dickinson
A
.
Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates
.
Neuropharmacology
1998
;
37
:
407
19
.

Balleine
BW
,
Dickinson
A
.
The effect of lesions of the insular cortex on instrumental conditioning: evidence for a role in incentive memory
.
J Neurosci
2000
;
20
:
8954
64
.

Balleine
BW
,
Killcross
AS
,
Dickinson
A
.
The effect of lesions of the basolateral amygdala on instrumental conditioning
.
J Neurosci
2003
;
23
:
666
75
.

Balleine
BW
,
Liljeholm
M
,
Ostlund
SB
.
The integrative function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning
.
Behav Brain Res
2009
;
199
:
43
52
.

Balleine
BW
,
O'Doherty
JP
.
Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
2010
;
35
:
48
69
.

Banks
SJ
,
Weintraub
S
.
Neuropsychiatric symptoms in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and primary progressive aphasia
.
J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol
2008
;
21
:
133
41
.

Bertoux
M
,
Funkiewiez
A
,
O'Callaghan
C
,
Dubois
B
,
Hornberger
M
.
Sensitivity and specificity of ventromedial prefrontal cortex tests in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Alzheimers Dement
2013
;
9
:
S84
94
.

Bertoux
M
,
Volle
E
,
Funkiewiez
A
,
de Souza
LC
,
Leclercq
D
,
Dubois
B
.
Social Cognition and Emotional Assessment (SEA) is a marker of medial and orbital frontal functions: a voxel-based morphometry study in behavioral variant of frontotemporal degeneration
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc
2012
;
18
:
972
85
.

Bradfield
LA
,
Dezfouli
A
,
van Holstein
M
,
Chieng
B
,
Balleine
BW
.
Medial orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome retrieval in partially observable task situations
.
Neuron
2015
;
88
:
1268
80
.

Carvalho
OM
,
Silva
AJ
,
Balleine
BW
.
Evidence of selective learning deficits on tests of Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning in α-CaMKIIT286A mutant mice
.
Int J Comp Psychol
2002
;
14
:
161
74
.

Chare
L
,
Hodges
JR
,
Leyton
CE
,
McGinley
C
,
Tan
RH
,
Kril
JJ
et al. .
New criteria for frontotemporal dementia syndromes: clinical and pathological diagnostic implications
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
2014
;
85
:
865
70
.

Chaudhuri
KR
,
Healy
DG
,
Schapira
AHV
,
National Institute for Clinical Excellence. Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson's disease: diagnosis and management
.
Lancet Neurol
2006
;
5
:
235
45
.

Chiong
W
,
Hsu
M
,
Wudka
D
,
Miller
BL
,
Rosen
HJ
.
Financial errors in dementia: testing a neuroeconomic conceptual framework
.
Neurocase
2014
;
20
:
389
96
.

Corbit
LH
,
Chieng
BC
,
Balleine
BW
.
Effects of repeated cocaine exposure on habit learning and reversal by N-acetylcysteine
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
2014
;
39
:
1893
901
.

Coyle-Gilchrist
IT
,
Dick
KM
,
Patterson
K
,
Vázquez Rodríquez
P
,
Wehmann
E
,
Wilcox
A
et al. .
Prevalence, characteristics, and survival of frontotemporal lobar degeneration syndromes
.
Neurology
2016
;
86
:
1736
43
.

de Wit
S
,
Corlett
PR
,
Aitken
MR
,
Dickinson
A
,
Fletcher
PC
.
Differential engagement of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex by goal-directed and habitual behavior toward food pictures in humans
.
J Neurosci
2009
;
29
:
11330
8
.

de Wit
S
,
Standing
HR
,
DeVito
EE
,
Robinson
OJ
,
Ridderinkhof
KR
,
Robbins
TW
et al. .
Reliance on habits at the expense of goal-directed control following dopamine precursor depletion
.
Psychopharmacology
2011
;
219
:
621
31
.

de Wit
S
,
van de Vijver
I
,
Ridderinkhof
KR
.
Impaired acquisition of goal-directed action in healthy aging
.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
2014
;
14
:
647
58
.

Dermody
N
,
Wong
S
,
Ahmed
R
,
Piguet
O
,
Hodges
JR
,
Irish
M
.
Uncovering the neural bases of cognitive and affective empathy deficits in alzheimer’s disease and the behavioral-variant of frontotemporal dementia
.
J Alzheimers Dis
2016
;
53
:
801
16
.

Dezfouli
A
,
Balleine
BW
.
Habits, action sequences and reinforcement learning
.
Eur J Neurosci
2012
;
35
:
1036
51
.

Eder
AB
,
Dignath
D
.
Cue-elicited food seeking is eliminated with aversive outcomes following outcome devaluation
.
Q J Exp Psychol
2015
;
69
:
574
88
.

Eryilmaz
H
,
Rodriguez-Thompson
A
,
Tanner
AS
,
Giegold
M
,
Huntington
FC
,
Roffman
JL
.
Neural determinants of human goal-directed vs. habitual action control and their relation to trait motivation
.
Sci Rep
2017
;
7
:
6002
.

Forman
MS
,
Farmer
J
,
Johnson
JK
,
Clark
CM
,
Arnold
SE
,
Coslett
HB
et al. .
Frontotemporal dementia: clinicopathological correlations
.
Ann Neurol
2006
;
59
:
952
62
.

Friedel
E
,
Koch
SP
,
Wendt
J
,
Heinz
A
,
Deserno
L
,
Schlagenhauf
F
.
Devaluation and sequential decisions: linking goal-directed and model-based behavior
.
Front Hum Neurosci
2014
;
8
:
587
.

Furlong
TM
,
Supit
ASA
,
Corbit
LH
,
Killcross
S
,
Balleine
BW
.
Pulling habits out of rats: adenosine 2A receptor antagonism in dorsomedial striatum rescues meth-amphetamine-induced deficits in goal-directed action
.
Addict Biol
2015
;
22
:
172
83
.

Geurts
HM
,
de Wit
S
.
Goal-directed action control in children with autism spectrum disorders
.
Autism
2014
;
18
:
409
18
.

Gillan
CM
,
Papmeyer
M
,
Morein-Zamir
S
,
Sahakian
BJ
,
Fineberg
NA
,
Robbins
TW
et al. .
Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder
.
Am J Psychiatry
2011
;
168
:
718
26
.

Goldman
AI
.
A theory of human action
.
Princeton, NJ
:
Prentice-Hall
;
1970
.

Grossman
M
,
Eslinger
PJ
,
Troiani
V
,
Anderson
C
,
Avants
B
,
Gee
JC
et al. .
The role of ventral medial prefrontal cortex in social decisions: converging evidence from fMRI and frontotemporal lobar degeneration
.
Neuropsychologia
2010
;
48
:
3505
12
.

Halabi
C
,
Halabi
A
,
Dean
DL
,
Wang
PN
,
Boxer
AL
,
Trojanowski
JQ
et al. .
Patterns of striatal degeneration in frontotemporal dementia
.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
2013
;
27
:
74
83
.

Harper
L
,
Bouwman
F
,
Burton
EJ
,
Barkhof
F
,
Scheltens
P
,
O'Brien
JT
et al. .
Patterns of atrophy in pathologically confirmed dementias: a voxelwise analysis
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
2017
;
88
:
908
16
.

Heilbronner
SR
,
Rodriguez-Romaguera
J
,
Quirk
GJ
,
Groenewegen
HJ
,
Haber
SN
.
Circuit-based corticostriatal homologies between rat and primate
.
Biol Psychiatry
2016
;
80
:
509
21
.

Hogarth
L
,
Chase
HW
.
Parallel goal-directed and habitual control of human drug-seeking: implications for dependence vulnerability
.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process
2011
;
37
:
261
76
.

Hornberger
M
,
Savage
S
,
Hsieh
S
,
Mioshi
E
,
Piguet
O
,
Hodges
JR
.
Orbitofrontal dysfunction discriminates behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
2010
;
30
:
547
52
.

Huey
ED
,
Garcia
C
,
Wassermann
EM
,
Tierney
MC
,
Grafman
J
.
Stimulant treatment of frontotemporal dementia in 8 patients
.
J Clin Psychiatry
2008
;
69
:
1981
2
.

Ibanez
A
,
Manes
F
.
Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
2012
;
78
:
1354
62
.

Kloeters
S
,
Bertoux
M
,
O'Callaghan
C
,
Hodges
JR
,
Hornberger
M
.
Money for nothing—atrophy correlates of gambling decision making in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease
.
Neuroimage Clin
2013
;
2
:
263
72
.

Klossek
UMH
,
Russell
J
,
Dickinson
A
.
The control of instrumental action following outcome devaluation in young children aged between 1 and 4 years
.
J Exp Psychol Gen
2008
;
137
:
39
51
.

Kumfor
F
,
Irish
M
,
Hodges
JR
,
Piguet
O
.
Discrete neural correlates for the recognition of negative emotions: insights from frontotemporal dementia
.
PLoS One
2013
;
8
:
e67457
11
.

Kumfor
F
,
Piguet
O
.
Disturbance of emotion processing in frontotemporal dementia: a synthesis of cognitive and neuroimaging findings
.
Neuropsychol Rev
2012
;
22
:
280
97
.

Liljeholm
M
,
Tricomi
E
,
O'Doherty
JP
,
Balleine
BW
.
Neural correlates of instrumental contingency learning: differential effects of action-reward conjunction and disjunction
.
J Neurosci
2011
;
31
:
2474
80
.

Lin
CP
,
Chu
CP
,
Liu
HC
.
Bupropion improved apathy in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a case report
.
Neurocase
2016
;
22
:
466
8
.

Logsdon
RG
,
McCurry
SM
,
Teri
L
.
Evidence-based psychological treatments for disruptive behaviors in individuals with dementia
.
Psychol Aging
2007
;
22
:
28
36
.

Lough
S
,
Kipps
CM
,
Treise
C
,
Watson
P
,
Blair
JR
,
Hodges
JR
.
Social reasoning, emotion and empathy in frontotemporal dementia
.
Neuropsychologia
2006
;
44
:
950
8
.

Mackenzie
IRA
,
Neumann
M
.
Molecular neuropathology of frontotemporal dementia: insights into disease mechanisms from postmortem studies
.
J Neurochem
2016
;
138
(Suppl 1):
54
70
.

Massimo
L
.
Apathy in frontotemporal degeneration: neuroanatomical evidence of impaired goal-directed behavior
.
Front Hum Neurosci
2015
;
9
:
611
.

Melloni
M
,
Billeke
P
,
Baez
S
,
Hesse
E
,
la Fuente de
L
,
Forno
G
et al. .
Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining
.
Brain
2016
;
139
:
3022
40
.

Mendez
MF
,
Shapira
JS
.
Hypersexual behavior in frontotemporal dementia: a comparison with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease
.
Arch Sex Behav
2013
;
42
:
501
9
.

Merrilees
J
.
A model for management of behavioral symptoms in frontotemporal lobar degeneration
.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
2007
;
21
:
S64
9
.

Merrilees
J
,
Dowling
GA
,
Hubbard
E
,
Mastick
J
,
Ketelle
R
,
Miller
BL
.
Characterization of apathy in persons with frontotemporal dementia and the impact on family caregivers
.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
2013
;
27
:
62
7
.

Mioshi
E
,
Hsieh
S
,
Savage
S
,
Hornberger
M
,
Hodges
JR
.
Clinical staging and disease progression in frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
2010
;
74
:
1591
7
.

Morris
RW
,
Quail
S
,
Griffiths
KR
,
Green
MJ
,
Balleine
BW
.
Corticostriatal control of goal-directed action is impaired in schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry
2015
;
77
:
187
95
.

Nyatsanza
S
,
Shetty
T
,
Gregory
C
,
Lough
S
,
Dawson
K
,
Hodges
JR
.
A study of stereotypic behaviours in Alzheimer’s disease and frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
2003
;
74
:
1398
402
.

O'Callaghan
C
,
Bertoux
M
,
Irish
M
,
Shine
JM
,
Wong
S
,
Spiliopoulos
L
et al. .
Fair play: social norm compliance failures in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
2016
;
139
:
204
16
.

94

O'Doherty
JP
.
Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
2004
;
14
:
769
76
.

Ostlund
SB
,
Balleine
BW
.
Lesions of medial prefrontal cortex disrupt the acquisition but not the expression of goal-directed learning
.
J Neurosci
2005
;
25
:
7763
70
.

Ostlund
SB
,
Balleine
BW
.
Orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome encoding in Pavlovian but not instrumental conditioning
.
J Neurosci
2007
;
27
:
4819
25
.

Ostlund
SB
,
Balleine
BW
.
On habits and addiction: an associative analysis of compulsive drug seeking
.
Drug Discov Today Dis Models
2008
;
5
:
235
45
.

Öngür
D
,
Price
JL
.
The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans
.
Cereb Cortex
2000
;
10
:
206
19
.

Parkes
SL
,
Balleine
BW
.
Incentive memory: evidence the basolateral amygdala encodes and the insular cortex retrieves outcome values to guide choice between goal-directed actions
.
J Neurosci
2013
;
33
:
8753
63
.

Parkes
SL
,
Bradfield
LA
,
Balleine
BW
.
Interaction of insular cortex and ventral striatum mediates the effect of incentive memory on choice between goal-directed actions
.
J Neurosci
2015
;
35
:
6464
71
.

Perry
DC
,
Datta
S
,
Sturm
VE
,
Wood
KA
,
Zakrzewski
J
,
Seeley
WW
et al. .
Reward deficits in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia include insensitivity to negative stimuli
.
Brain
2017
;
140
:
3346
56
.

Perry
DC
,
Kramer
JH
.
Reward processing in neurodegenerative disease
.
Neurocase
2013
;
21
:
120
33
.

Perry
DC
,
Sturm
VE
,
Seeley
WW
,
Miller
BL
,
Kramer
JH
,
Rosen
HJ
.
Anatomical correlates of reward-seeking behaviours in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
2014
;
137
:
1621
6
.

Perry
DC
,
Sturm
VE
,
Wood
KA
,
Miller
BL
,
Kramer
JH
.
Divergent processing of monetary and social reward in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease
.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
2015
;
29
:
161
4
.

Po
K
,
Leslie
FVC
,
Gracia
N
,
Bartley
L
,
Kwok
JBJ
,
Halliday
GM
et al. .
Heritability in frontotemporal dementia: more missing pieces?
J Neurol
2014
;
261
:
2170
7
.

Rahman
S
,
Robbins
TW
,
Hodges
JR
,
Mehta
MA
,
Nestor
PJ
,
Clark
L
et al. .
Methylphenidate (‘Ritalin’) can ameliorate abnormal risk-taking behavior in the frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
2006
;
31
:
651
8
.

Rahman
S
,
Sahakian
BJ
,
Hodges
JR
,
Rogers
RD
,
Robbins
TW
.
Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
1999
;
122
:
1469
93
.

Rascovsky
K
,
Hodges
JR
,
Knopman
D
,
Mendez
MF
,
Kramer
JH
,
Neuhaus
J
et al. .
Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
2011
;
134
:
2456
77
.

Reber
J
,
Feinstein
JS
,
O'Doherty
JP
,
Liljeholm
M
,
Adolphs
R
,
Tranel
D
.
Selective impairment of goal-directed decision-making following lesions to the human ventromedial prefrontal cortex
.
Brain
2017
;
140
:
1743
56
.

Rinne
JO
,
Laine
M
,
Kaasinen
V
,
Norvasuo-Heilä
MK
,
Någren
K
,
Helenius
H
.
Striatal dopamine transporter and extrapyramidal symptoms in frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
2002
;
58
:
1489
93
.

Roberson
ED
.
Mouse models of frontotemporal dementia
.
Ann Neurol
2012
;
72
:
837
49
.

Schwarz
AJ
,
Gass
N
,
Sartorius
A
,
Risterucci
C
,
Spedding
M
,
Schenker
E
, et al.
Anti-correlated cortical networks of intrinsic connectivity in the rat brain
.
Brain Connect
2013
;
3
:
503
11
.

Seeley
WW
.
Anterior insula degeneration in frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain Struct Funct
2010
;
214
:
465
75
.

Sescousse
G
,
Caldú
X
,
Segura
B
,
Dreher
JC
.
Processing of primary and secondary rewards: a quantitative meta-analysis and review of human functional neuroimaging studies
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
2013
;
37
:
681
96
.

Shan
Q
,
Christie
MJ
,
Balleine
BW
.
Plasticity in striatopallidal projection neurons mediates the acquisition of habitual actions
.
Eur J Neurosci
2015
;
42
:
2097
104
.

Shinagawa
S
,
Ikeda
M
,
Nestor
PJ
,
Shigenobu
K
,
Fukuhara
R
,
Nomura
M
et al. .
Characteristics of abnormal eating behaviours in frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a cross-cultural survey
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
2009
;
80
:
1413
14
.

Sturm
VE
,
Perry
DC
,
Wood
K
,
Hua
AY
,
Alcantar
O
,
Datta
S
et al. .
Prosocial deficits in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia relate to reward network atrophy
.
Brain Behav
2017
;
7
:
e00807
.

Tanaka
SC
,
Balleine
BW
,
O'Doherty
JP
.
Calculating consequences: brain systems that encode the causal effects of actions
.
J Neurosci
2008
;
28
:
6750
5
.

Torralva
T
,
Roca
M
,
Gleichgerrcht
E
,
Bekinschtein
T
,
Manes
F
.
A neuropsychological battery to detect specific executive and social cognitive impairments in early frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
2009
;
132
:
1299
309
.

Tricomi
E
,
Balleine
BW
,
O'Doherty
JP
.
A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning
.
Eur J Neurosci
2009
;
29
:
2225
32
.

Tsai
RM
,
Boxer
AL
.
Treatment of frontotemporal dementia
.
Curr Treat Options Neurol
2014
;
16
:
319
.

Valentin
VV
,
Dickinson
A
,
O'Doherty
JP
.
Determining the neural substrates of goal-directed learning in the human brain
.
J Neurosci
2007
;
27
:
4019
26
.

Vogt
BA
,
Paxinos
G
.
Cytoarchitecture of mouse and rat cingulate cortex with human homologies
.
Brain Struct Func
2014
;
219
:
185
92
.

Wassum
KM
,
Ostlund
SB
,
Maidment
NT
,
Balleine
BW
.
Distinct opioid circuits determine the palatability and the desirability of rewarding events
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2009
;
106
:
12512
17
.

Whitwell
JL
,
Sampson
EL
,
Loy
CT
,
Warren
JE
,
Rossor
MN
,
Fox
NC
et al. .
VBM signatures of abnormal eating behaviours in frontotemporal lobar degeneration
.
Neuroimage
2007
;
35
:
207
13
.

Wiltgen
BJ
,
Law
M
,
Ostlund
S
,
Mayford
M
,
Balleine
BW
.
The influence of Pavlovian cues on instrumental performance is mediated by CaMKII activity in the striatum
.
Eur J Neurosci
2007
;
25
:
2491
7
.

Wise
SP
.
Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function
.
Trends Neurosci
2008
;
31
:
599
608
.

Wong
S
,
Irish
M
,
O'Callaghan
C
,
Kumfor
F
,
Savage
G
,
Hodges
JR
et al. .
Should I trust you? Learning and memory of social interactions in dementia
.
Neuropsychologia
2017
;
104
:
157
67
.

Wood
EM
,
Falcone
D
,
Suh
E
,
Irwin
DJ
,
Chen-Plotkin
AS
,
Lee
EB
et al. .
Development and validation of pedigree classification criteria for frontotemporal lobar degeneration
.
JAMA Neurol
2013
;
70
:
1411
14
.

Woolley
JD
,
Gorno-Tempini
ML
,
Seeley
WW
,
Rankin
K
,
Lee
SS
,
Matthews
BR
, et al.
Binge eating is associated with right orbitofrontal-insular-striatal atrophy in frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
2007
;
69
:
1424
33
.

Woolley
DG
,
Laeremans
A
,
Gantois
I
,
Mantini
D
,
Vermaercke
B
,
Op de Beeck
HP
, et al.
Homologous involvement of striatum and prefrontal cortex in rodent and human water maze learning
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci
2013
;
110
:
3131
36
.

Yin
HH
,
Knowlton
BJ
,
Balleine
BW
.
Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning
.
Eur J Neurosci
2004
;
19
:
181
9
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/about_us/legal/notices)