Abstract

Theoretical accounts of developmental stuttering implicate dysfunctional cortico-striatal-thalamo-cortical motor loops through the putamen. However, the analysis of conventional MRI brain scans in individuals who stutter has failed to yield strong support for this theory in terms of reliable differences in the structure or function of the basal ganglia.

Here, we performed quantitative mapping of brain tissue, which can be used to measure iron content alongside markers sensitive to myelin and thereby offers particular sensitivity to the measurement of iron-rich structures such as the basal ganglia.

Analysis of these quantitative maps in 41 men and women who stutter and 32 individuals who are typically fluent revealed significant group differences in maps of R2*, indicative of higher iron content in individuals who stutter in the left putamen and in left hemisphere cortical regions important for speech motor control. Higher iron levels in brain tissue in individuals who stutter could reflect elevated dopamine levels or lysosomal dysfunction, both of which are implicated in stuttering.

This study represents the first use of these quantitative measures in developmental stuttering and provides new evidence of microstructural differences in the basal ganglia and connected frontal cortical regions.

See Sommer et al. (doi:10.1093/brain/awab348) for a scientific commentary on this article.

Introduction

Developmental stuttering is characterized by dysfluent speech and is observed in 8% of children and ∼1% of the general population.1 Theoretical accounts of developmental stuttering implicate dysfunctional cortico-basal ganglia-thalamocortical motor circuits through the putamen.2,3 Supportive evidence for this theory comes from observations of shared motor characteristics with basal ganglia disorders, such as Parkinson’s disease and dystonia, as well as changes to fluency in response to dopaminergic medication or to deep brain stimulation.2 Early PET studies indicated abnormal basal ganglia function during speech production and differences in dopamine metabolism in individuals who stutter.4,5 However, recent meta-analyses that included functional MRI studies failed to identify dysfunction in these regions as either a state or trait characteristic of stuttering.6 In terms of structure, conventional MRI scans have been used in individuals who stutter to measure grey matter volume, cortical thickness, and diffusion properties of white matter fibre tracts. Whereas some consensus on the occurrence of white matter abnormalities has been reached, analysis of cortical and subcortical volumes reveals inconsistent findings of grey matter differences in individuals who stutter.6,7 In sum, even though there is strong theoretical evidence that points to basal ganglia dysfunction in developmental stuttering, imaging evidence to support or refute this theory is lacking.

In the current study, we scanned the brains of a large sample of individuals who stutter and a group of age- and gender-matched individuals who are typically fluent using a multi-parameter mapping (MPM)8 protocol, which produces semi-quantitative whole-brain maps of three parameters: R1, MTsat, and R2* (longitudinal relaxation, magnetization transfer saturation, and effective transverse relaxation rate, equivalent to 1/T2*, respectively). These parameters reflect histologically-verified differences in tissue microstructure related to myelin and iron.8,9 R1 and MTsat correlate with the amount of myelin in grey and white matter,9 and R2* correlates with post-mortem estimates of iron deposits in grey matter.10

Iron is found in greatest concentration in the basal ganglia. Too little iron is considered detrimental during early development, and lower R2* in the basal ganglia is associated with poorer cognitive ability in adolescents.11 On the other hand, greater iron concentration, as indicated by higher R2*, is a hallmark of Parkinson’s disease12 and increases with ageing.13 We predicted, therefore, that this parameter would be sensitive to any previously undetected differences in the basal ganglia in individuals who stutter, but we did not have a directional hypothesis about whether R2* (iron concentration) would be higher or lower relative to individuals who are typically fluent.

Materials and methods

Participants

Multi-parameter maps were acquired from 73 participants: 41 individuals who stutter (nine women) and 32 individuals who are typically fluent (nine women). Data from an additional 18 participants were excluded because of: acquisition error related to head placement (two individuals who stutter, one individual who is typically fluent); low image or map quality due to movement (eight individuals who stutter; seven individuals who are typically fluent; Supplementary material). Individuals who stutter were aged 19–45 years (median: 31.2), and individuals who are typically fluent were aged 19–44 years (median: 28.6); the groups did not significantly differ in age [t(64.4) = −1.5, P >0.14]. Stuttering severity was assessed in individuals who stutter with the SSI-4 (Stuttering Severity Instrument-Fourth Edition14). The men who stutter were scanned as part of a baseline session of a treatment study that required them to have at least a mild/moderate score at screening (SSI ≥ 20). All participants who stutter met this criterion, but some had milder scores when retested during the baseline session, including one who was classed as very mild (SSI = 16). Thus, the final scores ranged from 16 to 40 (median: 25). Participants had no history of speech, language, or neurological conditions other than developmental stuttering. Participants provided written consent and were compensated for their participation. All procedures were approved by the University of Oxford ethics committee.

Image acquisition

Scans were acquired on a Siemens Prisma 3T scanner with three 3D multi-echo FLASH scans of predominantly T1 (T1w), proton density (PDw), and magnetization transfer weighting (MTw), along with a B1 transmit field map and a B0 static field map.8 A tailored pulse sequence was used: 1 × 1 × 1 mm resolution, field of view = 256 × 224 × 176 mm3, repetition time = 25 ms, bandwidth = 488 Hz/pixels, first echo time/echo spacing = 2.3/2.3 ms, 6° flip angle (PDw, MTw), or 21° (T1w), slab rotation = 30°, and number of echoes = 8 (PDw, MTw) or 6 (T1w), GRAPPA acceleration factor 2 × 2, 40 reference lines in each phase-encoded direction. B1 maps were acquired with spin echo and stimulated echo with a repetition time of 500 ms, a spin-echo echo time of 37.06 ms and a mixing time of 33.8 ms. B0 maps were acquired with repetition time of 1020 ms and echo times of 10 and 12.46 ms.

Quantitative map estimation

The hMRI toolbox was used to calculate and process parameter maps of R1, MTsat, and R2* from the proton density, magnetization transfer, and T1-weighted images using the integrated pipeline with default settings.9 Maps were segmented into grey and white matter and registered to MNI space. Segmented maps were smoothed with a 6-mm Gaussian full-width at half-maximum (sigma = 2.55). Importantly, all processing preserved the quantitative parameter values, without modulating for volume changes.9 A schematic with images and maps from these steps for a single participant is shown in Fig. 1.

Schematic of processing pipeline. Data from one participant shown through each stage of processing, with acquisition [proton density (PDw), magnetization transfer (MTw), and T1-weighted (T1w) images in native space] followed by processing in the hMRI toolbox running in SPM: map estimation (using PDw, MTw, and T1w images to calculate MTsat, R1, and R2* maps), map segmentation into separate grey and white matter tissue (calculated from MTsat and applied to all maps), warping maps to standard space (calculated from MTsat and applied to all maps), and smoothing. R1 and R2* values are in units of 1/s; MTsat are per cent units (p.u.). Colour maps are scaled per parameter to show variation in grey matter. GM = grey matter; WM = white matter.
Figure 1

Schematic of processing pipeline. Data from one participant shown through each stage of processing, with acquisition [proton density (PDw), magnetization transfer (MTw), and T1-weighted (T1w) images in native space] followed by processing in the hMRI toolbox running in SPM: map estimation (using PDw, MTw, and T1w images to calculate MTsat, R1, and R2* maps), map segmentation into separate grey and white matter tissue (calculated from MTsat and applied to all maps), warping maps to standard space (calculated from MTsat and applied to all maps), and smoothing. R1 and R2* values are in units of 1/s; MTsat are per cent units (p.u.). Colour maps are scaled per parameter to show variation in grey matter. GM = grey matter; WM = white matter.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL). For each map and tissue type (MTsat, R1, R2* × white, grey matter), a whole-brain general linear model analysis was performed using permutation testing with 5000 permutations. Statistical inference was drawn using threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) to identify voxels in which the measurements between groups differed significantly. TFCE identifies regions of statistical difference without specifying an arbitrary cluster-forming height threshold; instead, small regions that are very different between groups or large regions with smaller differences may be identified, as long as they exhibit cluster-like regional specificity.15 Significance was set at P < 0.025 to correct for the two single-tailed t-tests performed to assess group differences in both directions.16

The remaining statistical analyses were performed in R using analyses of variance (R version: 4.0.2; command: aov). Mean values of R2* were extracted for the statistical clusters in regions showing significant group differences to test relationships between R2* and age and compare these relationships between groups (age, group, and age × group as factors). Within the individuals who stutter group, the relationship to stuttering severity in these regions was tested statistically with R2* as the dependent measure and age, stuttering severity index, and age × stuttering severity as factors.

Data availability

Statistical maps of group differences are available at NeuroVault: https://neurovault.org/collections/UPUDTUZJ/.

Results

Individuals who stutter and those who are typically fluent did not differ in terms of whole brain volume or values averaged across all voxels in the grey and white tissue maps of R1, MTsat and R2* (Supplementary Table 1).

Whole-brain voxel-wise analysis of the quantitative maps revealed significantly higher grey matter R2* in individuals who stutter compared with individuals who are typically fluent subcortically in the left putamen, and cortically primarily in the left frontal lobe, including: the frontal opercular cortex extending to the anterior insula; the inferior frontal gyrus (pars opercularis) and posterior extent of the inferior frontal sulcus; and ventral precentral gyrus corresponding to the level of the representation of tongue movements17 (Table 1 and Fig. 2A). It was striking that at this threshold (P < 0.025), the group differences were restricted to the left hemisphere. At a lower threshold of P < 0.05, group differences were also seen in the left caudate nucleus, subcortically, and more extensive portions of the right and left hemispheres, cortically (Supplementary Fig. 1).

Areas with higher R2* in individuals who stutter. (A) The coloured overlay is the statistical map showing areas with higher R2* in individuals who stutter than individuals who are typically fluent (thresholded at P < 0.025) on top of the average MTsat map for all participants aligned to MNI space. Individual data for mean R2* are plotted against age × group in (B) left putamen, (C) left frontal operculum and insula. Red = individuals who stutter; black = individuals who are typically fluent. Circles are men and triangles are women. Shaded areas show 95% confidence intervals. In box plots, centre line is median and edges are 25th and 75th percentiles. L = left hemisphere; preCG = precentral gyrus; IFS = inferior frontal sulcus (posterior); IFGpo = inferior frontal gyrus, pars opercularis; FO & INS = frontal operculum/insula.
Figure 2

Areas with higher R2* in individuals who stutter. (A) The coloured overlay is the statistical map showing areas with higher R2* in individuals who stutter than individuals who are typically fluent (thresholded at P < 0.025) on top of the average MTsat map for all participants aligned to MNI space. Individual data for mean R2* are plotted against age × group in (B) left putamen, (C) left frontal operculum and insula. Red = individuals who stutter; black = individuals who are typically fluent. Circles are men and triangles are women. Shaded areas show 95% confidence intervals. In box plots, centre line is median and edges are 25th and 75th percentiles. L = left hemisphere; preCG = precentral gyrus; IFS = inferior frontal sulcus (posterior); IFGpo = inferior frontal gyrus, pars opercularis; FO & INS = frontal operculum/insula.

Table 1

Locations of increased R2* in individuals who stutter

Brain areaxyzVoxelsP
Left superior frontal sulcus−2427405130.023
Left inferior frontal gyrus, pars opercularis−4915142290.024
Left inferior frontal sulcus (posterior)−42112716770.019
Left putamen−2110−116150.023
Left frontal operculum/anterior insula−468366740.012
Left precentral gyrus (ventral)−52−6294990.024
Left planum temporale−56−301560.025
Left superior parietal lobule−28−605616260.018
Brain areaxyzVoxelsP
Left superior frontal sulcus−2427405130.023
Left inferior frontal gyrus, pars opercularis−4915142290.024
Left inferior frontal sulcus (posterior)−42112716770.019
Left putamen−2110−116150.023
Left frontal operculum/anterior insula−468366740.012
Left precentral gyrus (ventral)−52−6294990.024
Left planum temporale−56−301560.025
Left superior parietal lobule−28−605616260.018

Thresholded at P < 0.025. Coordinates of the centre of gravity of each cluster are provided in MNI152 space. The extent of each cluster is provided in voxels. The P-value for the peak voxel is shown in the final column.

Table 1

Locations of increased R2* in individuals who stutter

Brain areaxyzVoxelsP
Left superior frontal sulcus−2427405130.023
Left inferior frontal gyrus, pars opercularis−4915142290.024
Left inferior frontal sulcus (posterior)−42112716770.019
Left putamen−2110−116150.023
Left frontal operculum/anterior insula−468366740.012
Left precentral gyrus (ventral)−52−6294990.024
Left planum temporale−56−301560.025
Left superior parietal lobule−28−605616260.018
Brain areaxyzVoxelsP
Left superior frontal sulcus−2427405130.023
Left inferior frontal gyrus, pars opercularis−4915142290.024
Left inferior frontal sulcus (posterior)−42112716770.019
Left putamen−2110−116150.023
Left frontal operculum/anterior insula−468366740.012
Left precentral gyrus (ventral)−52−6294990.024
Left planum temporale−56−301560.025
Left superior parietal lobule−28−605616260.018

Thresholded at P < 0.025. Coordinates of the centre of gravity of each cluster are provided in MNI152 space. The extent of each cluster is provided in voxels. The P-value for the peak voxel is shown in the final column.

Mean R2* values for each significant cluster are shown for each participant (Fig. 2B, C and Supplementary Table 2) and examined further against age (Supplementary Fig. 2). As expected,13 there was a significant linear increase in R2* with age in all regions, but importantly these relationships did not differ between groups (i.e. no interaction between group and age; Supplementary Table 2).

To check that the focal differences in grey matter R2* were not due to morphometric differences in individuals who stutter, we performed a voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis using standard T1-weighted images (Supplementary material). The groups were not different in terms of their relative amounts of grey matter anywhere in the brain.

In the individuals who stutter, we examined if there was a significant relationship between stuttering severity and R2* when controlling for age in any of the grey matter regions showing higher R2*; no regions showed a significant relationship between R2* and stuttering severity (Supplementary Table 3).

There were no differences between groups in the white matter R2* maps or in the grey or white matter maps of the myelin-sensitive markers, R1 and MTsat.

Discussion

Obtaining multi-parameter maps in a large cohort of individuals who do and do not stutter allowed us to conduct a detailed examination of neural microstructure, tied to histologically- and neurobiologically-relevant processes. Our results provide evidence of microstructural differences in those who stutter consistent with theoretical accounts of developmental stuttering that implicate dysfunction in cortico-basal ganglia-thalamocortical loops through the putamen.2,3 Below we discuss these findings in the context of the relationships between iron in brain tissue and dopamine. We also discuss the link to lysosomal dysfunction, which is implicated in stuttering through the identification of causative mutations in related genes.

R2* differences in the putamen and cortical speech motor regions

The R2* parameter provided by the quantitative mapping protocol has known sensitivity to non-heme iron (that is, iron in the tissue rather than in blood), based on direct comparisons of R2* to histology in post-mortem brains with and without neurodegenerative diseases.10,18 Iron in the brain is found in highest concentration in the basal ganglia.13 For this reason, we predicted it would be sensitive to detecting differences in these nuclei in individuals who stutter. Accordingly, one region in the basal ganglia had higher R2* in individuals who stutter: the left putamen, which has previously been implicated in theoretical accounts of stuttering.2,3 Wu and colleagues4 found increased dopaminergic activity in the left insula and putamen in a very small sample of individuals who stutter. Another PET study showed that treatment success in individuals who stutter was predicted by decreased regional cortical blood flow in the left putamen.19

In addition to this basal ganglia difference, there were several cortical regions showing higher R2* in individuals who stutter (Table 1), all of which are part of the speech motor network. These left hemisphere cortical areas in inferior frontal and ventral motor cortex show disfluency-related activity and are commonly underactive in individuals who stutter relative to controls (state and trait).6 How the R2* differences in these brain regions relate to abnormal patterns of brain activity or to stuttering and other motor characteristics in individuals who stutter is as yet unknown. Our analyses failed to reveal relationships in any area with a standardized measure of stuttering severity.

Possible interpretations of higher R2*/iron concentration: dopamine and lysosomal dysfunction

One explanatory model for higher iron concentration in grey matter in individuals who stutter implicates dopamine. Iron and dopamine have complex interactions in the brain and must remain in a precise homeostatic balance for healthy function. Accordingly, increasing extracellular dopamine leads to higher intracellular iron levels, and when iron is introduced in the cell, D2 receptor protein concentrations increase.20,21 A recent study in typically fluent speakers has suggested R2* as a correlate of presynaptic vesicular dopamine concentration.22 Thus R2* increases seen here in individuals who stutter could be an indirect marker of excess dopamine levels. Developmental stuttering has been hypothesized to result from an excess of dopamine, although the evidence in support of this hypothesis from PET and pharmacological interventions is weak due to small sample sizes or side effects of the medication.2,4,23 On the other hand, R2* is also increased in Parkinson’s disease in which dopamine is depleted; in that case, it is thought that the increased iron may be the causative agent that leads to the death of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra.24 This mechanism is related to lysosomal dysfunction, which is also implicated in stuttering and forms the basis of our second explanatory model for the increased iron concentration in individuals who stutter.

In typical function, lysosomes degrade and recycle cellular waste, repair plasma membranes, and decompose intracellular stores of ferritin (Fe3+) into Fe2+ to be transported for cellular processes requiring iron ions.25 Accordingly, lysosomal storage disorders can result in an accumulation of substrates that are otherwise typically decomposed or transported. One such disorder, Gaucher disease, leads to an accumulation of iron in the brain and body. The genetic mutation that causes Gaucher disease is homozygous; when that same mutation is heterozygous, individuals have an increased risk of developing Parkinson’s disease.26 Heterozygous causative mutations in GNPTAB, GNPTG, and related genes have been identified in individuals who stutter, accounting for ∼10% of these individuals.27 Homozygous mutations in these genes result in dysfunctions in intracellular trafficking and lysosomal processes.27 Expression patterns of these genes in the Allen Human Brain Atlas are spatially coincident with the cortical networks showing differences in children who stutter and are particularly high in the frontal opercular cortex, where we found significantly higher concentrations of iron in adults who stutter.28 We therefore posit that the increased iron could be indicative of lysosomal dysfunction. We found that R2* increased with age, as expected,13 but increases were not accelerated in individuals who stutter (no significant interaction between age and group; Supplementary Table 2). Thus, it is possible that heterozygous lysosomal mutations in stuttering cause dysfunction at a critical period in development or lead to lysosomal dysfunction resulting in greater iron deposition that does not accumulate at a faster rate over time.

No differences were found in myelin markers or grey matter volume

Despite our reasonably large sample size, our analysis did not detect group differences in the other parameters provided by the quantitative maps or in grey matter volume. No differences were found in MTsat or R1 maps, both of which are thought to be sensitive to differences in the amount of myelin in a given area.9 This result may appear inconsistent with the results of many diffusion weighted imaging studies in individuals who stutter (including our own), which reported lower fractional anisotropy (FA) in white matter tracts.7 Lower FA could reflect a number of differences in white matter microstructure, including the orientation or dispersion of fibres in a voxel, axonal calibre and density, and not only the amount of myelin. We speculated previously that lower FA in individuals who stutter in some areas reflected differences in fibre organization, rather than amount of myelin.29 The current results are consistent with that interpretation. Nevertheless, further studies are warranted to understand the relationships among these different measures of microstructure.

VBM has previously been used to examine whether regional amounts of grey matter differ in individuals who stutter relative to control groups.7 The findings are equivocal: in some studies, individuals who stutter have more grey matter, in others less, and in others no group differences are reported. We found no significant differences in amounts of grey matter in any brain region, and thus find no evidence that the R2* differences are related to differing amounts of grey matter in individuals who stutter.

This is the first study using multi-parameter mapping to measure R2*, R1, or MTsat in individuals who stutter. Even though it is in quite a large cohort (41 individuals who stutter, 32 individuals who are typically fluent), these novel results need to be replicated. One recent study has indicated similar findings in regard to iron using ultrasound to reveal elevated iron accumulation in the substantia nigra in individuals who stutter.30 Here we similarly conclude that there are iron differences in the basal ganglia (and connected cortical areas) in individuals who stutter, although our analyses revealed no significant differences in iron levels in the substantia nigra. Further analyses are warranted and measurement using ultrasound in the same participants would clarify the current discrepancy between the results of the two studies.

Conclusion

In this study of a large sample of individuals who stutter, we provide evidence for elevated R2* in the left putamen and connected frontal cortical regions. This difference in R2* most likely reflects increased iron concentrations, which may be indicative of excess dopamine levels or lysosomal dysfunction in individuals who stutter. Further work is needed to link R2* differences to genetic profiles associated with developmental stuttering, to increased dopamine or lysosomal dysfunction, or to another neurobiological function that could point towards effective therapies for those who want them.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Dr Martina Callaghan of University College London for the multi-parameter mapping sequence code. We would also like to thank Máiréad Healy and Louisa Needham for their assistance in data collection, along with members of the OHBA centre MRI team: Juliet Semple, Nicky Aikin, and Nicola Filippini. Our sincerest thanks also go to all of the participants who took part in this study.

Funding

This work was funded by the Medical Research Council (MR/N025539/1) and the National Institutes of Health—National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (F32 DC017637) and supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust [203139/Z/16/Z].

Competing interests

The authors report no competing interests.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain online.

References

1

Yairi
E
,
Ambrose
N.
Epidemiology of stuttering: 21st century advances
.
J Fluency Disord
.
2013
;
38
(
2
):
66
87
.

2

Alm
PA.
Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: A critical review of possible relations
.
J Commun Disord
.
2004
;
37
(
4
):
325
369
.

3

Chang
S-E
,
Guenther
FH.
Involvement of the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamocortical loop in developmental stuttering
.
Front Psychol
.
2019
;
10
:
3088
.

4

Wu
JC
,
Maguire
G
,
Riley
G
, et al.
Increased dopamine activity associated with stuttering
.
Neuroreport
.
1997
;
8
(
3
):
767
770
.

5

Fox
PT
,
Ingham
RJ
,
Ingham
JC
, et al.
A PET study of the neural systems of stuttering
.
Nature
.
1996
;
382
(
6587
):
158
162
.

6

Neef
NE
,
Anwander
A
,
Friederici
AD.
The neurobiological grounding of persistent stuttering: from structure to function
.
Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep
.
2015
;
15
(
9
):
63
.

7

Etchell
AC
,
Civier
O
,
Ballard
KJ
,
Sowman
PF.
A systematic literature review of neuroimaging research on developmental stuttering between 1995 and 2016
.
J Fluency Disord
.
2018
;
55
:
6
45
.

8

Weiskopf
N
,
Suckling
J
,
Williams
G
, et al.
Quantitative multi-parameter mapping of R1, PD, MT, and R2 at 3T: A multi-center validation
.
Front Neurosci
.
2013
;
7
:
95
.

9

Tabelow
K
,
Balteau
E
,
Ashburner
J
, et al.
hMRI – a toolbox for quantitative MRI in neuroscience and clinical research
.
Neuroimage
.
2019
;
194
:
191
210
.

10

De Barros
A
,
Arribarat
G
,
Combis
J
,
Chaynes
P
,
Péran
P.
Matching Ex vivo MRI with iron histology: Pearls and pitfalls
.
Front Neuroanat
.
2019
;
13
:
68
.

11

Larsen
B
,
Bourque
J
,
Moore
XTM
, et al.
Longitudinal development of brain iron is linked to cognition in youth
.
J Neurosci
.
2020
;
40
(
9
):
1810
1818
.

12

Wallis
LI
,
Paley
MNJ
,
Graham
JM
, et al.
MRI assessment of basal ganglia iron deposition in Parkinson’s disease
.
J Magn Reson Imaging
.
2008
;
28
(
5
):
1061
1067
.

13

Daugherty
AM
,
Raz
N.
Appraising the role of iron in brain aging and cognition: Promises and limitations of MRI methods
.
Neuropsychol Rev
.
2015
;
25
(
3
):
272
287
.

14

Riley
GD.
Stuttering Severity Instrument–Fourth Edition
.
Pro-Ed
;
2009
.

15

Smith
SM
,
Nichols
TE.
Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference
.
Neuroimage
.
2009
;
44
(
1
):
83
98
.

16

Chen
G
,
Cox
RW
,
Glen
DR
,
Rajendra
JK
,
Reynolds
RC
,
Taylor
PA.
A tail of two sides: Artificially doubled false positive rates in neuroimaging due to the sidedness choice with t-tests
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2019
;
40
(
3
):
1037
1043
.

17

Eichert
N
,
Watkins
K
,
Mars
R
,
Petrides
M.
Morphological and functional variability in central and subcentral motor cortex of the human brain
.
Brain Struct Funct
. 2021;
226
(
1
):
263
279
.

18

Schenck
JF.
Magnetic resonance imaging of brain iron
.
J Neurol Sci
.
2003
;
207
(
1-2
):
99
102
.

19

Ingham
RJ
,
Wang
Y
,
Ingham
JC
,
Bothe
AK
,
Grafton
ST.
Regional brain activity change predicts responsiveness to treatment for stuttering in adults
.
Brain Lang
.
2013
;
127
(
3
):
510
519
.

20

Dichtl
S
,
Haschka
D
,
Nairz
M
, et al.
Dopamine promotes cellular iron accumulation and oxidative stress responses in macrophages
.
Biochem Pharmacol
.
2018
;
148
:
193
201
.

21

Unger
EL
,
Wiesinger
JA
,
Hao
L
,
Beard
JL.
Dopamine D2 receptor expression is altered by changes in cellular iron levels in PC12 cells and rat brain tissue
.
J Nutr
.
2008
;
138
(
12
):
2487
2494
.

22

Larsen
B
,
Olafsson
V
,
Calabro
F
, et al.
Maturation of the human striatal dopamine system revealed by PET and quantitative MRI
.
Nat Commun
.
2020
;
11
(
1
):
846
.

23

Maguire
GA
,
Nguyen
DL
,
Simonson
KC
,
Kurz
TL.
The pharmacologic treatment of stuttering and its neuropharmacologic basis
.
Front Neurosci
.
2020
;
14
:
158
.

24

Funke
C
,
Schneider
SA
,
Berg
D
,
Kell
DB.
Genetics and iron in the systems biology of Parkinson’s disease and some related disorders
.
Neurochem Int
.
2013
;
62
(
5
):
637
652
.

25

Rouault
TA.
Iron on the brain
.
Nat Genet
.
2001
;
28
(
4
):
299
300
.

26

Bras
JM.
Lysosomal storage disorders and iron. In: Bhatia KP, Schneider SA, eds.
International Review of Neurobiology
. Vol
110
.
Academic Press Inc
;
2013
:
251
275
.

27

Frigerio-Domingues
C
,
Drayna
D.
Genetic contributions to stuttering: The current evidence
.
Mol Genet Genomic Med
.
2017
;
5
(
2
):
95
102
.

28

Benito-Aragón
C
,
Gonzalez-Sarmiento
R
,
Liddell
T
, et al.
Neurofilament-lysosomal genetic intersections in the cortical network of stuttering
.
Prog Neurobiol
.
2020
;
184
:
101718
.

29

Connally
EL
,
Ward
D
,
Howell
P
,
Watkins
KE.
Disrupted white matter in language and motor tracts in developmental stuttering
.
Brain Lang
.
2014
;
131
:
25
35
.

30

Liman
J
,
Wolff von Gudenberg
A
,
Baehr
M
,
Paulus
W
,
Neef
NE
,
Sommer
M.
Enlarged area of mesencephalic iron deposits in adults who stutter
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2021
;
15
:
639269
.

     
  • MTsat

    magnetization transfer saturation

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Supplementary data