Abstract

Dopaminergic dysfunction in the basal ganglia, particularly in the posterior putamen, is often viewed as the primary pathological mechanism behind motor slowing (i.e. bradykinesia) in Parkinson’s disease. However, striatal dopamine loss fails to account for interindividual differences in motor phenotype and rate of decline, implying that the expression of motor symptoms depends on additional mechanisms, some of which may be compensatory in nature. Building on observations of increased motor-related activity in the parieto-premotor cortex of Parkinson patients, we tested the hypothesis that interindividual differences in clinical severity are determined by compensatory cortical mechanisms and not just by basal ganglia dysfunction.

Using functional MRI, we measured variability in motor- and selection-related brain activity during a visuomotor task in 353 patients with Parkinson’s disease (≤5 years disease duration) and 60 healthy controls. In this task, we manipulated action selection demand by varying the number of possible actions that individuals could choose from. Clinical variability was characterized in two ways. First, patients were categorized into three previously validated, discrete clinical subtypes that are hypothesized to reflect distinct routes of α-synuclein propagation: diffuse-malignant (n = 42), intermediate (n = 128) or mild motor-predominant (n = 150). Second, we used the scores of bradykinesia severity and cognitive performance across the entire sample as continuous measures.

Patients showed motor slowing (longer response times) and reduced motor-related activity in the basal ganglia compared with controls. However, basal ganglia activity did not differ between clinical subtypes and was not associated with clinical scores. This indicates a limited role for striatal dysfunction in shaping interindividual differences in clinical severity. Consistent with our hypothesis, we observed enhanced action selection-related activity in the parieto-premotor cortex of patients with a mild-motor predominant subtype, both compared to patients with a diffuse-malignant subtype and controls. Furthermore, increased parieto-premotor activity was related to lower bradykinesia severity and better cognitive performance, which points to a compensatory role.

We conclude that parieto-premotor compensation, rather than basal ganglia dysfunction, shapes interindividual variability in symptom severity in Parkinson’s disease. Future interventions may focus on maintaining and enhancing compensatory cortical mechanisms, rather than only attempting to normalize basal ganglia dysfunction.

Introduction

Bradykinesia is one of the cardinal motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease. It manifests as slowness during the selection and execution of voluntary movements, worsening as task complexity increases.1-3 The severity of bradykinesia, which varies substantially between individuals, has been considered an outcome of dopamine depletion and basal ganglia dysfunction.4,5 However, motor symptoms may also be shaped by compensatory cortical processes.2,3,6-11 It remains unclear to what extent such compensatory processes, along with interindividual differences in their efficacy, contribute to clinical heterogeneity in Parkinson’s disease, over and above basal ganglia dysfunction. We tested this by analysing data from a cohort of patients (Personalized Parkinson Project12; n = 353) who performed an action selection task designed to activate compensatory cerebral mechanisms implicated in motor control whilst undergoing functional MRI.

In Parkinson’s disease, progressive degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra leads to dopamine depletion in the striatum, resulting in dysfunctional basal ganglia output and impaired motor performance.5,13-17 These motor impairments become particularly pronounced in situations where patients are required to make voluntary choices between multiple competing movement options,18-22 potentially as a result of increased cognitive load.3,23,24 However, striatal dopamine depletion occurs gradually over several years prior to the onset of bradykinesia.8,15,25-29 Furthermore, motor symptoms progress despite the fact that the motor region of the striatum (posterior putamen) is almost entirely depleted of dopamine 4 years after diagnosis.25 Additionally, several studies have failed to demonstrate an association between changes in motor symptom severity and striatal dopamine levels over time15,30-33 (although see Parkinson Study Group34). These observations strongly imply that basal ganglia dysfunction is not the sole mechanism underlying bradykinesia and action selection deficits in Parkinson’s disease.

Over recent decades, several studies have suggested that action selection is partially maintained in Parkinson’s disease by compensatory cerebral processes.7 Cerebral compensation has been conceptualized as a performance-enhancing recruitment of neural resources that enable individuals to meet moderately high task demands despite deficits in the neural mechanisms that typically support task performance.11,35,36 Cerebral compensation can manifest in at least two forms: upregulated activation of mechanisms already dedicated to task performance, or selective recruitment of mechanisms that are typically dedicated to other processes.37,38 Such compensatory mechanisms are thought to be instantiated during the long preclinical phase of Parkinson’s disease, which may last for several years,39,40 to stabilize behavioural performance as basal ganglia dysfunction gradually worsens.6,8,10,41,42 However, with disease progression, the degree of basal ganglia dysfunction will eventually exceed the capacities of compensatory mechanisms, leading to the appearance and subsequent worsening of motor deficits.10,43,44 Importantly, the efficacy of these compensatory mechanisms likely differs between individuals owing to idiosyncrasies in patterns of pathology (e.g. focal versus diffuse propagation of α-synucleinopathy) and may therefore contribute to the clinical heterogeneity that characterizes Parkinson’s disease.45,46

Demonstrating compensation with neuroimaging is not straightforward, since increased brain activity during a task may reflect either recruitment of compensatory resources or reduced efficiency of processes that support task performance.35,47 Two basic functional criteria for establishing compensation have been suggested. First, it should be clear what is being compensated for, such as basal ganglia dysfunction in the case of Parkinson’s disease.5 Second, compensatory brain activity should have beneficial effects on behavioural performance.11 Furthermore, recruitment of compensatory mechanisms is expected particularly when task demands are higher, i.e. when pathological deficits would otherwise lead to impaired performance. More generally, demonstrating compensation requires statistical power adequate to detect biologically plausible effects linking neural compensation and behavioural performance in Parkinson’s disease.48,49 Recent meta-analyses of functional MRI studies have shown that patients with Parkinson’s disease have increased motor-related activation in parieto-premotor regions that are implicated in healthy motor control.16,17,50 This increase in activity is suggestive of compensatory upregulation, e.g. reflecting enhanced reliance on goal-directed control during motor execution.7 However, most studies were unable to verify this possibility due to sample sizes inadequate to quantify brain-behaviour correlations. Other studies have focused on presymptomatic carriers of gene mutations associated with a high risk of developing Parkinson’s disease, who showed intact behavioural performance in combination with increased premotor activity relative to healthy controls during action selection.51,52 This selection-related increase in premotor activation decreases as motor symptoms worsen after the onset of Parkinson’s disease, indicating that it may reflect compensatory mechanisms in the presymptomatic stage.53 Finally, we and others have shown that patients with Parkinson’s disease rely more heavily on the extrastriate visual cortex during motor imagery of their most affected hand,54 and that disruption of this region with transcranial magnetic stimulation impaired motor imagery in Parkinson’s disease patients but not in healthy controls.55 This imagery-related extrastriate visual cortex activity might be compensatory, but a relationship with actual motor behaviour has not yet been established.

Here, we tested the hypothesis that the clinical heterogeneity in Parkinson’s disease depends on compensatory upregulation of parieto-premotor function, over and above basal ganglia dysfunction. We used a motor task in combination with functional MRI to assess motor- and selection-related brain activity in early-to-moderate Parkinson’s disease patients.56 We verified that Parkinson’s disease is associated with reduced activity in the basal ganglia by comparing task-related activity in patients against healthy controls. We also tested for normalizing effects of dopamine replacement therapy on motor network dysfunction. The novelty of this study concerns the neural mechanisms underlying clinical heterogeneity in Parkinson’s disease. First, we compared clinical subtypes (mild-motor predominant, intermediate and diffuse-malignant) that were defined based on motor symptoms, cognitive performance, REM sleep behaviour disorder and autonomic dysfunction.57 Second, we quantified the relationship between interindividual variability in brain activity and the clinically rated severity of bradykinesia as well as a composite measure of global cognitive performance. Our findings show that enhanced parieto-premotor activity is related to a more benign subtype of Parkinson’s disease, less severe bradykinesia and better cognitive performance, suggesting that it may be compensating for a basal ganglia deficit.

Materials and methods

Participants

Data from 367 patients diagnosed with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease and 60 healthy controls were retrieved from the Personalized Parkinson Project database in March 2022. The Personalized Parkinson Project is an ongoing single-centre longitudinal cohort study taking place at Radboud University Medical Center (Nijmegen, the Netherlands; ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT03364894 and NCT05169827).12 All patients underwent sequential motor symptom assessments OFF (i.e. 12 h since the last dose of dopaminergic medication) and ON dopaminergic medication with the Movement Disorders Society Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale part III (MDS-UPDRS III).58 MRI measurements were acquired in the ON-medicated state. Fifty-six patients returned for identical MRI measurements OFF medication. Half of these 56 patients were assessed within 3 months after their first ON-state measurement. The other half were assessed within 3 months prior to a 2-year follow-up visit, to reduce potential learning effects and even out the distribution of disease durations in the OFF-medicated group. Healthy controls were matched to the OFF-medicated group with respect to age, sex and handedness. Written informed consent was obtained for all participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by a medical ethical committee (METC Oost-Nederland, formerly CMO Arnhem-Nijmegen; #2016-2934 and #2018-4785). See the Supplementary material for detailed information on inclusion and exclusion criteria. During baseline assessments, diagnoses of eight patients were re-evaluated to either a form of atypical parkinsonism (n = 2) or other (n = 6). Diagnosis re-evaluations at 2-year follow-up confirmed that an additional six patients did not have Parkinson’s disease (three multiple system atrophy, two progressive supranuclear palsy and one indeterminate). All patients with a verified non-Parkinson’s disease diagnosis at either baseline or follow-up were excluded from further analysis, resulting in a total sample size of 353 ON-medicated patients (of whom 55 also had an OFF-medication assessment) and 60 controls. All healthy controls had complete data. Numbers of missing and excluded data points from patients can be found in the Supplementary material. Demographic information can be found in Table 1.

Table 1

Demographic information and clinical characteristics

VariableControlPD-OFFPD-ONMMPIMDMUndefined
Sample size60543531501284233
Age60.0 (9.6)61.1 (8.5)62.5 (8.6)61.4 (8.9)63.1 (8.5)65.5 (6.9)61.7 (8.8)
Sex (F/M)27/3322/32129/22467/8346/828/348/25
Years of education16.2 (3.3)17.1 (4.1)17.2 (4.1)17.3 (4.3)17.3 (4.3)16.5 (4.3)16.4 (3.7)
Dominant hand (L/R)5/557/4756/29725/12521/1078/342/31
Responding hand (L/R)29/3128/24167/17866/8166/6022/1913/18
Responding hand is dominant (N/Y)26/3427/25159/18663/8462/6419/2215/16
Disease duration (months)NA34.2 (15.6)34.2 (17.3)32.7 (16.6)34.9 (17.9)34.8 (17.7)37.4 (17.3)
Most affected side (L/R/Bi)NA27/23/5173/163/1766/79/565/57/617/21/415/16/2
Hoehn and Yahr-stageNA11/37/5/140/265/44/322/117/11/013/103/11/10/26/14/25/19/8/0
Medication use (N/Y)NA0/5417/33511/1393/1250/423/29
LEDDNA501.5 (220.3)545.2 (319.8)467.8 (256.5)573.8 (334.1)624.1 (353.7)715.4 (405.9)
MoCA27.6 (1.9)27.1 (1.8)26.7 (2.6)27.2 (2.0)26.7 (2.5)26.0 (3.5)25.9 (3.4)
Cognitive compositeNA0.2 (0.5)0.0 (0.6)0.3 (0.4)−0.2 (0.6)−0.4 (0.8)−0.3 (0.6)
MDS-UPDRS IINA7.7 (5.0)8.4 (5.6)5.9 (4.1)8.8 (4.8)15.3 (6.2)9.5 (6.8)
MDS-UPDRS III: OFF-state assessment
TotalNA33.6 (13.8)33.7 (13.0)29.6 (10.9)33.3 (12.3)47.8 (10.5)36.4 (15.6)
BradykinesiaNA16.2 (7.7)16.7 (7.5)14.4 (6.5)16.7 (7.0)24.6 (6.4)17.5 (8.3)
RigidityNA7.0 (3.0)6.6 (3.3)6.1 (3.2)6.6 (3.3)8.7 (3.0)6.4 (3.3)
PIGDNA2.1 (1.4)2.0 (1.4)1.6 (1.0)1.9 (1.1)3.9 (1.7)2.2 (1.4)
TremorNA5.8 (5.1)6.1 (5.2)5.9 (5.0)5.9 (5.2)7.2 (5.2)7.6 (5.6)
MDS-UPDRS III: ON-state assessment
TotalNA28.0 (13.3)28.7 (12.5)25.4 (10.6)29.2 (13.0)38.7 (10.8)28.3 (14.0)
BradykinesiaNA13.7 (7.2)14.4 (7.2)12.6 (6.5)14.5 (7.3)20.0 (6.2)13.7 (7.6)
RigidityNA6.0 (3.3)6.0 (3.3)5.5 (3.1)6.0 (3.5)7.7 (3.1)5.5 (3.3)
PIGDNA1.6 (1.3)1.6 (1.2)1.2 (0.9)1.6 (1.1)2.6 (1.6)1.7 (1.2)
TremorNA4.6 (3.5)5.0 (4.5)4.6 (4.1)5.3 (5.0)5.3 (4.4)5.2 (3.1)
VariableControlPD-OFFPD-ONMMPIMDMUndefined
Sample size60543531501284233
Age60.0 (9.6)61.1 (8.5)62.5 (8.6)61.4 (8.9)63.1 (8.5)65.5 (6.9)61.7 (8.8)
Sex (F/M)27/3322/32129/22467/8346/828/348/25
Years of education16.2 (3.3)17.1 (4.1)17.2 (4.1)17.3 (4.3)17.3 (4.3)16.5 (4.3)16.4 (3.7)
Dominant hand (L/R)5/557/4756/29725/12521/1078/342/31
Responding hand (L/R)29/3128/24167/17866/8166/6022/1913/18
Responding hand is dominant (N/Y)26/3427/25159/18663/8462/6419/2215/16
Disease duration (months)NA34.2 (15.6)34.2 (17.3)32.7 (16.6)34.9 (17.9)34.8 (17.7)37.4 (17.3)
Most affected side (L/R/Bi)NA27/23/5173/163/1766/79/565/57/617/21/415/16/2
Hoehn and Yahr-stageNA11/37/5/140/265/44/322/117/11/013/103/11/10/26/14/25/19/8/0
Medication use (N/Y)NA0/5417/33511/1393/1250/423/29
LEDDNA501.5 (220.3)545.2 (319.8)467.8 (256.5)573.8 (334.1)624.1 (353.7)715.4 (405.9)
MoCA27.6 (1.9)27.1 (1.8)26.7 (2.6)27.2 (2.0)26.7 (2.5)26.0 (3.5)25.9 (3.4)
Cognitive compositeNA0.2 (0.5)0.0 (0.6)0.3 (0.4)−0.2 (0.6)−0.4 (0.8)−0.3 (0.6)
MDS-UPDRS IINA7.7 (5.0)8.4 (5.6)5.9 (4.1)8.8 (4.8)15.3 (6.2)9.5 (6.8)
MDS-UPDRS III: OFF-state assessment
TotalNA33.6 (13.8)33.7 (13.0)29.6 (10.9)33.3 (12.3)47.8 (10.5)36.4 (15.6)
BradykinesiaNA16.2 (7.7)16.7 (7.5)14.4 (6.5)16.7 (7.0)24.6 (6.4)17.5 (8.3)
RigidityNA7.0 (3.0)6.6 (3.3)6.1 (3.2)6.6 (3.3)8.7 (3.0)6.4 (3.3)
PIGDNA2.1 (1.4)2.0 (1.4)1.6 (1.0)1.9 (1.1)3.9 (1.7)2.2 (1.4)
TremorNA5.8 (5.1)6.1 (5.2)5.9 (5.0)5.9 (5.2)7.2 (5.2)7.6 (5.6)
MDS-UPDRS III: ON-state assessment
TotalNA28.0 (13.3)28.7 (12.5)25.4 (10.6)29.2 (13.0)38.7 (10.8)28.3 (14.0)
BradykinesiaNA13.7 (7.2)14.4 (7.2)12.6 (6.5)14.5 (7.3)20.0 (6.2)13.7 (7.6)
RigidityNA6.0 (3.3)6.0 (3.3)5.5 (3.1)6.0 (3.5)7.7 (3.1)5.5 (3.3)
PIGDNA1.6 (1.3)1.6 (1.2)1.2 (0.9)1.6 (1.1)2.6 (1.6)1.7 (1.2)
TremorNA4.6 (3.5)5.0 (4.5)4.6 (4.1)5.3 (5.0)5.3 (4.4)5.2 (3.1)

Values are presented as mean (SD) unless otherwise stated. Bi = bilateral; DM = diffuse-malignant; F = female; FWEc = family-wise error cluster; IM = intermediate; L = left; LEDD = levodopa equivalent daily dose; M = male; MDS-UPDRS = Movement Disorders Society Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale; MMP = mild-motor predominant; MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment; N = no; PD-OFF = patients scanned in the OFF-medicated state; PD-ON = patients scanned in the ON-medicated state; PIGD = postural instability and gait disorder; R = right; Y = yes.

Table 1

Demographic information and clinical characteristics

VariableControlPD-OFFPD-ONMMPIMDMUndefined
Sample size60543531501284233
Age60.0 (9.6)61.1 (8.5)62.5 (8.6)61.4 (8.9)63.1 (8.5)65.5 (6.9)61.7 (8.8)
Sex (F/M)27/3322/32129/22467/8346/828/348/25
Years of education16.2 (3.3)17.1 (4.1)17.2 (4.1)17.3 (4.3)17.3 (4.3)16.5 (4.3)16.4 (3.7)
Dominant hand (L/R)5/557/4756/29725/12521/1078/342/31
Responding hand (L/R)29/3128/24167/17866/8166/6022/1913/18
Responding hand is dominant (N/Y)26/3427/25159/18663/8462/6419/2215/16
Disease duration (months)NA34.2 (15.6)34.2 (17.3)32.7 (16.6)34.9 (17.9)34.8 (17.7)37.4 (17.3)
Most affected side (L/R/Bi)NA27/23/5173/163/1766/79/565/57/617/21/415/16/2
Hoehn and Yahr-stageNA11/37/5/140/265/44/322/117/11/013/103/11/10/26/14/25/19/8/0
Medication use (N/Y)NA0/5417/33511/1393/1250/423/29
LEDDNA501.5 (220.3)545.2 (319.8)467.8 (256.5)573.8 (334.1)624.1 (353.7)715.4 (405.9)
MoCA27.6 (1.9)27.1 (1.8)26.7 (2.6)27.2 (2.0)26.7 (2.5)26.0 (3.5)25.9 (3.4)
Cognitive compositeNA0.2 (0.5)0.0 (0.6)0.3 (0.4)−0.2 (0.6)−0.4 (0.8)−0.3 (0.6)
MDS-UPDRS IINA7.7 (5.0)8.4 (5.6)5.9 (4.1)8.8 (4.8)15.3 (6.2)9.5 (6.8)
MDS-UPDRS III: OFF-state assessment
TotalNA33.6 (13.8)33.7 (13.0)29.6 (10.9)33.3 (12.3)47.8 (10.5)36.4 (15.6)
BradykinesiaNA16.2 (7.7)16.7 (7.5)14.4 (6.5)16.7 (7.0)24.6 (6.4)17.5 (8.3)
RigidityNA7.0 (3.0)6.6 (3.3)6.1 (3.2)6.6 (3.3)8.7 (3.0)6.4 (3.3)
PIGDNA2.1 (1.4)2.0 (1.4)1.6 (1.0)1.9 (1.1)3.9 (1.7)2.2 (1.4)
TremorNA5.8 (5.1)6.1 (5.2)5.9 (5.0)5.9 (5.2)7.2 (5.2)7.6 (5.6)
MDS-UPDRS III: ON-state assessment
TotalNA28.0 (13.3)28.7 (12.5)25.4 (10.6)29.2 (13.0)38.7 (10.8)28.3 (14.0)
BradykinesiaNA13.7 (7.2)14.4 (7.2)12.6 (6.5)14.5 (7.3)20.0 (6.2)13.7 (7.6)
RigidityNA6.0 (3.3)6.0 (3.3)5.5 (3.1)6.0 (3.5)7.7 (3.1)5.5 (3.3)
PIGDNA1.6 (1.3)1.6 (1.2)1.2 (0.9)1.6 (1.1)2.6 (1.6)1.7 (1.2)
TremorNA4.6 (3.5)5.0 (4.5)4.6 (4.1)5.3 (5.0)5.3 (4.4)5.2 (3.1)
VariableControlPD-OFFPD-ONMMPIMDMUndefined
Sample size60543531501284233
Age60.0 (9.6)61.1 (8.5)62.5 (8.6)61.4 (8.9)63.1 (8.5)65.5 (6.9)61.7 (8.8)
Sex (F/M)27/3322/32129/22467/8346/828/348/25
Years of education16.2 (3.3)17.1 (4.1)17.2 (4.1)17.3 (4.3)17.3 (4.3)16.5 (4.3)16.4 (3.7)
Dominant hand (L/R)5/557/4756/29725/12521/1078/342/31
Responding hand (L/R)29/3128/24167/17866/8166/6022/1913/18
Responding hand is dominant (N/Y)26/3427/25159/18663/8462/6419/2215/16
Disease duration (months)NA34.2 (15.6)34.2 (17.3)32.7 (16.6)34.9 (17.9)34.8 (17.7)37.4 (17.3)
Most affected side (L/R/Bi)NA27/23/5173/163/1766/79/565/57/617/21/415/16/2
Hoehn and Yahr-stageNA11/37/5/140/265/44/322/117/11/013/103/11/10/26/14/25/19/8/0
Medication use (N/Y)NA0/5417/33511/1393/1250/423/29
LEDDNA501.5 (220.3)545.2 (319.8)467.8 (256.5)573.8 (334.1)624.1 (353.7)715.4 (405.9)
MoCA27.6 (1.9)27.1 (1.8)26.7 (2.6)27.2 (2.0)26.7 (2.5)26.0 (3.5)25.9 (3.4)
Cognitive compositeNA0.2 (0.5)0.0 (0.6)0.3 (0.4)−0.2 (0.6)−0.4 (0.8)−0.3 (0.6)
MDS-UPDRS IINA7.7 (5.0)8.4 (5.6)5.9 (4.1)8.8 (4.8)15.3 (6.2)9.5 (6.8)
MDS-UPDRS III: OFF-state assessment
TotalNA33.6 (13.8)33.7 (13.0)29.6 (10.9)33.3 (12.3)47.8 (10.5)36.4 (15.6)
BradykinesiaNA16.2 (7.7)16.7 (7.5)14.4 (6.5)16.7 (7.0)24.6 (6.4)17.5 (8.3)
RigidityNA7.0 (3.0)6.6 (3.3)6.1 (3.2)6.6 (3.3)8.7 (3.0)6.4 (3.3)
PIGDNA2.1 (1.4)2.0 (1.4)1.6 (1.0)1.9 (1.1)3.9 (1.7)2.2 (1.4)
TremorNA5.8 (5.1)6.1 (5.2)5.9 (5.0)5.9 (5.2)7.2 (5.2)7.6 (5.6)
MDS-UPDRS III: ON-state assessment
TotalNA28.0 (13.3)28.7 (12.5)25.4 (10.6)29.2 (13.0)38.7 (10.8)28.3 (14.0)
BradykinesiaNA13.7 (7.2)14.4 (7.2)12.6 (6.5)14.5 (7.3)20.0 (6.2)13.7 (7.6)
RigidityNA6.0 (3.3)6.0 (3.3)5.5 (3.1)6.0 (3.5)7.7 (3.1)5.5 (3.3)
PIGDNA1.6 (1.3)1.6 (1.2)1.2 (0.9)1.6 (1.1)2.6 (1.6)1.7 (1.2)
TremorNA4.6 (3.5)5.0 (4.5)4.6 (4.1)5.3 (5.0)5.3 (4.4)5.2 (3.1)

Values are presented as mean (SD) unless otherwise stated. Bi = bilateral; DM = diffuse-malignant; F = female; FWEc = family-wise error cluster; IM = intermediate; L = left; LEDD = levodopa equivalent daily dose; M = male; MDS-UPDRS = Movement Disorders Society Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale; MMP = mild-motor predominant; MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment; N = no; PD-OFF = patients scanned in the OFF-medicated state; PD-ON = patients scanned in the ON-medicated state; PIGD = postural instability and gait disorder; R = right; Y = yes.

Action selection task

Task instructions

Participants performed an action selection task whilst undergoing mixed block/event-related functional MRI (Fig. 1A).59-61 This task was specifically designed to elicit compensatory mechanisms that may contribute to preserving motor (bradykinesia) and cognitive function. This was achieved by assessing action selection deficits resulting from Parkinson’s disease under varying levels of task difficulty.21,22 Participants were instructed to respond to highlighted cues with a single button press as quickly and as accurately as possible and to try to make equal use of all response options. The number of highlighted cues varied between one and three. If multiple cues were highlighted, participants were instructed to choose and respond to one cue only, which imposed greater demands on action selection capabilities. Behavioural performance was assessed during low (one-choice), moderate (two-choice) and high (three-choice) action selection demand. See Supplementary material for a more detailed account of the task.

Action selection task and Parkinson-related deficits. (A) Participants respond to highlighted circles with one out of four response buttons. When multiple circles are highlighted, participants are instructed to select one response button. Action selection demand is parametrically manipulated by varying the number of highlighted circles that are presented. During catch trials, participants are instructed to withhold a response. No feedback was given to indicate the correctness of responses. (B) Parkinson’s disease leads to general deficits in task performance. (C) Parkinson’s disease leads to reduced motor-related activity in a network of core motor regions and reduced selection-related deactivation in the middle temporal gyrus. Box plots show the first and third quartiles (boxes), median (horizontal line), mean (diamond) and 1.5 × interquartile range (whiskers). Brain images show t-values of significant clusters. FWEc = Family-wise error cluster; HC = healthy control; PD = Parkinson’s disease; PD-On = ON-medicated Parkinson’s disease; SE = standard error. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001.
Figure 1

Action selection task and Parkinson-related deficits. (A) Participants respond to highlighted circles with one out of four response buttons. When multiple circles are highlighted, participants are instructed to select one response button. Action selection demand is parametrically manipulated by varying the number of highlighted circles that are presented. During catch trials, participants are instructed to withhold a response. No feedback was given to indicate the correctness of responses. (B) Parkinson’s disease leads to general deficits in task performance. (C) Parkinson’s disease leads to reduced motor-related activity in a network of core motor regions and reduced selection-related deactivation in the middle temporal gyrus. Box plots show the first and third quartiles (boxes), median (horizontal line), mean (diamond) and 1.5 × interquartile range (whiskers). Brain images show t-values of significant clusters. FWEc = Family-wise error cluster; HC = healthy control; PD = Parkinson’s disease; PD-On = ON-medicated Parkinson’s disease; SE = standard error. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001.

Measurements of behavioural performance

Response times, errors and misses were aggregated within participants by trial condition (one-choice, two-choice, three-choice) and block (one, two, three), resulting in nine values per participant for each measurement. Response times were aggregated by taking the median after excluding misses, incorrect responses and response times below 300 ms. Errors were similarly aggregated by taking the number of incorrect trials divided by the total number of correct and incorrect trials, excluding misses. Misses were aggregated by taking the number of misses divided by the total number of trials. In trials where multiple choices were possible, participants sometimes had the option to repeat their previous response or switch to a new response. Based on such trials, two additional metrics (response variability and switching) were calculated to characterize the use of stereotyped response strategies.53,62 First, for each of the four response options, the number of button presses were calculated. Response variability (i.e. the degree to which all available response options were used) was subsequently characterized as a coefficient of variation, calculated by taking the standard deviation of button presses across response options divided by the mean. Larger coefficients of variation indicate that button presses are not uniformly distributed (i.e. that there was a bias towards one or more response options). Second, response switching (i.e. the degree to which the same response was repeated over consecutive trials) was characterized as the ratio of switches relative to the total number of switches and repeats. Smaller ratios indicate that response repetitions are more likely than switches.

Subtyping

We used a recently developed57 and previously validated46,63 clinical subtype classification to parse clinical heterogeneity. The classification used in the present study has been described in detail in a previous publication where we investigated clinical differences between subtypes in the larger Personalized Parkinson Project cohort.46 In short, patients were classified based on motor symptoms, cognitive function, REM movement sleep behaviour disorder and autonomic function (Supplementary material). Classification resulted in three distinct subtypes: diffuse-malignant (n = 42), intermediate (n = 128) and mild-motor predominant (n = 150). Thirty-three patients could not be classified due to missing data. In short, the diffuse-malignant subtype showed relatively severe motor symptoms, impairment in more clinical domains and faster progression in comparison to the mild-motor predominant subtype.46

Clinical measurements

Action selection involves motor performance under cognitive load. Clinical scores of motor symptoms and cognitive performance were therefore used to test for associations with task-related performance and brain activity. In the motor domain, bradykinesia was specifically selected as a symptom-of-interest owing to its previously established relationship with action selection.18-22 Bradykinesia severity was defined as the sum of 11 MDS-UPDRS III items (assessed in the OFF-medicated state) targeting bradykinesia (4–9 and 14).64,65 Cognitive performance was defined as the same composite score that was used for subtype classification, which was based on extensive neuropsychological assessment. This composite score was computed by taking the mean across age-, education- and sex-adjusted z-scores from tests in six cognitive domains (visuospatial perception, executive function, semantic fluency, processing speed, working memory and episodic memory; Supplementary material).

Image acquisition and preprocessing

All scans were acquired using a Siemens MAGNETOM Prisma 3T (Siemens) equipped with a 32-channel head coil. T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence [repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE)/inversion time (TI) = 2000/2/880 ms; flip angle = 8°; voxel size = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm; slices = 192; field of view (FOV) = 256 mm; scanning time = 5 min). T2*-weighted functional images were acquired during the performance of the action selection task using a multi-band sequence (TR/TE = 1000/34 ms; acceleration factor = 6; acquisition mode = interleaved; flip angle = 60°; voxel-size = 2.0 × 2.0 × 2.0 mm; slices = 72; FOV = 210 mm; scanning time = 9–10 min).

Preprocessing of functional MRI data was performed using a standardized pipeline in fmriprep (v20.2.1).66 In short, functional images were motion- and slice time-corrected and normalized to MNI152Lin6Asym-space. Lastly, corrected and normalized images were spatially smoothed with Gaussian kernel of 6 mm at full-width half-maximum. See Supplementary material for detailed information.

First-level analysis

SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12) was used to carry out all first-level and group-level analyses. Task regressors were generated for one-, two- and three-choice conditions by convolving cue onsets with a canonical haemodynamic response function. Cue duration was defined as the average response time across choice conditions. Time derivatives were included for each condition together with parametric regressors for response time and their first-order derivatives. Additional regressors were generated for catch trials and incorrect responses. Confound time series were included in the first-level model to correct for anatomical and motion-related sources of noise (Supplementary material). Task-related activity for each choice condition was defined by contrasting each separate task regressor against an implicit baseline (one-choice > 0; two-choice > 0; three-choice > 0; catch > 0). The resulting contrast images were used as inputs for group comparisons. Additional contrasts were formed to encode motor-related [(one-choice, two-choice, three-choice) > 0] and selection-related (two-choice > one-choice; three-choice > one-choice) activity (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for mean activation associated with each contrast of interest). These contrasts were used to assess associations with bradykinesia severity and cognitive performance. Contrast images of all participants who responded with the left side were flipped horizontally to ensure that the most-affected side was consistent across patients.

Statistical analysis

Behavioural performance

The influences of disease status (between-subjects factor Group: ON-medicated patients versus controls), dopaminergic medication (within-subjects factor Group: ON- versus OFF-medicated states) and subtype (between-subjects factor Group: mild-motor predominant versus intermediate versus diffuse-malignant; ON-medicated state only) were assessed with linear mixed-effects models for log-transformed response times and with weighted binomial logistic mixed-effects models for error and miss rates using the lme4-package67 in R 4.2.1 (R Core Team). Each model included fixed effects for Group and Choice (within-subjects factor Choice: one-choice, two-choice, three-choice) as well as their interaction. Repeated measures were accounted for with by-subject random intercepts. Block was included as a fixed effect to account for task habituation. Associations between behavioural performance and clinical severity were assessed within patients using the same model formula, with the exception that the factor of Group was removed and a term was added for Symptom severity, defined as the bradykinesia subscore of the MDS-UPDRS III (assessed in the OFF-medicated state) or the cognitive composite score. Models were fitted using a restricted maximum likelihood approach. P-values for fixed effects were derived through type III analyses of deviance using Wald χ2 tests. Response variability and switching were analysed using one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs), with Group as a between-subjects factor. Clinical associations were assessed within patients using similar mixed-effects modelling as above. All behavioural analyses included age, sex, years of education and dominance of the responding hand as covariates of non-interest. Two-tailed post hoc t-tests were performed on estimated marginal means. Participants with less than 25% correct responses on one-choice trials, averaged across blocks, were excluded from further analysis. In analyses of response variability and switching, additional outliers scoring above or below three standard deviations from the mean were excluded.

Brain activity

Group comparisons of task-related brain activity closely followed the group comparisons that were conducted for behavioural performance. Three separate repeated-measures ANCOVAs, implemented using the full factorial design option in SPM12, were used to carry out voxel-wise tests for effects of disease status (ON-medicated patients versus controls), medication (ON- versus OFF-medicated states) and subtype (mild-motor predominant versus intermediate versus diffuse-malignant; patients assessed in the ON-medicated state) on brain activity. Estimates of brain activity for each task condition (one-choice, two-choice, three-choice, catch) were used as inputs. Contrasts were set up to compare motor- and selection-related activity between groups (see Supplementary material for average task-related effects). The comparison between subtypes was followed by a post hoc comparison between each subtype and controls.

The relationship between brain activity and clinical severity was investigated with voxel-wise one-way ANCOVAs where bradykinesia severity and cognitive performance were simultaneously considered as regressors of interest and fitted to first-level contrasts of motor- and selection-related activity. This enabled the assessment of the contributions of bradykinesia and cognitive performance independently of each other. Separate correlation analyses for each clinical domain (Supplementary material) and a conjunction analysis were conducted post hoc to assess between-domain cluster overlap. Mean framewise displacement, age, sex, years of education and responding hand dominance were included as covariates of non-interest in all analyses of brain activity. Here, years of education controlled for individual differences in functional reserve68,69 whereas hand dominance controlled for differential extent of bilateral recruitment between dominant versus non-dominant hand responders.70,71 Cluster-based thresholding, with a cluster-forming threshold of Z = 3.1, was used to correct for family-wise error at P < 0.05.72 Anatomical labels and functional subdivisions of significant clusters were derived from the JuBrain Anatomy Toolbox (v3.0)73 and Glasser atlas,74 respectively.

Two control analyses were carried out. First, interindividual differences in compensatory capacity may be influenced by underlying differences in reserve, which can be defined as a pre-morbid accumulation of neural resources that reduces susceptibility to the effects of pathological decline.11,69,75 In the analyses above, four reserve proxies were already accounted for (i.e. age, sex, years of education and dominant side onset).76 However, reserve may be influenced by additional factors. This was explored in between-subtype comparisons and brain-clinical correlations that adjusted for four additional reserve proxies: body-mass index, smoking history, physical activity and non-motor burden (Supplementary material).1,68,76 Second, between-group differences in cortical compensation may result from underlying differences in brain structure. Voxel-based morphometry was therefore used to compare gray matter volume in regions that showed significant group differences in task-related activity (Supplementary material).

Results

The influence of disease status

Behavioural performance

Response times were ‘longer’ in Parkinson patients than controls [Fig. 1B; main effect of Group χ2(1) = 11.2, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.03; patient > control; log-ratio = 1.07, standard error (SE) = 0.02, t-ratio(391) = 3.3, P = 0.001], and they increased with action selection demand [main effect of Choice χ2(2) = 79.7, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.07; two-choice > one-choice; log-ratio = 1.08, SE = 0.006, t-ratio(3170) = 14.0, P < 0.001; three-choice > one-choice; log-ratio = 1.08, SE = 0.006, t-ratio(3170) = 13.5, P < 0.001]. Patients were not disproportionally slower for multi-choice trials than controls (no Group × Choice interaction, P = 0.86).

Error rates were higher in Parkinson patients than controls specifically during single-choice versus multi-choice trials compared with controls [Fig. 1B; Group × Choice χ2(2) = 6.4, P = 0.041; one-choice > three-choice, patient > control; odds ratio (OR) = 2.0, SE = 0.55, Z-ratio = 2.5, P = 0.035].

Miss rates were ‘higher’ in Parkinson patients than controls [main effect of Group χ2(1) = 10.0, P = 0.002; patient > control; OR = 3.5, SE = 1.4, Z-ratio = 3.2, P = 0.001]. There was no effect of Choice (P = 0.42).

There was no effect of Group on response variability (Supplementary Fig. 2A; P = 0.09) or switching (Supplementary Fig. 3A; P = 0.24).

Brain activity

Motor-related activity

Patients with Parkinson’s disease showed (reduced) motor-related activity in the left putamen, left precentral gyrus (area 4) and right cerebellum (IV–V) compared with controls (Table 2 and Fig. 1B; control > patient, mean > baseline).

Table 2

Voxel-wise group comparisons

Anatomical label (% cluster volume)AreaP-value (FWEc-corrected)Cluster extent (voxels)Max t-valueMNI: x, y, z
Influence of Parkinson’s disease
Control > PD-ON, Mean > Baseline
 L putamen (88%)<0.0014335.6−27,−15,6
 L precentral gyrus (68%)M1<0.0014165.4−31,−27,62
 R cerebellum (71%)IV-V<0.0013135.617,−47,−20
PD-ON > Control, Two > One
 R putamen (45%)0.0171204.731,−13,8
PD-ON > Control, Three > One
 R middle temporal gyrus (82%)TE1p0.0321855.161,−37,−14
Influence of subtype
Mild-motor predominant > Diffuse-malignant, Mean > Baseline
 R postcentral gyrus (87%)S10.0101163.951,−31,56
Mild-motor predominant > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 R middle frontal gyrus (72%)i6-8<0.0014214.635,17,52
 R inferior parietal lobule (61%)PFm<0.0012204.247,−55,38
 R superior parietal lobule (96%)7Pm0.0021504.35,−59,52
 L superior parietal lobule (87%)7Am0.035984.4−11,−67,64
Intermediate > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 L middle frontal gyrus (68%)6a0.029944.6−21,3,56
Post hoc comparison between subtypes and controls
Mild-motor predominant > Control, Three > One
 R inferior parietal lobule (48%)PFm<0.0013845.451,−63,46
 R middle temporal gyrus (88%)TE1p0.0021514.361,−37,−14
 R superior frontal gyrus (72%)9a0.0041344.123,56,30
 R middle frontal gyrus (70%)8Av0.0161024.427,11,40
Intermediate > Control, Two > One
 R putamen (42%)0.0021495.631,−13,8
Control > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 L middle frontal gyrus (83%)6a0.0091144.3−25,5,60
Anatomical label (% cluster volume)AreaP-value (FWEc-corrected)Cluster extent (voxels)Max t-valueMNI: x, y, z
Influence of Parkinson’s disease
Control > PD-ON, Mean > Baseline
 L putamen (88%)<0.0014335.6−27,−15,6
 L precentral gyrus (68%)M1<0.0014165.4−31,−27,62
 R cerebellum (71%)IV-V<0.0013135.617,−47,−20
PD-ON > Control, Two > One
 R putamen (45%)0.0171204.731,−13,8
PD-ON > Control, Three > One
 R middle temporal gyrus (82%)TE1p0.0321855.161,−37,−14
Influence of subtype
Mild-motor predominant > Diffuse-malignant, Mean > Baseline
 R postcentral gyrus (87%)S10.0101163.951,−31,56
Mild-motor predominant > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 R middle frontal gyrus (72%)i6-8<0.0014214.635,17,52
 R inferior parietal lobule (61%)PFm<0.0012204.247,−55,38
 R superior parietal lobule (96%)7Pm0.0021504.35,−59,52
 L superior parietal lobule (87%)7Am0.035984.4−11,−67,64
Intermediate > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 L middle frontal gyrus (68%)6a0.029944.6−21,3,56
Post hoc comparison between subtypes and controls
Mild-motor predominant > Control, Three > One
 R inferior parietal lobule (48%)PFm<0.0013845.451,−63,46
 R middle temporal gyrus (88%)TE1p0.0021514.361,−37,−14
 R superior frontal gyrus (72%)9a0.0041344.123,56,30
 R middle frontal gyrus (70%)8Av0.0161024.427,11,40
Intermediate > Control, Two > One
 R putamen (42%)0.0021495.631,−13,8
Control > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 L middle frontal gyrus (83%)6a0.0091144.3−25,5,60

Anatomical labels were derived from the Anatomy Toolbox v3.0. Area labels were derived from the Glasser atlas. FWEc = family-wise error cluster; L = left; MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute; PD-ON = Patients scanned in the ON-medicated state; R = right.

Table 2

Voxel-wise group comparisons

Anatomical label (% cluster volume)AreaP-value (FWEc-corrected)Cluster extent (voxels)Max t-valueMNI: x, y, z
Influence of Parkinson’s disease
Control > PD-ON, Mean > Baseline
 L putamen (88%)<0.0014335.6−27,−15,6
 L precentral gyrus (68%)M1<0.0014165.4−31,−27,62
 R cerebellum (71%)IV-V<0.0013135.617,−47,−20
PD-ON > Control, Two > One
 R putamen (45%)0.0171204.731,−13,8
PD-ON > Control, Three > One
 R middle temporal gyrus (82%)TE1p0.0321855.161,−37,−14
Influence of subtype
Mild-motor predominant > Diffuse-malignant, Mean > Baseline
 R postcentral gyrus (87%)S10.0101163.951,−31,56
Mild-motor predominant > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 R middle frontal gyrus (72%)i6-8<0.0014214.635,17,52
 R inferior parietal lobule (61%)PFm<0.0012204.247,−55,38
 R superior parietal lobule (96%)7Pm0.0021504.35,−59,52
 L superior parietal lobule (87%)7Am0.035984.4−11,−67,64
Intermediate > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 L middle frontal gyrus (68%)6a0.029944.6−21,3,56
Post hoc comparison between subtypes and controls
Mild-motor predominant > Control, Three > One
 R inferior parietal lobule (48%)PFm<0.0013845.451,−63,46
 R middle temporal gyrus (88%)TE1p0.0021514.361,−37,−14
 R superior frontal gyrus (72%)9a0.0041344.123,56,30
 R middle frontal gyrus (70%)8Av0.0161024.427,11,40
Intermediate > Control, Two > One
 R putamen (42%)0.0021495.631,−13,8
Control > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 L middle frontal gyrus (83%)6a0.0091144.3−25,5,60
Anatomical label (% cluster volume)AreaP-value (FWEc-corrected)Cluster extent (voxels)Max t-valueMNI: x, y, z
Influence of Parkinson’s disease
Control > PD-ON, Mean > Baseline
 L putamen (88%)<0.0014335.6−27,−15,6
 L precentral gyrus (68%)M1<0.0014165.4−31,−27,62
 R cerebellum (71%)IV-V<0.0013135.617,−47,−20
PD-ON > Control, Two > One
 R putamen (45%)0.0171204.731,−13,8
PD-ON > Control, Three > One
 R middle temporal gyrus (82%)TE1p0.0321855.161,−37,−14
Influence of subtype
Mild-motor predominant > Diffuse-malignant, Mean > Baseline
 R postcentral gyrus (87%)S10.0101163.951,−31,56
Mild-motor predominant > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 R middle frontal gyrus (72%)i6-8<0.0014214.635,17,52
 R inferior parietal lobule (61%)PFm<0.0012204.247,−55,38
 R superior parietal lobule (96%)7Pm0.0021504.35,−59,52
 L superior parietal lobule (87%)7Am0.035984.4−11,−67,64
Intermediate > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 L middle frontal gyrus (68%)6a0.029944.6−21,3,56
Post hoc comparison between subtypes and controls
Mild-motor predominant > Control, Three > One
 R inferior parietal lobule (48%)PFm<0.0013845.451,−63,46
 R middle temporal gyrus (88%)TE1p0.0021514.361,−37,−14
 R superior frontal gyrus (72%)9a0.0041344.123,56,30
 R middle frontal gyrus (70%)8Av0.0161024.427,11,40
Intermediate > Control, Two > One
 R putamen (42%)0.0021495.631,−13,8
Control > Diffuse-malignant, Three > One
 L middle frontal gyrus (83%)6a0.0091144.3−25,5,60

Anatomical labels were derived from the Anatomy Toolbox v3.0. Area labels were derived from the Glasser atlas. FWEc = family-wise error cluster; L = left; MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute; PD-ON = Patients scanned in the ON-medicated state; R = right.

Selection-related activity

Patients with Parkinson’s disease showed differential activity in the right putamen at moderate demand compared with controls (Table 2 and Fig. 1B; patient > control, two-choice > one-choice), which was driven by a relative reduction in putamen activity during one-choice trials for patients versus controls (Supplementary Fig. 4A). Furthermore, patients showed increased activity in the right middle temporal gyrus (TE1p) at high demand compared with controls (Table 2 and Fig. 2B; patient > control, three-choice > one-choice), which was driven by opposing directions of selection-related effects between groups (Supplementary Fig. 4A).

Evidence for parieto-premotor compensation. (A) Greater selection-related increase in response times in intermediate compared to mild-motor predominant patients. (B) Reduced motor-related activity in diffuse-malignant compared to mild-motor predominant and intermediate patients. Box plots show the first and third quartiles (boxes), median (horizontal line), mean ( diamond) and 1.5 × interquartile range (whiskers). Brain images show t-values of significant clusters. DM = diffuse-malignant; FWEc = family-wise error cluster; IM = intermediate; L = left; MMP = mild-motor predominant; R = right. +P < 0.09, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.
Figure 2

Evidence for parieto-premotor compensation. (A) Greater selection-related increase in response times in intermediate compared to mild-motor predominant patients. (B) Reduced motor-related activity in diffuse-malignant compared to mild-motor predominant and intermediate patients. Box plots show the first and third quartiles (boxes), median (horizontal line), mean ( diamond) and 1.5 × interquartile range (whiskers). Brain images show t-values of significant clusters. DM = diffuse-malignant; FWEc = family-wise error cluster; IM = intermediate; L = left; MMP = mild-motor predominant; R = right. +P < 0.09, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.

The influence of clinical subtype

Behavioural performance

The influence of action selection demand on response times differed between subtypes [Fig. 2A; Group × Choice χ2(4) = 11.9, P = 0.018, η2p = 0.005]. The difference in response times between high and low action selection demand was ‘increased’ for intermediate compared with mild-motor predominant patients [intermediate > mild-motor predominant, three-choice > one-choice; log-ratio = 1.028, SE = 0.01, t-ratio(2456) = 2.8, P = 0.032] but not for diffuse-malignant compared with mild-motor predominant patients (P = 0.96).

Error rate increased as a function of action selection demand [Fig. 2A; main effect of Choice χ2(2) = 34.5, P < 0.001; one-choice > two-choice (OR = 1.3, SE = 0.11, Z-ratio = 2.5, P = 0.031), one-choice > three-choice (OR = 2.6, SE = 0.29, Z-ratio = 8.6, P < 0.001), two-choice > three-choice (OR = 2.1, SE = 0.26, Z-ratio = 5.8, P < 0.001)]. There was a trend towards increased error rates for diffuse-malignant compared to mild-motor predominant patients [main effect of Group χ2(2) = 5.7, P = 0.058; diffuse-malignant > mild-motor predominant (OR = 1.8, SE = 0.54, P = 0.089)].

There was no effect of Group on miss rates (P = 0.14), response variability (Supplementary Fig. 2A; P = 0.64) or switching (Supplementary Fig. 3A; P = 0.24).

Brain activity

Motor-related activity

Patients with a mild-motor predominant subtype had ‘increased’ motor-related activity in the right postcentral gyrus (area 2) compared to patients with a diffuse-malignant subtype (Table 2 and Fig. 2B; mild-motor predominant > diffuse-malignant, mean > baseline). There were no differences in basal ganglia activity between subtypes (Supplementary Fig. 5).

Selection-related activity

Patients with a mild-motor predominant subtype had ‘increased’ selection-related activity in the right middle frontal gyrus (i6-8), right inferior parietal lobule (PFm), right superior parietal lobule (7Pm) and left superior parietal lobule (7Am) compared to patients with a diffuse-malignant subtype (Table 2 and Fig. 2C; mild-motor predominant > diffuse-malignant, three-choice > one-choice). Furthermore, patients with an intermediate subtype also had ‘increased’ selection-related activity in the left middle frontal gyrus (6a) compared with the diffuse-malignant subtype (Fig. 2C; intermediate > diffuse-malignant, three-choice > one-choice). These results remained significant after accounting for additional measures of reserve (Supplementary material), suggesting that a more benign clinical phenotype is associated with ‘higher’ selection-related activation in a network that involves premotor, inferior parietal and superior parietal cortex.

Post hoc comparisons of selection-related activity between subtypes and controls were carried out to assess the clinical relevance of the results above. Patients with a mild-motor predominant subtype had ‘increased’ selection-related activity in the right inferior parietal lobule (PFm), right middle temporal gyrus (TE1p), right superior frontal gyrus (9a) and right middle frontal gyrus (8Av) compared with controls (Fig. 3; mild-motor predominant > control, three-choice > one-choice). In contrast, patients with a diffuse-malignant subtype had ‘decreased’ selection-related activity in the left middle frontal gyrus (6a) compared with controls (Fig. 3; control > mild-motor predominant, three-choice > one-choice). Patients with an intermediate subtype had differential selection-related activity in the right putamen (intermediate > control, two-choice > one-choice), which was driven by ‘reduced’ putamen activity in patients versus controls for the one-choice condition (Supplementary Fig. 4B).

Post hoc comparisons between subtypes and controls. Reduced selection-related activity in diffuse-malignant compared to mild-motor predominant and intermediate patients (MMP > HC, Three > One). In comparison to controls, mild-motor predominant patients show increased selection-related parieto-premotor activity whereas diffuse-malignant patients show the opposite (HC > DM, Three > One). Box plots show the first and third quartiles (boxes), median (horizontal line), mean (diamond) and 1.5 × interquartile range (whiskers). Brain images show t-values of significant clusters. DM = diffuse-malignant; FWEc = family-wise error cluster; HC = healthy control; L = left; MMP = mild-motor predominant; R = right. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
Figure 3

Post hoc comparisons between subtypes and controls. Reduced selection-related activity in diffuse-malignant compared to mild-motor predominant and intermediate patients (MMP > HC, Three > One). In comparison to controls, mild-motor predominant patients show increased selection-related parieto-premotor activity whereas diffuse-malignant patients show the opposite (HC > DM, Three > One). Box plots show the first and third quartiles (boxes), median (horizontal line), mean (diamond) and 1.5 × interquartile range (whiskers). Brain images show t-values of significant clusters. DM = diffuse-malignant; FWEc = family-wise error cluster; HC = healthy control; L = left; MMP = mild-motor predominant; R = right. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Associations with clinical severity

Behavioural performance

Bradykinesia severity was inversely related to cognitive performance [r = −0.28, t(330) = 5.2, P < 0.001].

Response times ‘increased’ as a function of bradykinesia severity [Fig. 4A; main effect of Severity χ2(1) = 14.3, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.04] and ‘decreased’ as a function of cognitive performance [Fig. 4A; main effect of Severity χ2(1) = 34.1, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.11]. There were no interactions between Severity and Choice (both P > 0.3).

Associations with specific clinical domains. (A) Task performance predicts bradykinesia severity (left) and cognitive performance (right). (B) Associations with bradykinesia severity. (C) Associations with cognitive performance. Line-plots show linear associations (solid line) and standard errors (outline). Brain images show t-values of significant clusters. Bradykinesia severity was defined as a subscore of the Movement Disorders Society Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale part III (MDS-UPDRS part III). Cognitive performance was defined as a composite score of clinical assessments across multiple cognitive domains. FWEc = family-wise error cluster; L = left; PMC = premotor cortex; SE = standard error; SPL = superior parietal lobule; R = right; ± = positive/negative correlation. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
Figure 4

Associations with specific clinical domains. (A) Task performance predicts bradykinesia severity (left) and cognitive performance (right). (B) Associations with bradykinesia severity. (C) Associations with cognitive performance. Line-plots show linear associations (solid line) and standard errors (outline). Brain images show t-values of significant clusters. Bradykinesia severity was defined as a subscore of the Movement Disorders Society Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale part III (MDS-UPDRS part III). Cognitive performance was defined as a composite score of clinical assessments across multiple cognitive domains. FWEc = family-wise error cluster; L = left; PMC = premotor cortex; SE = standard error; SPL = superior parietal lobule; R = right; ± = positive/negative correlation. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

The influence of action selection demand on error rates depended on bradykinesia severity [Severity × Choice χ2(2) = 11.2, P = 0.003] and cognitive performance [Severity × Choice χ2(2) = 13.4, P = 0.001]. Patients with more severe bradykinesia had ‘increased’ error rates [one-choice > three-choice; β = 0.042, SE = 0.013, Z-ratio = 3.12, P = 0.005], whereas patients with better cognitive performance had ‘decreased’ error rates [one-choice > three-choice; β = −0.470, SE = 0.141, Z-ratio = 3.35, P = 0.002] during low versus high action selection demands.

Patients with more severe bradykinesia had ‘increased’ miss rates [χ2(1) = 10.1, P = 0.001, OR = 1.059], whereas patients with better cognitive performance had ‘decreased’ miss rates [χ2(1) = 41.0, P < 0.001, OR = 0.276]. There were no interactions between Severity and Choice (both P < 0.14).

Patients with better cognitive performance had ‘decreased’ button press variability [Supplementary Fig. 2B; F(1) = 6.6, P = 0.011, η2p = 0.04] and ‘increased’ switch rates [Supplementary Fig. 3B; F(1) = 7.4, P = 0.007, η2p = 0.05]. There were no associations between bradykinesia and response variability (Supplementary Fig. 2B; P = 0.15) or switching (Supplementary Fig. 3B; P = 0.43).

Brain activity

Motor-related activity

There were no associations with motor-related activity.

Selection-related activity

At moderate demands, lower bradykinesia severity was associated with ‘greater’ selection-related activity in the left superior parietal lobule (7Am) and superior frontal gyrus (6ma; Table 3 and Fig. 4B; negative correlation, two-choice > one-choice), whereas better cognitive performance was associated with ‘greater’ selection-related activity in the right middle frontal gyrus (i6-8; Table 3 and Fig. 4C; positive correlation, two-choice > one-choice). At high demand, associations with bradykinesia became more constrained to the superior parietal cortex, while associations with cognitive performance extended to additional prefrontal regions. Specifically, lower bradykinesia severity was associated with ‘greater’ selection-related activity in the left superior parietal lobule (7Am; Table 3 and Fig. 3B; negative correlation, three-choice > one-choice). Better cognitive performance was associated with ‘greater’ selection-related activity in the right middle frontal gyrus (areas i6-8 and p9-46v) and right inferior frontal gyrus (6r; Table 3 and Fig. 4C; positive correlation, three-choice > one-choice) and with ‘less’ selection-related activity in the left cuneus (V2; negative correlation, three-choice > one-choice). A post hoc conjunction analysis revealed no brain regions where selection-related activity correlated with bradykinesia severity and cognitive performance. When analysed separately, bradykinesia severity (Supplementary Fig. 7A) and cognitive performance (Supplementary Fig. 7B) showed negative and positive correlations, respectively, with a wider network of parieto-premotor regions as a function of increasing action selection-demand (Supplementary material and Supplementary Tables 1 and 2).

Table 3

Voxel-wise clinical correlations

Anatomical label (% cluster volume)AreaP-value (FWEc-corrected)Cluster extent (voxels)Max t-valueMNI: x, y, z
Two > One
Bradykinesia, negative correlation
 L superior parietal lobule (83%)7Am<0.0013814.9−9,−61,52
 L superior frontal gyrus (76%)6ma0.0021524.4−15,19,66
Cognitive performance, positive correlation
 R middle frontal gyrus (97%)i6-80.0161034.335,−1,58
Three > One
Bradykinesia, negative
 L superior parietal lobule (92%)7Am0.0011664.3−9,−63,54
Cognitive performance, positive correlation
 R middle frontal gyrus (52%)i6-80.0031474.031,9,46
 R middle frontal gyrus (54%)p9-46v0.0131144.647,36,28
 R inferior frontal gyrus (62%)6r0.029963.945,9,28
Cognitive performance, negative correlation
 L cuneus (70%)V20.0221024.5−9,−94,20
Anatomical label (% cluster volume)AreaP-value (FWEc-corrected)Cluster extent (voxels)Max t-valueMNI: x, y, z
Two > One
Bradykinesia, negative correlation
 L superior parietal lobule (83%)7Am<0.0013814.9−9,−61,52
 L superior frontal gyrus (76%)6ma0.0021524.4−15,19,66
Cognitive performance, positive correlation
 R middle frontal gyrus (97%)i6-80.0161034.335,−1,58
Three > One
Bradykinesia, negative
 L superior parietal lobule (92%)7Am0.0011664.3−9,−63,54
Cognitive performance, positive correlation
 R middle frontal gyrus (52%)i6-80.0031474.031,9,46
 R middle frontal gyrus (54%)p9-46v0.0131144.647,36,28
 R inferior frontal gyrus (62%)6r0.029963.945,9,28
Cognitive performance, negative correlation
 L cuneus (70%)V20.0221024.5−9,−94,20

Anatomical labels were derived from the Anatomy Toolbox v3.0. Area labels were derived from the Glasser atlas. FWEc = family-wise error cluster; L = left; MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute; R = right.

Table 3

Voxel-wise clinical correlations

Anatomical label (% cluster volume)AreaP-value (FWEc-corrected)Cluster extent (voxels)Max t-valueMNI: x, y, z
Two > One
Bradykinesia, negative correlation
 L superior parietal lobule (83%)7Am<0.0013814.9−9,−61,52
 L superior frontal gyrus (76%)6ma0.0021524.4−15,19,66
Cognitive performance, positive correlation
 R middle frontal gyrus (97%)i6-80.0161034.335,−1,58
Three > One
Bradykinesia, negative
 L superior parietal lobule (92%)7Am0.0011664.3−9,−63,54
Cognitive performance, positive correlation
 R middle frontal gyrus (52%)i6-80.0031474.031,9,46
 R middle frontal gyrus (54%)p9-46v0.0131144.647,36,28
 R inferior frontal gyrus (62%)6r0.029963.945,9,28
Cognitive performance, negative correlation
 L cuneus (70%)V20.0221024.5−9,−94,20
Anatomical label (% cluster volume)AreaP-value (FWEc-corrected)Cluster extent (voxels)Max t-valueMNI: x, y, z
Two > One
Bradykinesia, negative correlation
 L superior parietal lobule (83%)7Am<0.0013814.9−9,−61,52
 L superior frontal gyrus (76%)6ma0.0021524.4−15,19,66
Cognitive performance, positive correlation
 R middle frontal gyrus (97%)i6-80.0161034.335,−1,58
Three > One
Bradykinesia, negative
 L superior parietal lobule (92%)7Am0.0011664.3−9,−63,54
Cognitive performance, positive correlation
 R middle frontal gyrus (52%)i6-80.0031474.031,9,46
 R middle frontal gyrus (54%)p9-46v0.0131144.647,36,28
 R inferior frontal gyrus (62%)6r0.029963.945,9,28
Cognitive performance, negative correlation
 L cuneus (70%)V20.0221024.5−9,−94,20

Anatomical labels were derived from the Anatomy Toolbox v3.0. Area labels were derived from the Glasser atlas. FWEc = family-wise error cluster; L = left; MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute; R = right.

The influence of medication

Motor symptom severity decreased following dopaminergic medication [MDS-UPDRS-III total χ2(1) = 221.1, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.40; bradykinesia χ2(1) = 127.3, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.29]. Dopaminergic medication led to ‘decreased’ miss rates during low versus high action selection demand [Group × Choice χ2(1) = 8.0, P = 0.019; Parkinson’s disease-ON > Parkinson’s disease-OFF, Three > One (OR = 0.35, SE = 0.14, Z-ratio = 2.72, P = 0.018)]. There were no additional effects of dopaminergic medication on task performance or brain activity (Supplementary material and Supplementary Fig. 8).

Discussion

We investigated the cerebral mechanisms underlying clinical heterogeneity in Parkinson's disease in a cohort of early-to-moderately affected56 patients (n = 353) and healthy controls (n = 60). By leveraging clinical subtyping and brain-symptom associations, we showed that lower symptom severity was consistently associated with higher activation in superior parietal and premotor cortex, particularly during high demands on action selection. In contrast, we found no evidence for a relationship between symptom severity and basal ganglia activity, which was reduced in all clinical subtypes, independently of symptom severity, compared to controls. These findings support the hypothesis that interindividual variability in symptom severity in Parkinson’s disease may be determined by compensatory cortical processes rather than basal ganglia dysfunction.

Action selection deficits are associated with basal ganglia dysfunction and clinical severity

We used an action selection paradigm to investigate cerebral mechanisms underlying abnormal motor control in Parkinson’s disease, which implicates both motor symptoms (e.g. bradykinesia) and cognitive deficits (e.g. decision-making), and which was designed to elicit cortical compensatory mechanisms. Consistent with previous neuroimaging and behavioural studies, we found that Parkinson’s disease was associated with reduced motor-related activity in a core network of sensorimotor regions (putamen, primary motor cortex and cerebellum) accompanied by general slowing of motor performance, lower accuracy and more misses.16,17,77,78 Furthermore, larger deficits in each of the three behavioural indices were associated with higher severity of bradykinesia and lower cognitive performance. In combination, these findings suggest that the task was sensitive to both action selection deficits and basal ganglia dysfunction.

This sensitivity was built into the task by virtue of two design improvements over previous action selection tasks.21,53,79 First, we varied the number of response options parametrically on a trial-by-trial basis, using a mixed block/event-related design with a jittered inter-stimulus interval.59-61 We also explicitly instructed participants to make equal use of all response options. This reduced predictability in the task, motivating participants to generate responses based on a process of selection rather than a predefined strategy, such as responding with the same finger on each trial.7 Indeed, we found no between-group differences in response variability or switching. In combination with the behavioural deficits described above, this suggests that Parkinson’s disease patients and controls utilized similar behavioural strategies, although the task was relatively more demanding for patients, especially at higher levels of disease severity. The parametric variation of response options additionally enabled us to show that compensatory cerebral mechanisms are recruited primarily when action selection demands increase (see later). Second, patients were asked to respond with their most affected hand, which enhances sensitivity to Parkinson’s disease-related deficits and excludes a potential difference in action selection capacity between the right and left sides.

Evidence for a compensatory role of parieto-premotor cortex

This study provides evidence that the parieto-premotor cortex may support a compensatory role in Parkinson’s disease. Conceptually, compensatory cerebral alterations involve a performance-enhancing recruitment of neural resources that maintain behaviour during relatively demanding movements despite deficits in the neural mechanisms that typically support their performance.10,11,41 This can be distinguished from the related concept of neural reserve, which manifests as a trait-like property that confers beneficial effects across clinical domains in a task-independent manner.11,35 Accordingly, motor impairments are expected to emerge when dysfunction exceeds the capacities of compensatory mechanisms that support motor performance, such as increased reliance on sensory cueing80 or goal-directed control.7 This may explain why motor symptoms differentially worsen during voluntary movements that require a selection between multiple actions.18-21

Our findings fit with a compensatory role for the parieto-premotor cortex in Parkinson’s disease, for the three following reasons. First, we demonstrate that Parkinson’s disease is associated with decreased basal ganglia activity and impaired action selection performance. This demonstrates the presence of dysfunction in one brain region, which would in turn call for compensation elsewhere in the nervous system. Second, we report stronger selection-related upregulation of parieto-premotor cortex activity in Parkinson patients with the most benign clinical subtype, consistent with the expected dependency between compensation and behavioural demand.11 Specifically, compared to healthy controls, mild-motor predominant patients showed ‘increased’ parieto-premotor activity, whereas diffuse-malignant patients showed ‘reduced’ premotor activity. The lack of between-group differences in response variability and switching further suggests that these effects were not caused by differences in the ability to perform the task. Third, we provide evidence that upregulated parieto-premotor activity is not just a by-product of inefficient cerebral processing: across the entire cohort, a stronger enhancement of parieto-premotor activity was independently associated with both lower bradykinesia severity and better cognitive performance. In combination, these findings suggest that enhanced parieto-premotor function may confer beneficial effects on motor performance by partially compensating for deficits in basal ganglia function,11,37 which fits with the recent reconceptualization of bradykinesia as a consequence of large-scale network dysfunction.2,9,81-83 Importantly, these findings remained intact even when accounting for multiple proxies of reserve (Supplementary material). Accordingly, we predict that the worsening of motor symptoms is primarily driven by decline in parieto-premotor compensation (Fig. 5A). We are currently addressing this hypothesis in an upcoming study where we extend the methodology of the present study to a longitudinal setting.

Model of the relationship between changes in brain activity and motor symptom progression. (A) Parieto-premotor activity is enhanced to compensate for basal ganglia dysfunction during the presymptomatic phase of Parkinson’s disease. Eventually, parieto-premotor compensation begins to decline, leading to the emergence of motor impairments caused by basal ganglia dysfunction. According to this model, decline in motor performance depends on loss of cortical compensation rather than progressive basal ganglia dysfunction. (B) Subtypes may be differentiated by the rate at which cortical compensation declines, the degree to which cortical resources can be recruited for compensatory purposes, or a combination of the two. DM = diffuse-malignant; IM = intermediate; MMP = mild-motor predominant.
Figure 5

Model of the relationship between changes in brain activity and motor symptom progression. (A) Parieto-premotor activity is enhanced to compensate for basal ganglia dysfunction during the presymptomatic phase of Parkinson’s disease. Eventually, parieto-premotor compensation begins to decline, leading to the emergence of motor impairments caused by basal ganglia dysfunction. According to this model, decline in motor performance depends on loss of cortical compensation rather than progressive basal ganglia dysfunction. (B) Subtypes may be differentiated by the rate at which cortical compensation declines, the degree to which cortical resources can be recruited for compensatory purposes, or a combination of the two. DM = diffuse-malignant; IM = intermediate; MMP = mild-motor predominant.

Neural mechanisms underlying focal and diffuse clinical subtypes

Subtypes of Parkinson’s disease have been linked to functional and structural alterations that may contribute to differences in clinical phenotype, symptom severity and progression.84 For example, the diffuse-malignant subtype, which is characterized by relatively severe motor and non-motor symptoms,46 is associated with increased excitability and reduced plasticity in the primary motor cortex,85 wide-spread disruptions in structural connectivity,86 and more extensive atrophy at both cortical and subcortical levels.57,86 These alterations may reflect decline in neural reserves that enable the recruitment of compensatory mechanisms during behaviourally demanding tasks.11 We add to these findings by showing that the diffuse-malignant subtype is characterized by more extensive cortical dysfunction, but not by a differential reduction in basal ganglia dysfunction. More specifically, we show that the diffuse-malignant subtype is characterized by deficits in the compensatory upregulation of parieto-premotor activity during action selection, which remains intact in the mild-motor predominant subtype. Weaker compensation in diffuse-malignant patients may result from deficits in the ability to recruit compensatory resources (Fig. 5B, left), faster decline in compensatory function (Fig. 5B, right), or both.

Variability in compensatory function likely depends on subtype-specific differences in pathological mechanisms, such as the spread of α-synucleinopathy.87 α-synucleinopathy may begin either in the CNS, spreading towards the peripheral nervous system (‘brain-first’), or the other way around (‘body-first’),88 resulting in distinct clinical phenotypes that resemble the subtypes that we utilized in this study.89 That is, a brain-first form of α-synucleinopathy (which overlaps with the clinical ‘mild motor-predominant’ subtype) has been linked to younger disease onset and motor symptoms that are confined to single effectors, likely as a result of somatotopically dependent retrograde nigral degeneration.90 The body-first form, on the other hand, has been associated with older disease onset, diffuse symptomatology involving multiple clinical domains and rapid clinical progression, which overlaps with the clinical ‘diffuse-malignant’ subtype. These phenotypic differences suggest that a brain-first type of pathology leads to focal deficits in brain function that are restricted to cortico-striatal loops whereas a body-first type leads to diffuse deficits that involve more widespread cortical networks.89 Hence, the diffuse-malignant subtype may be characterized by weaker parieto-premotor compensation as a result of diffuse cortical α-synucleinopathy, which effectively constrains the neural resources that patients with this subtype are able to recruit for compensatory purposes.

Mechanisms underlying parieto-premotor compensation

While the mechanisms that enable the parieto-premotor cortex to compensate for basal ganglia dysfunction cannot be directly inferred from this study, we suggest that they may involve non-motor territories of the basal ganglia (anterior striatum and the caudate nucleus)37,38,91 and motor control pathways that bypass the basal ganglia.92 A recent theory holds that sensorimotor regions specify and select between competing motor responses through a process of evidence accumulation towards a decision boundary.93,94 This process is dynamically influenced by urgency signals originating from the basal ganglia that determine the vigour of responses in a contextually dependent manner.95,96 In Parkinson’s disease, urgency signalling is disrupted by dopamine depletion, thereby increasing the amount of evidence that sensorimotor regions must accumulate to activate motor patterns, leading to loss of movement vigour (slowness and inability to initiate movements). However, sensorimotor regions receive additional biasing inputs that may be relied on to compensate for basal ganglia dysfunction. For example, cognitive and affective territories of the basal ganglia that are relatively spared from dopamine depletion in earlier stages of Parkinson’s disease may become increasingly involved in action selection.7,37,97 Furthermore, movements can be triggered via pathways that bypass the basal ganglia,92 which is consistent with a wealth of evidence suggesting that patients can rely on sensory input, such as visual cues,80 to compensate for movement deficits. We therefore speculate that an upregulation of parieto-premotor function in Parkinson’s disease may reflect increased processing of inputs from non-motor territories of the basal ganglia as well as other cortical regions that enable sensorimotor regions to exceed decision boundaries and select actions despite dysfunctional urgency signalling.

Hemispheric lateralization of activity may offer potential insight into the compensatory nature of selection-related parietal cortex activity.98,99 For example, lower bradykinesia severity was preferentially associated with activity in the left parietal cortex, which has been implicated in predictive motor control. Speculatively, enhanced left parietal cortex function may enable patients to more efficiently adapt movement patterns to counteract perturbations caused by underlying pathological dysfunction.99

Strengths, limitations and interpretational issues

The primary strength of this study is a large sample size combined with in-depth clinical phenotyping, which enabled us to identify sources of clinical heterogeneity and quantity relationships with brain activity derived from a brief task specifically designed to elicit compensation. However, some limitations require consideration.

We did not observe previously demonstrated differential effects of action selection on response times in patients with Parkinson’s disease compared to healthy controls.18-21 Instead, we found that Parkinson’s disease was associated with slower response times across levels of action selection demand, suggesting a general deficit in action selection performance. In contrast with previous cohorts, ours had a relatively short disease duration (maximally 5 years), raising the possibility that deficits resulting from increased action selection demand mainly arise in later stages of Parkinson’s disease, potentially as a result of decline in compensatory resources. This is partially supported by the finding that that action selection led to a greater increase in response times for intermediate patients compared to the mild-motor predominant patients. The relatively small sample size of diffuse-malignant patients may explain why we did not find the same effect in this group. The lack of a differential effect of action selection demand on task performance between Parkinson’s disease patients and healthy controls eliminates the potential confound of different behavioural strategies, thereby improving the interpretability of the between-group effects on brain activity that we observed.100 This claim is further substantiated by a lack of between-group effects on response variability and switching.

This study was primarily set up to test for compensation through group and interindividual differences in activity amplitude. However, compensation may also manifest in other forms, such as altered spatial extent of activation.101 Further research is required to test the involvement of additional aspects of compensation.

The lack medication effects on parieto-premotor function suggest that cortical compensation in Parkinson’s disease does not depend on dopaminergic state and may therefore require support from alternative therapeutic approaches.6 However, despite clear improvements in motor symptoms following dopaminergic medication, we did not observe normalization of response times102 or basal ganglia function.16 One possible explanation is that patients were not completely OFF, due to long-lasting medication effects which are now well recognized.103 The effect of levodopa on motor symptoms is mediated by a short-duration response that alleviates symptoms within hours and a long-duration response that provides symptomatic relief over days to weeks.104 The long-duration response is particularly strong in early-stage Parkinson’s disease, meaning that the clinically defined OFF-state in which participants were assessed, defined as the withdrawal of dopaminergic medication for at least 12 h, may not have allowed the effect of medication to subside enough to detect subtle alterations in task performance and brain activity.103

Parkinson’s disease is associated with wide-spread brain atrophy that may have driven interindividual differences in compensatory capacity in this study.87,105,106 Contrary to these findings, we observed no grey matter volume alterations in parieto-premotor areas where differences in selection-related activity between clinical subtypes of Parkinson’s disease and healthy controls were located (Supplementary material). Instead, differences in grey matter volume were confined to a more inferiorly located network consisting of occipital, inferior parietal and temporal areas of the cortex, which is consistent with previous findings of brain atrophy in early-to-moderate stages of Parkinson’s disease (Supplementary Fig. 7).106 We therefore conclude that compensatory alterations in parieto-premotor cortex primarily reflect functional neuroplasticity rather than alterations in underlying brain structure.

Conclusion

This study suggests that interindividual variability in Parkinson’s disease with respect to clinical subtype and symptom severity [bradykinesia and cognitive (dys)function] are determined, in part, by the degree to which parieto-premotor cortex is able to compensate for progressive basal ganglia dysfunction. Interventions and treatments that aim to modify the progression of Parkinson’s disease may benefit from focusing on enhancing the efficiency and maintenance of compensatory cortical processes in addition to restoring basal ganglia dysfunction.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available upon request only, to ensure the privacy of the participants. A data acquisition request can be sent to the corresponding author. All analysis code used in the present study is freely available at https://github.com/mejoh/Personalized-Parkinson-Project-Motor.

Acknowledgements

We would like to extend a sincere thank you to everyone who participated in the Personalized Parkinson Project. We would also like to thank Paul Gaalman and Jose Marques for their assistance with the MRI protocol that was used in this study, Jorryt Tichelaar and Marcel Zwiers for their assistance with data organization and Freek Nieuwhof for his valuable assistance during the initiation of this study.

Funding

This study was supported by The Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research (grant ID #15581). The Personalized Parkinson Project was co-funded by Verily Life Sciences LLC, the city of Nijmegen and the Province of Gelderland, Radboud University Medical Center and Radboud University. Allowance made available by Health∼Holland (Top Sector Life Sciences and Health) to stimulate public-private partnerships. The Centre of Expertise for Parkinson & Movement Disorders was supported by a centre of excellence grant of the Parkinson’s Foundation.

Competing interests

The authors report no competing interests.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain online.

References

1

Bloem
BR
,
Okun
MS
,
Klein
C
.
Parkinson’s disease
.
Lancet
.
2021
;
397
:
2284
2303
.

2

Bologna
M
,
Paparella
G
,
Fasano
A
,
Hallett
M
,
Berardelli
A
.
Evolving concepts on bradykinesia
.
Brain
.
2020
;
143
:
727
750
.

3

Wu
T
,
Hallett
M
,
Chan
P
.
Motor automaticity in Parkinson’s disease
.
Neurobiol Dis
.
2015
;
82
:
226
234
.

4

Obeso
JA
,
Stamelou
M
,
Goetz
CG
, et al.
Past, present, and future of Parkinson’s disease: A special essay on the 200th Anniversary of the Shaking Palsy
.
Mov Disord
.
2017
;
32
:
1264
1310
.

5

DeLong
MR
,
Wichmann
T
.
Circuits and circuit disorders of the basal ganglia
.
Arch Neurol
.
2007
;
64
:
20
24
.

6

Bezard
E
,
Gross
CE
,
Brotchie
JM
.
Presymptomatic compensation in Parkinson’s disease is not dopamine-mediated
.
Trends Neurosci
.
2003
;
26
:
215
221
.

7

Redgrave
P
,
Rodriguez
M
,
Smith
Y
, et al.
Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: Implications for Parkinson’s disease
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2010
;
11
:
760
772
.

8

Blesa
J
,
Trigo-Damas
I
,
Dileone
M
,
del Rey
NLG
,
Hernandez
LF
,
Obeso
JA
.
Compensatory mechanisms in Parkinson’s disease: Circuits adaptations and role in disease modification
.
Exp Neurol
.
2017
;
298
(
Pt B
):
148
161
.

9

Chen
R
,
Berardelli
A
,
Bhattacharya
A
, et al.
Clinical neurophysiology of Parkinson’s disease and parkinsonism
.
Clin Neurophysiol Pract
.
2022
;
7
:
201
227
.

10

Palop
JJ
,
Chin
J
,
Mucke
L
.
A network dysfunction perspective on neurodegenerative diseases
.
Nature
.
2006
;
443
:
768
773
.

11

Cabeza
R
,
Albert
M
,
Belleville
S
, et al.
Maintenance, reserve and compensation: The cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2018
;
19
:
701
710
.

12

Bloem
BR
,
Marks
WJ
Jr
,
Silva de Lima
AL
, et al.
The personalized Parkinson project: Examining disease progression through broad biomarkers in early Parkinson’s disease
.
BMC Neurol
.
2019
;
19
:
160
.

13

Benamer
HT
,
Oertel
WH
,
Patterson
J
, et al.
Prospective study of presynaptic dopaminergic imaging in patients with mild parkinsonism and tremor disorders: Part 1. Baseline and 3-month observations
.
Mov Disord
.
2003
;
18
:
977
984
.

14

Ravina
B
,
Marek
K
,
Eberly
S
, et al.
Dopamine transporter imaging is associated with long-term outcomes in Parkinson’s disease
.
Mov Disord
.
2012
;
27
:
1392
1397
.

15

Morrish
PK
,
Rakshi
JS
,
Bailey
DL
,
Sawle
GV
,
Brooks
DJ
.
Measuring the rate of progression and estimating the preclinical period of Parkinson’s disease with [18F]dopa PET
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
.
1998
;
64
:
314
319
.

16

Herz
DM
,
Eickhoff
SB
,
Løkkegaard
A
,
Siebner
HR
.
Functional neuroimaging of motor control in Parkinson’s disease: A meta-analysis
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2014
;
35
:
3227
3237
.

17

Herz
DM
,
Meder
D
,
Camilleri
JA
,
Eickhoff
SB
,
Siebner
HR
.
Brain motor network changes in Parkinson’s disease: Evidence from meta-analytic modeling
.
Mov Disord
.
2021
;
36
:
1180
1190
.

18

Jahanshahi
M
,
Brown
RG
,
Marsden
CD
.
The effect of withdrawal of dopaminergic medication on simple and choice reaction time and the use of advance information in Parkinson’s disease
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
.
1992
;
55
:
1168
1176
.

19

Brown
RG
,
Jahanshahi
M
,
Marsden
CD
.
Response choice in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
1993
;
116
:
869
885
.

20

Jahanshahi
M
,
Jenkins
IH
,
Brown
RG
,
Marsden
CD
,
Passingham
RE
,
Brooks
DJ
.
Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements
.
Brain
.
1995
;
118
:
913
933
.

21

Jahanshahim
M
,
Brown
RG
,
Marsden
CD
.
Simple and choice reaction time and the use of advance information for motor preparation in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
1992
;
115
(
Pt 2
):
539
564
.

22

Mink
JW
.
The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs
.
Prog Neurobiol
.
1996
;
50
:
381
425
.

23

Zach
H
,
Dirkx
MF
,
Pasman
JW
,
Bloem
BR
,
Helmich
RC
.
Cognitive stress reduces the effect of levodopa on Parkinson’s resting tremor
.
CNS Neurosci Ther
.
2017
;
23
:
209
215
.

24

Dirkx
MF
,
Zach
H
,
van Nuland
AJ
,
Bloem
BR
,
Toni
I
,
Helmich
RC
.
Cognitive load amplifies Parkinson’s tremor through excitatory network influences onto the thalamus
.
Brain
.
2020
;
143
:
1498
1511
.

25

Kordower
JH
,
Olanow
CW
,
Dodiya
HB
, et al.
Disease duration and the integrity of the nigrostriatal system in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
2013
;
136
:
2419
2431
.

26

Lee
MJ
,
Pak
K
,
Kim
HK
, et al.
Genetic factors affecting dopaminergic deterioration during the premotor stage of Parkinson disease
.
NPJ Parkinsons Dis
.
2021
;
7
:
104
.

27

Nurmi
E
,
Ruottinen
HM
,
Bergman
J
, et al.
Rate of progression in Parkinson’s disease: A 6-[18F]fluoro-L-dopa PET study
.
Mov Disord
.
2001
;
16
:
608
615
.

28

Hilker
R
,
Schweitzer
K
,
Coburger
S
, et al.
Nonlinear progression of Parkinson disease as determined by serial positron emission tomographic imaging of striatal fluorodopa F 18 activity
.
Arch Neurol
.
2005
;
62
:
378
382
.

29

Fearnley
JM
,
Lees
AJ
.
Ageing and Parkinson’s disease: Substantia nigra regional selectivity
.
Brain
.
1991
;
114
:
2283
2301
.

30

Morrish
PK
,
Sawle
GV
,
Brooks
DJ
.
An [18F]dopa–PET and clinical study of the rate of progression in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
1996
;
119
:
585
591
.

31

Marek
K
,
Innis
R
,
van Dyck
C
, et al.
[123i] β-CIT SPECT imaging assessment of the rate of Parkinson’s disease progression
.
Neurology
.
2001
;
57
:
2089
2094
.

32

Nurmi
E
,
Bergman
J
,
Eskola
O
, et al.
Progression of dopaminergic hypofunction in striatal subregions in Parkinson’s disease using [18F]CFT PET
.
Synapse
.
2003
;
48
:
109
115
.

33

Pirker
W
,
Holler
I
,
Gerschlager
W
,
Asenbaum
S
,
Zettinig
G
,
Brücke
T
.
Measuring the rate of progression of Parkinson’s disease over a 5-year period with β-CIT SPECT
.
Mov Disord
.
2003
;
18
:
1266
1272
.

34

Parkinson Study Group
.
Dopamine transporter brain imaging to assess the effects of pramipexole vs levodopa on Parkinson disease progression
.
JAMA
.
2002
;
287
:
1653
1661
.

35

Barulli
D
,
Stern
Y
.
Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: Emerging concepts in cognitive reserve
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2013
;
17
:
502
509
.

36

Poston
KL
,
Yorkwilliams
S
,
Zhang
K
, et al.
Compensatory neural mechanisms in cognitively unimpaired Parkinson disease
.
Ann Neurol
.
2016
;
79
:
448
463
.

37

Helmich
RC
,
Derikx
LC
,
Bakker
M
,
Scheeringa
R
,
Bloem
BR
,
Toni
I
.
Spatial remapping of cortico-striatal connectivity in Parkinson’s disease
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2010
;
20
:
1175
1186
.

38

Helmich
RC
,
Thaler
A
,
van Nuenen
BF
, et al.
Reorganization of corticostriatal circuits in healthy G2019S LRRK2 carriers
.
Neurology
.
2015
;
84
:
399
406
.

39

Kalia
LV
,
Lang
AE
.
Parkinson’s disease
.
Lancet
.
2015
;
386
:
896
912
.

40

Fereshtehnejad
SM
,
Yao
C
,
Pelletier
A
,
Montplaisir
JY
,
Gagnon
JF
,
Postuma
RB
.
Evolution of prodromal Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies: A prospective study
.
Brain
.
2019
;
142
:
2051
2067
.

41

Edelman
GM
,
Gally
JA
.
Degeneracy and complexity in biological systems
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2001
;
98
:
13763
13768
.

42

Wu
T
,
Hallett
M
.
The cerebellum in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
2013
;
136
:
696
709
.

43

Jankovic
J
.
Progression of Parkinson disease: Are we making progress in charting the course?
.
Arch Neurol
.
2005
;
62
:
351
352
.

44

Gregory
S
,
Long
JD
,
Klöppel
S
, et al.
Operationalizing compensation over time in neurodegenerative disease
.
Brain
.
2017
;
140
:
1158
1165
.

45

Horsager
J
,
Andersen
KB
,
Knudsen
K
, et al.
Brain-first versus body-first Parkinson’s disease: A multimodal imaging case-control study
.
Brain
.
2020
;
143
:
3077
3088
.

46

Johansson
ME
,
van Lier
NM
,
Kessels
RPC
,
Bloem
BR
,
Helmich
RC
.
Two-year clinical progression in focal and diffuse subtypes of Parkinson’s disease
.
NPJ Parkinsons Dis
.
2023
;
9
:
29
.

47

Seghier
ML
,
Price
CJ
.
Interpreting and utilising intersubject variability in brain function
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2018
;
22
:
517
530
.

48

Marek
S
,
Tervo-Clemmens
B
,
Calabro
FJ
, et al.
Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals
.
Nature
.
2022
;
603
:
654
660
.

49

Gratton
C
,
Nelson
SM
,
Gordon
EM
.
Brain-behavior correlations: Two paths toward reliability
.
Neuron
.
2022
;
110
:
1446
1449
.

50

Shadmehr
R
,
Krakauer
JW
.
A computational neuroanatomy for motor control
.
Exp Brain Res
.
2008
;
185
:
359
381
.

51

van Nuenen
BF
,
van Eimeren
T
,
van Der Vegt
JP
, et al.
Mapping preclinical compensation in Parkinson’s disease: An imaging genomics approach
.
Mov Disord
.
2009
;
24 Suppl 2
:
S703
S710
.

52

Buhmann
C
,
Binkofski
F
,
Klein
C
, et al.
Motor reorganization in asymptomatic carriers of a single mutant parkin allele: A human model for presymptomatic parkinsonism
.
Brain
.
2005
;
128
:
2281
2290
.

53

Hughes
LE
,
Barker
RA
,
Owen
AM
,
Rowe
JB
.
Parkinson’s disease and healthy aging: Independent and interacting effects on action selection
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2010
;
31
:
1886
1899
.

54

Helmich
RC
,
de Lange
FP
,
Bloem
BR
,
Toni
I
.
Cerebral compensation during motor imagery in Parkinson’s disease
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2007
;
45
:
2201
2215
.

55

van Nuenen
BF
,
Helmich
RC
,
Buenen
N
,
van de Warrenburg
BP
,
Bloem
BR
,
Toni
I
.
Compensatory activity in the extrastriate body area of Parkinson’s disease patients
.
J Neurosci
.
2012
;
32
:
9546
9553
.

56

Mitchell
T
,
Lehéricy
S
,
Chiu
SY
,
Strafella
AP
,
Stoessl
AJ
,
Vaillancourt
DE
.
Emerging neuroimaging biomarkers across disease stage in Parkinson disease: A review
.
JAMA Neurol
.
2021
;
78
:
1262
1272
.

57

Fereshtehnejad
SM
,
Zeighami
Y
,
Dagher
A
,
Postuma
RB
.
Clinical criteria for subtyping Parkinson’s disease: Biomarkers and longitudinal progression
.
Brain
.
2017
;
140
:
1959
1976
.

58

Goetz
CG
,
Tilley
BC
,
Shaftman
SR
, et al.
Movement disorder society-sponsored revision of the unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale (MDS-UPDRS): Scale presentation and clinimetric testing results
.
Mov Disord
.
2008
;
23
:
2129
2170
.

59

Petersen
SE
,
Dubis
JW
.
The mixed block/event-related design
.
Neuroimage
.
2012
;
62
:
1177
1184
.

60

Amaro
E
Jr,
Barker
GJ
.
Study design in fMRI: Basic principles
.
Brain Cogn
.
2006
;
60
:
220
232
.

61

Dale
AM
.
Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
1999
;
8
(
2–3
):
109
114
.

62

Helmich
RC
,
Aarts
E
,
de Lange
FP
,
Bloem
BR
,
Toni
I
.
Increased dependence of action selection on recent motor history in Parkinson’s disease
.
J Neurosci
.
2009
;
29
:
6105
6113
.

63

De Pablo-Fernández
E
,
Lees
AJ
,
Holton
JL
,
Warner
TT
.
Prognosis and neuropathologic correlation of clinical subtypes of Parkinson disease
.
JAMA Neurol
.
2019
;
76
:
470
479
.

64

Pal
G
,
Goetz
CG
.
Assessing bradykinesia in parkinsonian disorders
.
Front Neurol
.
2013
;
4
:
54
.

65

Zach
H
,
Dirkx
MF
,
Roth
D
,
Pasman
JW
,
Bloem
BR
,
Helmich
RC
.
Dopamine-responsive and dopamine-resistant resting tremor in Parkinson disease
.
Neurology
.
2020
;
95
:
e1461
e1470
.

66

Esteban
O
,
Markiewicz
CJ
,
Blair
RW
, et al.
fMRIPrep: A robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI
.
Nat Methods
.
2019
;
16
:
111
116
.

67

Bates
D
,
Mächler
M
,
Bolker
B
,
Walker
S
.
Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4
.
J Stat Soft
.
2015
;
67
:
1
48
.

68

Noyce
AJ
,
Bestwick
JP
,
Silveira-Moriyama
L
, et al.
Meta-analysis of early nonmotor features and risk factors for Parkinson disease
.
Ann Neurol
.
2012
;
72
:
893
901
.

69

Stern
Y
.
Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease
.
Lancet Neurol
.
2012
;
11
:
1006
1012
.

70

Mattay
VS
,
Weinberger
DR
.
Organization of the human motor system as studied by functional magnetic resonance imaging
.
Eur J Radiol
.
1999
;
30
:
105
114
.

71

Mattay
VS
,
Callicott
JH
,
Bertolino
A
, et al.
Hemispheric control of motor function: A whole brain echo planar fMRI study
.
Psychiatry Res
.
1998
;
83
:
7
22
.

72

Eklund
A
,
Nichols
TE
,
Knutsson
H
.
Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2016
;
113
:
7900
7905
.

73

Eickhoff
SB
,
Stephan
KE
,
Mohlberg
H
, et al.
A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data
.
Neuroimage
.
2005
;
25
:
1325
1335
.

74

Glasser
MF
,
Coalson
TS
,
Robinson
EC
, et al.
A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex
.
Nature
.
2016
;
536
:
171
178
.

75

Stern
Y
,
Barnes
CA
,
Grady
C
,
Jones
RN
,
Raz
N
.
Brain reserve, cognitive reserve, compensation, and maintenance: Operationalization, validity, and mechanisms of cognitive resilience
.
Neurobiol Aging
.
2019
;
83
:
124
129
.

76

Chung
SJ
,
Lee
JJ
,
Lee
PH
,
Sohn
YH
.
Emerging concepts of motor reserve in Parkinson’s disease
.
J Mov Disord
.
2020
;
13
:
171
184
.

77

Wang
J
,
Thomas
JR
,
Stelmach
GE
.
A meta-analysis on cognitive slowing in Parkinson’s disease: Are simple and choice reaction times differentially impaired?
Parkinsonism Relat Disord
.
1998
;
4
:
17
29
.

78

Agostino
R
,
Currà
A
,
Giovannelli
M
,
Modugno
N
,
Manfredi
M
,
Berardelli
A
.
Impairment of individual finger movements in Parkinson’s disease
.
Mov Disord
.
2003
;
18
:
560
565
.

79

Michely
J
,
Volz
LJ
,
Barbe
MT
, et al.
Dopaminergic modulation of motor network dynamics in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
2015
;
138
(
Pt 3
):
664
678
.

80

Rubinstein
TC
,
Giladi
N
,
Hausdorff
JM
.
The power of cueing to circumvent dopamine deficits: A review of physical therapy treatment of gait disturbances in Parkinson’s disease
.
Mov Disord
.
2002
;
17
:
1148
1160
.

81

Berardelli
A
,
Rothwell
JC
,
Thompson
PD
,
Hallett
M
.
Pathophysiology of bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
2001
;
124
:
2131
2146
.

82

Moustafa
AA
,
Chakravarthy
S
,
Phillips
JR
, et al.
Motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease: A unified framework
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2016
;
68
:
727
740
.

83

Caligiore
D
,
Helmich
RC
,
Hallett
M
, et al.
Parkinson’s disease as a system-level disorder
.
NPJ Parkinsons Dis
.
2016
;
2
:
16025
.

84

Berg
D
,
Borghammer
P
,
Fereshtehnejad
SM
, et al.
Prodromal Parkinson disease subtypes—Key to understanding heterogeneity
.
Nat Rev Neurol
.
2021
;
17
:
349
361
.

85

Belvisi
D
,
Fabbrini
A
,
De Bartolo
MI
, et al.
The pathophysiological correlates of Parkinson’s disease clinical subtypes
.
Mov Disord
.
2021
;
36
:
370
379
.

86

Abbasi
N
,
Fereshtehnejad
SM
,
Zeighami
Y
,
Larcher
KM
,
Postuma
RB
,
Dagher
A
.
Predicting severity and prognosis in Parkinson’s disease from brain microstructure and connectivity
.
NeuroImage Clin
.
2020
;
25
:
102111
.

87

Braak
H
,
Del Tredici
K
,
Rüb
U
,
de Vos
RA
,
Jansen Steur
EN
,
Braak
E
.
Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease
.
Neurobiol Aging
.
2003
;
24
:
197
211
.

88

Arotcarena
ML
,
Dovero
S
,
Prigent
A
, et al.
Bidirectional gut-to-brain and brain-to-gut propagation of synucleinopathy in non-human primates
.
Brain
.
2020
;
143
:
1462
1475
.

89

Blesa
J
,
Foffani
G
,
Dehay
B
,
Bezard
E
,
Obeso
JA
.
Motor and non-motor circuit disturbances in early Parkinson disease: Which happens first?
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2021
;
23
:
115
128
.

90

Foffani
G
,
Obeso
JA
.
A cortical pathogenic theory of Parkinson’s disease
.
Neuron
.
2018
;
99
:
1116
1128
.

91

Johansson
ME
,
Cameron
IGM
,
Van der Kolk
NM
, et al.
Aerobic exercise alters brain function and structure in Parkinson’s disease: A randomized controlled trial
.
Ann Neurol
.
2022
;
91
:
203
216
.

92

Klaus
A
,
Alves da Silva
J
,
Costa
RM
.
What, if, and when to move: Basal ganglia circuits and self-paced action initiation
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
.
2019
;
42
:
459
483
.

93

Carland
MA
,
Thura
D
,
Cisek
P
.
The urge to decide and act: Implications for brain function and dysfunction
.
Neuroscientist
.
2019
;
25
:
491
511
.

94

Thura
D
,
Cisek
P
.
The basal ganglia do not select reach targets but control the urgency of commitment
.
Neuron
.
2017
;
95
:
1160
1170.e5
.

95

Shadmehr
R
,
Huang
HJ
,
Ahmed
AA
.
A representation of effort in decision-making and motor control
.
Curr Biol
.
2016
;
26
:
1929
1934
.

96

Mazzoni
P
,
Hristova
A
,
Krakauer
JW
.
Why don’t we move faster? Parkinson’s disease, movement vigor, and implicit motivation
.
J Neurosci
.
2007
;
27
:
7105
7116
.

97

Cools
R
.
Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for L-DOPA treatment in Parkinson’s disease
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2006
;
30
:
1
23
.

98

Zapparoli
L
,
Seghezzi
S
,
Paulesu
E
.
The what, the when, and the whether of intentional action in the brain: A meta-analytical review
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2017
;
11
:
238
.

99

Mutha
PK
,
Haaland
KY
,
Sainburg
RL
.
The effects of brain lateralization on motor control and adaptation
.
J Mot Behav
.
2012
;
44
:
455
469
.

100

Price
CJ
,
Friston
KJ
.
Functional imaging studies of neuropsychological patients: Applications and limitations
.
Neurocase
.
2002
;
8
:
345
354
.

101

Palmer
SJ
,
Ng
B
,
Abugharbieh
R
,
Eigenraam
L
,
McKeown
MJ
.
Motor reserve and novel area recruitment: Amplitude and spatial characteristics of compensation in Parkinson’s disease
.
Eur J Neurosci
.
2009
;
29
:
2187
2196
.

102

Vaillancourt
DE
,
Prodoehl
J
,
Verhagen Metman
L
,
Bakay
RA
,
Corcos
DM
.
Effects of deep brain stimulation and medication on bradykinesia and muscle activation in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
2004
;
127
:
491
504
.

103

Cilia
R
,
Cereda
E
,
Akpalu
A
, et al.
Natural history of motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease and the long-duration response to levodopa
.
Brain
.
2020
;
143
:
2490
2501
.

104

Nutt
JG
,
Holford
NH
.
The response to levodopa in Parkinson’s disease: Imposing pharmacological law and order
.
Ann Neurol
.
1996
;
39
:
561
573
.

105

Filippi
M
,
Sarasso
E
,
Piramide
N
, et al.
Progressive brain atrophy and clinical evolution in Parkinson’s disease
.
NeuroImage Clin
.
2020
;
28
:
102374
.

106

Laansma
MA
,
Bright
JK
,
Al-Bachari
S
, et al.
International multicenter analysis of brain structure across clinical stages of Parkinson’s disease
.
Mov Disord
.
2021
;
36
:
2583
2594
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Supplementary data