Abstract

Visuospatial neglect is a common and debilitating condition following unilateral stroke, significantly impacting cognitive functioning and daily life. There is an urgent need for effective treatments that can provide clinically relevant and sustained benefits. In addition to traditional stroke treatment, non-invasive brain stimulation, such as transcranial alternating current stimulation, shows promise as a complementary approach to enhance stroke recovery. In the current study, we aimed to evaluate the additive effects of multi-session transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency when combined with visual scanning training in chronic stroke patients with visuospatial neglect. In this double-blind randomized controlled trial, we compared the effects of active transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency to sham (placebo) transcranial alternating current stimulation, both combined with visual scanning training. Both groups received eighteen 40-minute training sessions over a 6-week period. A total of 22 chronic visuospatial neglect patients participated in the study (active group n = 12, sham group n = 10). The median age was 61.0 years, with a median time since stroke of 36.1 months. We assessed the patients at six time-points: at baseline, after the first, ninth and eighteenth training sessions, as well as 1 week and 3 months following the completion of the combined neuromodulation intervention. The primary outcome measure was the change in performance on a visual search task, specifically the star cancellation task. Secondary outcomes included performance on a visual detection task, two line bisection tasks and three tasks evaluating visuospatial neglect in daily living. We found significantly improved visual search (primary outcome) and visual detection performance in the neglected side in the active transcranial alternating current stimulation group, compared to the sham transcranial alternating current stimulation group. We did not observe stimulation effects on line bisection performance nor in daily living. Time effects were observed on all but one outcome measures. Multi-session transcranial alternating current stimulation combined with visual scanning training may be a more effective treatment for chronic visuospatial neglect than visual scanning training alone. These findings provide valuable insights into novel strategies for stroke recovery, even long after the injury, with the aim of enhancing cognitive rehabilitation outcomes and improving the overall quality of life for individuals affected by this condition.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov; registration number: NCT05466487; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT05466487

Introduction

Visuospatial attention allows us to select and prioritize input from specific locations in space of our visual environment. In patients with visuospatial neglect (VSN), lateralized spatial attention processes are disrupted, usually due to unilateral stroke, leading to the inability to attend and respond to the contralesional side of space.1,2 While spontaneous neuronal recovery occurs in many VSN patients,3 up to 40% of patients continue to experience neglect symptoms even up to 1 year post-stroke.4 VSN is a strong predictor of poor functional recovery and significantly impairs activities of daily living (ADL).5-8 As such, adequate treatment of neglect is of utmost importance.

In recent decades, a wide range of rehabilitation methods has emerged to attenuate neglect symptoms, spanning from those that enhance awareness of neglect behavior through a top–down approach to those involving a low-level bottom–up approach with many trials and few therapeutic-guided cueing.9 Current neglect treatment guidelines primarily recommend behavioral compensation-based approaches such as visual scanning training (VST),10 aimed at improving viewing and searching behavior through top–down strategies11; yet, the supporting evidence for these methods remains limited.9,12,13 However, it should be noted that the review by Longley et al. ignores crossover design studies thereby not giving a full picture of all existing studies.12 In past years, non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques have been explored as a potential rehabilitation tool aimed at directly modulating brain network activity implicated in visuospatial processing. NIBS techniques in rehabilitation treatment are often based on the interhemispheric rivalry model proposed by Kinsbourne in 1977,14 often utilizing inhibitory stimulation protocols to reduce contralesional cortical excitability and restore interhemispheric balance in VSN patients. Yet, while promising, reported clinical effects of these NIBS interventions for VSN have remained small and heterogeneous.12,15,16

Recently, researchers have focused on NIBS techniques that utilize oscillatory-based neural entrainment, capable of modulating the intrinsic brain rhythms associated with brain network communication. Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) has gained attention for its ability to entrain or synchronize neural oscillations. This entrainment significantly enhances the coherence and power of these oscillations, thereby influencing associated network communications, cognitive processes and behavior.17,18

To comprehend the association between oscillatory frequencies and cognitive processes, neuroimaging techniques such as electroencephalography (EEG) have proven invaluable. In the realm of attention, EEG studies in healthy participants have shown that posterior oscillatory activity within the alpha range (8–12 Hz) is crucially involved in the mechanisms underlying the control of visuospatial attention.19-24 Voluntary shifts of attention towards one visual field are associated with oscillatory alpha lateralization in parieto-occipital areas. For example, shifting attention to the right hemifield is accompanied by alpha power decreases in the left hemisphere and alpha power increases in the right hemisphere. The successful tACS-induced modulation of alpha power lateralization in healthy individuals including corresponding improvements in visuospatial attention,24-27 indicate that such an entrainment-based neuromodulation approach may also represent a novel treatment approach for patients suffering from asymmetric attentional deficits like VSN. It must be mentioned here that several other previous experiments have reported no or inconsistent effects.28-30

We recently put this oscillation-based NIBS intervention to the test in subacute stroke patients suffering from VSN, and were able to reduce the spatial attention bias with tACS at alpha frequency targeting the contralesional posterior parietal cortex in a single session.31 Besides immediate stimulation effects, we were also able to show that effects were outlasting the stimulation itself, suggesting that our approach qualifies for a clinical treatment protocol aimed at achieving longer lasting and sustainable clinically relevant improvements. It is likely that long-term benefits would require a multi-session multi-day protocol, like is demonstrated in depression treatment with rTMS,32 but to this day the cumulative effects of multi-session tACS remain largely unknown as extended human trials with tACS are lacking.17

Importantly, when designing clinical protocols using neuromodulation techniques, one should consider that one of the most compelling aspects of tACS is its capacity to support neuroplasticity. As such, it induces a brain state at which the effects of other treatments are facilitated, potentially amplifying both the magnitude and duration of its benefits.17 The impact of tACS on the local neural entrainment is contingent upon the state of the brain,33,34 as brain networks tend to be more responsive when they are already in an active state. For example, when the targeted brain rhythm is already task-engaged and the frequency and phase of endogenous and exogenous oscillations align.17,35 Thus, neuronal plasticity induced by stimulation could be stronger when patients are currently active in a spatial training task. Therefore, to prime the brain for optimal learning conditions and to optimize the outcomes of the treatment, it is important that required attentional networks are activated through attention task performance (VST), executed concurrently with the application of tACS.

In the current study, we therefore combined tACS at alpha frequency with VST rehabilitation in a multi-session protocol, offered three times a week for 6 weeks (18 sessions). The overall aim was to evaluate the additive effects of multi-session (active) tACS in combination with VST, compared to multi-session sham (placebo) tACS with VST, in chronic VSN patients. Effects were measured on a cancellation task (primary outcome), a visual detection task, two line bisection tasks and on three measures assessing VSN behavior in basic ADL. Based on our previous work, we expected to achieve a synergistic effect in which tACS strengthens the efficacy of other neurobehavioral interventions, such as VST, and potentially lead to long-lasting benefits.17

Materials and methods

Study design

A double-blind randomized placebo-controlled intervention study with an allocation ratio of 1:1 was conducted. We compared the effects of active tACS to sham tACS, both combined with VST. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant by the researcher before participation. The study was approved by the Medical-Ethical Committee azM/UM of Maastricht University (NL70256.068.19/METC 19-047) and is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT05466487). More specific information of the methodology of the study can be obtained from our protocol paper published earlier.36 At the time of registration (July 2022), 13 out of 22 patients had already participated in the study.

Participants

Chronic stroke patients, as defined by stroke occurrence more than 6 months ago, with VSN were considered eligible for our study. Patients were recruited by psychologists of healthcare organizations in The Netherlands that are specialized in supporting and treating people with acquired brain injury. Recruitment of participants started in September 2020 and data collection ended in March 2023.

Inclusion criteria were: (i) neurologically objectified stroke (first or recurrent, ischaemic or intracerebral or subarachnoid haemorrhagic lesion); (ii) stroke occurred at 18–80 years of age; (iii) at least 6 months ago; (iv) sufficient ability to comprehend and communicate as assessed by a psychologist; and (v) presence of VSN as confirmed with a screening (see screening tests and associated cut-off criteria in Supplementary Table 1). Exclusion criteria were: (i) current engagement in cognitive rehabilitation treatment or other neglect treatment to avoid potential cross-contamination; (ii) physically or mentally unable to participate as assessed by a psychologist; (iii) presence of hemianopia based on clinical judgement; (iv) severe communicative disability as task descriptions need to be understood; (v) local scalp injuries; (vi) eczema on scalp or psoriasis; (vii) diagnosed (neuro)psychiatric or neurodegenerative diseases; (viii) current alcohol and/or drug abuse; and (ix) pregnancy. Excluding patients with hemianopia means that it is possible that there was a stronger focus on more restricted middle cerebral artery strokes, primarily located outside the temporal lobe (due to the presence of the optic radiations there37) and the inferior parts of the parietal lobe.

We determined the required sample size based on the results of previous studies that combined VSN treatment with NIBS in repeated sessions.38-41 Following an a priori power analysis with effect size of d = 0.80 and power of 0.80, and taking into consideration a drop-out estimated at 25%, a total sample size of 22 patients was necessary.36

Interventions

Transcranial alternating current stimulation

The experimental group received active tACS and the placebo group received sham tACS using a DC-stimulator plus (NeuroConn, Ilmenau, Germany). A small circular tACS electrode was placed onto the contralesional parietal cortex (either P3 or P4, according to the international 10–20 system), and a large ring electrode was centred around it. TACS ring electrodes were attached to the patient’s head with conductive gel (ten20 paste, Weaver and Company, Aurora, CO, USA). The gel was used to reduce the impedance between skin and electrodes to below 10 kΩ. Stimulation frequency and peak-to-peak intensity was set to 10 Hz and 1.5 mA, phase offset was set to zero and 100 cycles was used for ramping up. Figure 1 illustrates the size and position of the electrodes on the scalp as well as a current simulation for the electrode montage.

TACS electrode montage and simulation of the electric field. (A) A small circular tACS electrode (diameter: 2.1 cm, thickness: 2 mm) was placed onto the contralesional parietal cortex (either P3 or P4, according to the international 10–20 system) and a large ring electrode (outer diameter: 11 cm; inner diameter: 9 cm, thickness: 2 mm) was centred around it. (B) Simulation of the electric field in a standard head model (MNI 152 space). The software SimNIBS was used to run the simulation. Abbreviations: A, ampere; normE, norm electric field; tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation; V/m, volt per metre.
Figure 1

TACS electrode montage and simulation of the electric field. (A) A small circular tACS electrode (diameter: 2.1 cm, thickness: 2 mm) was placed onto the contralesional parietal cortex (either P3 or P4, according to the international 10–20 system) and a large ring electrode (outer diameter: 11 cm; inner diameter: 9 cm, thickness: 2 mm) was centred around it. (B) Simulation of the electric field in a standard head model (MNI 152 space). The software SimNIBS was used to run the simulation. Abbreviations: A, ampere; normE, norm electric field; tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation; V/m, volt per metre.

At the start of every first VST task in a session, the tACS device was turned on. When the VST ended, after maximally 40 minutes, the tACS was switched off. For sham tACS, we used the same electrode montage and stimulation frequency as for active tACS, but the current was immediately ramped down after the ramp up period.

Visual scanning training

In every session, patients performed the VST on a touchscreen laptop (HP EliteBook × 360 1040 G5 Notebook; screen size: 14 inch). The aim of the VST was to train patients to actively explore and consciously pay attention to stimuli on the contralesional side.10,36 The VST program comprised eight evidence-based training tasks.36 In these tasks, while predominantly employing top–down techniques relying on a voluntary effort from the patient, bottom–up elements such as exogenous cues were also integrated if the patient showed difficulty in initiating head and eye movements. Each session featured a variable combination of three to five tasks, depending on the patient’s speed and performance. Tasks were designed with multiple levels of difficulty, ensuring task difficulty aligned with the patient's level of performance, as individuals varied substantially in their capabilities and neglect severity. This allowed for individualized sessions, with different tasks to be conducted in each session, ultimately ensuring that all tasks were covered over multiple sessions.

Primary outcome measure: star cancellation task (SCT)

The SCT consisted of 52 large stars, 13 letters and 10 short words interspersed with 56 smaller stars,42 presented on a laptop screen. The patient was instructed to mark all targets (small stars) by touching the screen with the finger. The quality of search (QoS) score combines accuracy and speed in a single measure (i.e. optimal accuracy/speed search ratio; see formula in Supplementary Table 2).43 A high score reflects a combination of a high number of crossed out targets and a high cancellation speed.43

Secondary outcome measures

Computerized visual detection task (CVDT)

The CVDT measures perceptual sensitivity and attentional selection in each hemifield.24,31,44,45 The patient was asked to fixate on the fixation cross at the centre of the laptop screen. Gabor patches were presented to the left, right and bilateral sides of the screen (40 trials per location) and the patient had to indicate the location of the stimulus by pressing the <, > or ∨ key, respectively. For each of the three locations independently, the contrast of the stimuli was adaptively changed on a trial-by-trial basis. For (offline) analysis, correct hits were weighted by the contrast level (see formula in Supplementary Table 2) and performance of the CVDT was the sum of weighted hits per condition (ipsilesional/contralesional/bilateral), resulting in a score of 0 to 76.49 per condition. As we expected attention deficits in the contralesional hemifield,31 our primary focus was directed towards the analyses of the contralesional and bilateral conditions and we only briefly reported on the ipsilesional condition. In the bilateral condition, the score depends on performance in both contralesional and ipsilesional hemifields.

McIntosh line bisection task-digitized (MLBT-d)

The MLBT-d was used to measure the so-called endpoint weightings bias (EWB), a measure of lateral asymmetry.46,47 There were eight repetitions of each of four unique lines, presented in a fixed-random order on the laptop screen. The patient was instructed to mark the subjective midpoint of each line by touching the screen with the finger. The analysis then focuses on how this response position varies from trial-to-trial as a consequence of changes in the left endpoint and changes in the right endpoint (see formulas in Supplementary Table 2). The EWB value above zero indicates a greater influence of the right endpoint (over the left) and would be a sign for left-sided neglect.

Schenkenberg line bisection task (SLBT)

The SLBT consisted of 20 horizontal lines, varying from 10 to 20 cm in length, at three different positions (left, middle and right) on a landscape-oriented A4 sheet.48 The patient was asked to mark their perceived midpoint of each line. The relative deviation scores were then calculated (see formula in Supplementary Table 2) and were averaged per line position to generate the left, middle and right average scores. We analyzed only the lines positioned on the contralesional side as we expected worst performance there.

Daily living tasks

Baking tray task (BTT)

The patient was asked to distribute 16 cubes of 3.5 cm as evenly as possible over a 75 × 100 cm board (as if spreading out buns on a baking tray).49 The entire board was scanned using the Microsoft Lens iOS app. Coordinates of all cubes were manually identified using a custom Python script. An average positive x-coordinate indicates a rightward bias.

Catherine Bergego scale (CBS)

The CBS is a 10-item observation scale for measuring VSN severity in ADL, and results in a total score of zero (no neglect) to 30 (severe neglect).50,51 The CBS was filled out by the patient’s therapist or proxy (partner or caregiver), but we only considered data from forms completed by therapists, as intended. In case <50% of the items of the CBS were observed, the total score was considered not reliable and therefore a missing value.

Subjective neglect questionnaire (SNQ)

The SNQ is a 19-item questionnaire for measuring the presence of common problems associated with VSN.52 The SNQ is scored on a five-point scale according to the frequency of the occurrence of the difficulty, resulting in a score of 19 (no reported problems) to 95 (many/frequently reported problems).11 The SNQ was administered to patients and proxies, but our analysis was based exclusively on forms completed by patients, as intended. In case <50% of the items of the SNQ were filled out, the total score was considered not reliable and therefore a missing value.

Demographic and injury characteristics

Baseline descriptors were collected, including demographics (age, gender and educational level), stroke characteristics (time post-stroke onset, stroke history, stroke type and lesion side), and global cognitive functioning as measured by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA version 8.1).53

Procedure

Eligible patients who met the inclusion criteria were identified by psychologists. After informed consent was given, baseline measurements were performed. Included patients were then randomly assigned to either the experimental or placebo group and received eighteen training sessions spread over 6 weeks (i.e. three sessions per week). The training sessions (including tACS and VST) were offered by the researchers at the patients’ homes. The researchers tested the patients six times on an array of tasks: at baseline (T0), after the first (T1), ninth (T2) and eighteenth (T3) training session, as well as 1 week (T4) and 3 months (T5) after termination of the training. The SCT, CVDT, MLBT-d and SLBT were assessed during all six assessments in the study (T0–T5). The BTT, CBS and SNQ were administered at four assessments (T0, T2, T4 and T5). The SCT, CVDT and MLBT-d were presented on the same touchscreen laptop as was used for the training. PsychoPy was used to control stimulus presentation and recording of behavioral responses.

Blinding, randomization and treatment allocation

Researchers, therapists and patients were blinded to treatment allocation. We applied minimization as randomization method using MinimPy.54 Patients were stratified according to age, gender and having had previous neglect treatment. To double-blind the tACS protocols, the ‘study mode’ of the NeuroConn DC Stimulator (neuroConn GMBH) was implemented using five-digit codes that either initiated the pre-programmed active stimulation protocol or the sham protocol. There was only one unblinded research assistant who assigned a unique code to every enrolled patient and sent the code to the (blinded) researchers who then carried out all other study procedures (including the interventions and assessments). The unblinded research assistant was not further involved in the study. Blinding was removed after data analysis was finalized.

Statistical analyses

Chi-square (χ2) and non-parametric Mann–Whitney tests were used to compare demographic and stroke-related characteristics between both groups. Baseline performance on neglect outcome variables was compared with a t-test or Mann–Whitney test where appropriate, to detect differences at the start of the trial. To study any associations between the outcome variables, we conducted correlation analyses at T0 and at T5.

To test for change in the primary and secondary outcome measures both within and between groups, linear mixed model regression analysis was used, with a spatial power covariance structure to account for (time-decaying) residual covariance between repeated measures and a random intercept for patients. The predictors of interest were the effects of time and the interaction between time and group (fixed effects). We tested linear, quadratic and cubic effects of time, although conceptually the latter was not expected to be plausible. Gender, age and time since stroke were introduced as potential fixed covariates. A maximum likelihood estimation was used in the process of model selection. We started by focusing on potential removal of higher order interactions between group and time, and higher order effects of time, and finally the covariates. Terms were removed from the model if P > 0.05. The coefficients (and their tests) of the final model are reported per outcome measure, based on restricted maximum likelihood estimation. Supplementary post hoc contrasts with Bonferroni correction were performed to probe the interaction between time and group by testing differences between groups at specific time-points. As the actual time-points (in days) of measurement vary between patients, we used the mean number of days (across participants) since baseline (T0) to determine the time-points of interest.

The intention-to-treat principle was used by including all patients as randomized in the analyses, regardless of whether they received the complete program. In the context of mixed-model analysis, it is important to note there is no case-wise deletion, but all available data is incorporated. Alpha was set to 0.05. Analyses were performed in IBM SPSS Statistics version 26. Besides the correlation analyses, other analyses were preregistered.36

Results

Patient characteristics

A total of 125 VSN patients were recruited by the healthcare organizations (Fig. 2). Forty-two patients were screened for inclusion, of whom 22 were included in the study. Supplementary Table 3 depicts the patients’ performances on each screening test on the basis of which they were admitted to the study. The median age of the study sample was 61.0 years and 72.7% (n = 16) was male. Of the 22 included patients, 10 were randomly assigned to the sham group and 12 to the active tACS group (Table 1). Three patients in the active tACS group terminated participation prematurely as a result of illness (n = 2) or mismatched expectations of the program (n = 1), and two patients in the sham group did not perform assessments at T1, both due to fatigue. Figure 2 depicts the remaining number of patients included in each assessment.

Patient flow through the study. Assessments took place before the training (T0, baseline), after the first (T1), ninth (T2) and eighteenth (T3) training session, as well as 1 week (T4) and 3 months (T5) after the end of the training. aTwo patients did not perform assessments at T1, due to fatigue. Abbreviation: tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation.
Figure 2

Patient flow through the study. Assessments took place before the training (T0, baseline), after the first (T1), ninth (T2) and eighteenth (T3) training session, as well as 1 week (T4) and 3 months (T5) after the end of the training. aTwo patients did not perform assessments at T1, due to fatigue. Abbreviation: tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation.

Table 1

Baseline demographic and stroke- and neglect-related characteristics

Sham tACSActive tACSComparison
nMedian (IQR)nMedian (IQR)
Demographics
 Age, years1061.00 (12.50)1261.00 (20.50)U = 52.00, z = −0.528, P = 0.597
 Gender, % male10701275P = 1.000a
 Educational level, Verhage (0–7)106.00 (1.25)124.50 (2.00)U = 40.50, z = −1.389, P = 0.165
Stroke characteristics
 Time post-stroke onset, months1031.80 (55.82)1239.88 (116.86)U = 52.00, z = −0.528, P = 0.598
 Stroke history, % first ever10801267P = 0.646a
 Stroke type, %χ2 = 0.76, df = 2, P = 0.683
  Ischaemic10601275
  Intracerebral haemorrhage10201217
  Subarachnoid haemorrhage1020128
 Stroke side, % right1010012100N/A
 Neglect side, % left1010012100N/A
 MoCA (0–30)1023.50 (4.75)1224.00 (5.50)U = 42.50, z = −1.163, P = 0.245
Neglect characteristics
 Previous neglect treatment, % yes10801283P = 1.000a
Neglect variables at baseline
 SCT
  Misses on contralesional side of screen102.50 (19)121.50 (3)U = 49.50, z = −0.707, P = 0.479
 CVDT, weighted hits
  Contralesional condition106.50 (21.48)1110.00 (15.01)U = 53.00, z = −0.141, P = 0.887
  Bilateral condition100.50 (11.48)115.00 (10.90)U = 35.00, z = 1.437, P = 0.151
 SNQ (19–95)1034.03 (14.50)1129.86 (32.99)U = 40.00, z = −1.06, P = 0.29
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
 SCT
  QoS for contralesional side of screen100.44 (0.32)120.67 (0.47)t(20) = 1.293, P = 0.211
 MLBT-d, EWB100.25 (0.18)120.23 (0.18)t(20) = 0.34, P = 0.735
 SLBT, % deviation of contralesional lines1023.93 (20.39)1221.30 (14.67)t(20) = 0.35, P = 0.729
 BTT, mean x-coordinate100.13 (0.11)110.02 (0.08)t(19) = 2.72, P = 0.014
 CBS (0–30)710.98 (9.13)610.46 (6.94)t(11) = 0.112, P = 0.913
Sham tACSActive tACSComparison
nMedian (IQR)nMedian (IQR)
Demographics
 Age, years1061.00 (12.50)1261.00 (20.50)U = 52.00, z = −0.528, P = 0.597
 Gender, % male10701275P = 1.000a
 Educational level, Verhage (0–7)106.00 (1.25)124.50 (2.00)U = 40.50, z = −1.389, P = 0.165
Stroke characteristics
 Time post-stroke onset, months1031.80 (55.82)1239.88 (116.86)U = 52.00, z = −0.528, P = 0.598
 Stroke history, % first ever10801267P = 0.646a
 Stroke type, %χ2 = 0.76, df = 2, P = 0.683
  Ischaemic10601275
  Intracerebral haemorrhage10201217
  Subarachnoid haemorrhage1020128
 Stroke side, % right1010012100N/A
 Neglect side, % left1010012100N/A
 MoCA (0–30)1023.50 (4.75)1224.00 (5.50)U = 42.50, z = −1.163, P = 0.245
Neglect characteristics
 Previous neglect treatment, % yes10801283P = 1.000a
Neglect variables at baseline
 SCT
  Misses on contralesional side of screen102.50 (19)121.50 (3)U = 49.50, z = −0.707, P = 0.479
 CVDT, weighted hits
  Contralesional condition106.50 (21.48)1110.00 (15.01)U = 53.00, z = −0.141, P = 0.887
  Bilateral condition100.50 (11.48)115.00 (10.90)U = 35.00, z = 1.437, P = 0.151
 SNQ (19–95)1034.03 (14.50)1129.86 (32.99)U = 40.00, z = −1.06, P = 0.29
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
 SCT
  QoS for contralesional side of screen100.44 (0.32)120.67 (0.47)t(20) = 1.293, P = 0.211
 MLBT-d, EWB100.25 (0.18)120.23 (0.18)t(20) = 0.34, P = 0.735
 SLBT, % deviation of contralesional lines1023.93 (20.39)1221.30 (14.67)t(20) = 0.35, P = 0.729
 BTT, mean x-coordinate100.13 (0.11)110.02 (0.08)t(19) = 2.72, P = 0.014
 CBS (0–30)710.98 (9.13)610.46 (6.94)t(11) = 0.112, P = 0.913

aFisher’s exact test (two-tailed) is reported when assumptions of χ2 have been violated.

Abbreviations: BTT, baking tray task; CBS, Catherine Bergego scale; CVDT, computerized visual detection task; EWB, endpoint weightings bias; IQR, interquartile range; MLBT-d, McIntosh line bisection task-digitized; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; N/A, not applicable; QoS, quality of search; SCT, star cancellation task; SLBT, Schenkenberg line bisection task; SNQ, subjective neglect questionnaire.

Table 1

Baseline demographic and stroke- and neglect-related characteristics

Sham tACSActive tACSComparison
nMedian (IQR)nMedian (IQR)
Demographics
 Age, years1061.00 (12.50)1261.00 (20.50)U = 52.00, z = −0.528, P = 0.597
 Gender, % male10701275P = 1.000a
 Educational level, Verhage (0–7)106.00 (1.25)124.50 (2.00)U = 40.50, z = −1.389, P = 0.165
Stroke characteristics
 Time post-stroke onset, months1031.80 (55.82)1239.88 (116.86)U = 52.00, z = −0.528, P = 0.598
 Stroke history, % first ever10801267P = 0.646a
 Stroke type, %χ2 = 0.76, df = 2, P = 0.683
  Ischaemic10601275
  Intracerebral haemorrhage10201217
  Subarachnoid haemorrhage1020128
 Stroke side, % right1010012100N/A
 Neglect side, % left1010012100N/A
 MoCA (0–30)1023.50 (4.75)1224.00 (5.50)U = 42.50, z = −1.163, P = 0.245
Neglect characteristics
 Previous neglect treatment, % yes10801283P = 1.000a
Neglect variables at baseline
 SCT
  Misses on contralesional side of screen102.50 (19)121.50 (3)U = 49.50, z = −0.707, P = 0.479
 CVDT, weighted hits
  Contralesional condition106.50 (21.48)1110.00 (15.01)U = 53.00, z = −0.141, P = 0.887
  Bilateral condition100.50 (11.48)115.00 (10.90)U = 35.00, z = 1.437, P = 0.151
 SNQ (19–95)1034.03 (14.50)1129.86 (32.99)U = 40.00, z = −1.06, P = 0.29
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
 SCT
  QoS for contralesional side of screen100.44 (0.32)120.67 (0.47)t(20) = 1.293, P = 0.211
 MLBT-d, EWB100.25 (0.18)120.23 (0.18)t(20) = 0.34, P = 0.735
 SLBT, % deviation of contralesional lines1023.93 (20.39)1221.30 (14.67)t(20) = 0.35, P = 0.729
 BTT, mean x-coordinate100.13 (0.11)110.02 (0.08)t(19) = 2.72, P = 0.014
 CBS (0–30)710.98 (9.13)610.46 (6.94)t(11) = 0.112, P = 0.913
Sham tACSActive tACSComparison
nMedian (IQR)nMedian (IQR)
Demographics
 Age, years1061.00 (12.50)1261.00 (20.50)U = 52.00, z = −0.528, P = 0.597
 Gender, % male10701275P = 1.000a
 Educational level, Verhage (0–7)106.00 (1.25)124.50 (2.00)U = 40.50, z = −1.389, P = 0.165
Stroke characteristics
 Time post-stroke onset, months1031.80 (55.82)1239.88 (116.86)U = 52.00, z = −0.528, P = 0.598
 Stroke history, % first ever10801267P = 0.646a
 Stroke type, %χ2 = 0.76, df = 2, P = 0.683
  Ischaemic10601275
  Intracerebral haemorrhage10201217
  Subarachnoid haemorrhage1020128
 Stroke side, % right1010012100N/A
 Neglect side, % left1010012100N/A
 MoCA (0–30)1023.50 (4.75)1224.00 (5.50)U = 42.50, z = −1.163, P = 0.245
Neglect characteristics
 Previous neglect treatment, % yes10801283P = 1.000a
Neglect variables at baseline
 SCT
  Misses on contralesional side of screen102.50 (19)121.50 (3)U = 49.50, z = −0.707, P = 0.479
 CVDT, weighted hits
  Contralesional condition106.50 (21.48)1110.00 (15.01)U = 53.00, z = −0.141, P = 0.887
  Bilateral condition100.50 (11.48)115.00 (10.90)U = 35.00, z = 1.437, P = 0.151
 SNQ (19–95)1034.03 (14.50)1129.86 (32.99)U = 40.00, z = −1.06, P = 0.29
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
 SCT
  QoS for contralesional side of screen100.44 (0.32)120.67 (0.47)t(20) = 1.293, P = 0.211
 MLBT-d, EWB100.25 (0.18)120.23 (0.18)t(20) = 0.34, P = 0.735
 SLBT, % deviation of contralesional lines1023.93 (20.39)1221.30 (14.67)t(20) = 0.35, P = 0.729
 BTT, mean x-coordinate100.13 (0.11)110.02 (0.08)t(19) = 2.72, P = 0.014
 CBS (0–30)710.98 (9.13)610.46 (6.94)t(11) = 0.112, P = 0.913

aFisher’s exact test (two-tailed) is reported when assumptions of χ2 have been violated.

Abbreviations: BTT, baking tray task; CBS, Catherine Bergego scale; CVDT, computerized visual detection task; EWB, endpoint weightings bias; IQR, interquartile range; MLBT-d, McIntosh line bisection task-digitized; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; N/A, not applicable; QoS, quality of search; SCT, star cancellation task; SLBT, Schenkenberg line bisection task; SNQ, subjective neglect questionnaire.

The two groups were not significantly different with respect to demographic and stroke-related characteristics (all P values > 0.165, Table 1). Also, baseline scores on neglect outcome variables were comparable between groups, except for the BTT scores where patients in the active tACS group scored significantly lower (i.e. better performance) at baseline compared to patients in the sham group. Raw mean scores for all assessments (T0–T5) are shown in Supplementary Table 4. Results of the correlation analyses between the outcome variables are shown in Supplementary Table 5.

Primary outcome: QoS (SCT)

We derived the QoS score for the contralesional side of the screen. The final regression model (Table 2) included a linear interaction between group and time (F(1, 96) = 5.527, P = 0.021), but not a quadratic (F(1, 98) = 0.158, P = 0.692) nor a cubic (F(1, 99) = 2.756, P = 0.100) group by time interaction. The significant group by time interaction indicates that the time effect was significantly different between the active group and the sham group (Fig. 3). To conduct supplementary contrast tests of mean treatment differences at specific time-points, the mean number of days (across participants) since baseline was used to determine the time-points (in days) of interest: Day 0 (baseline), Days 4, 24, 46, 53 and 138. These contrasts showed a significant higher mean QoS at Day 138 in the active tACS group compared to sham (t(33) = 2.532, P = 0.016; P values at all other time points were ≥ 0.092). As can be seen in Fig. 3, both groups showed initial improvement in QoS performance (Days 0–53); but in the sham group, this was followed by a decline in scores (Days 53–138). Although further enhancement stagnated, overall, the active group showed significantly more improvement compared to the sham group.

Mean model-predicted QoS scores for the contralesional side of the screen. Linear mixed regression analysis including post hoc contrasts with Bonferroni correction was performed to probe the interaction between time and group by testing for a difference between active and sham tACS groups at specific time-points. Predicted scores are based on the model that includes linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms. 95% confidence intervals for the mean in the active group (dashed orange line) and the mean in the sham group (solid blue line) are included at the time-points of interest at which the active versus sham contrasts were tested. Higher scores indicate less severe neglect. Asterisks (*) depict significant difference (P < 0.05). At Day 138, the active tACS group showed a significantly higher mean QoS score compared to sham (t(36) = 2.463, P = 0.019). Please note that these test statistics deviate slightly from the test statistics as mentioned in the text, as the predicted scores shown here are based on the regression model that includes linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms, whereas in the text the comparison is based on the final regression model including only a linear group by time interaction term. Abbreviations: QoS, quality of search; SCT, star cancellation task.
Figure 3

Mean model-predicted QoS scores for the contralesional side of the screen. Linear mixed regression analysis including post hoc contrasts with Bonferroni correction was performed to probe the interaction between time and group by testing for a difference between active and sham tACS groups at specific time-points. Predicted scores are based on the model that includes linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms. 95% confidence intervals for the mean in the active group (dashed orange line) and the mean in the sham group (solid blue line) are included at the time-points of interest at which the active versus sham contrasts were tested. Higher scores indicate less severe neglect. Asterisks (*) depict significant difference (P < 0.05). At Day 138, the active tACS group showed a significantly higher mean QoS score compared to sham (t(36) = 2.463, P = 0.019). Please note that these test statistics deviate slightly from the test statistics as mentioned in the text, as the predicted scores shown here are based on the regression model that includes linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms, whereas in the text the comparison is based on the final regression model including only a linear group by time interaction term. Abbreviations: QoS, quality of search; SCT, star cancellation task.

Table 2

Final model of fixed-effect predictors and covariates for predicting primary and secondary outcomes

PredictorβaSEβ95% CI lower bound95% CI higher boundP value
QoS, contralesional side of screen (SCT) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time0.0010.0012.84E-040.0030.016
 Group−0.1610.155−0.4820.1600.310
 Group × time−0.0020.001−0.004−3.12E-040.021
Weighted hits, contralesional condition (CVDT) across T0 to T5 (n = 21)
 Time0.0320.018−0.0050.0690.088
 Group−1.3703.322−8.2575.5170.684
 Group × time−0.0740.026−0.126−0.0230.005
 Gender−9.4203.482−16.747−2.0940.015
Weighted hits, bilateral condition (CVDT) across T0 to T5 (n = 21)
 Time0.1980.0500.0980.297<0.001
 Time × time−0.0013.22E-04−0.002−3.06E-040.005
 Group−2.1304.621−11.7507.4900.650
 Group × time−0.0710.027−0.124−0.0170.010
 Gender−14.1484.977−24.581−3.7150.011
EWB (MLBT-d) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.0020.001−0.003−2.92E-040.018
 Time × time1.08E-054.51E-061.77E-061.99E-050.020
Relative deviation on contralesional lines (SLBT) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.0470.020−0.088−0.0070.023
Mean x-coordinate (BTT) at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 21)
 Group0.0780.0300.0140.1420.019
CBS at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 13)
 Time−0.1340.051−0.239−0.0300.014
 Time × time0.0013.47E-043.89E-050.0010.040
SNQ at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.2330.066−0.366−0.1010.001
 Time × time0.0014.39E-044.93E-040.0020.003
PredictorβaSEβ95% CI lower bound95% CI higher boundP value
QoS, contralesional side of screen (SCT) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time0.0010.0012.84E-040.0030.016
 Group−0.1610.155−0.4820.1600.310
 Group × time−0.0020.001−0.004−3.12E-040.021
Weighted hits, contralesional condition (CVDT) across T0 to T5 (n = 21)
 Time0.0320.018−0.0050.0690.088
 Group−1.3703.322−8.2575.5170.684
 Group × time−0.0740.026−0.126−0.0230.005
 Gender−9.4203.482−16.747−2.0940.015
Weighted hits, bilateral condition (CVDT) across T0 to T5 (n = 21)
 Time0.1980.0500.0980.297<0.001
 Time × time−0.0013.22E-04−0.002−3.06E-040.005
 Group−2.1304.621−11.7507.4900.650
 Group × time−0.0710.027−0.124−0.0170.010
 Gender−14.1484.977−24.581−3.7150.011
EWB (MLBT-d) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.0020.001−0.003−2.92E-040.018
 Time × time1.08E-054.51E-061.77E-061.99E-050.020
Relative deviation on contralesional lines (SLBT) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.0470.020−0.088−0.0070.023
Mean x-coordinate (BTT) at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 21)
 Group0.0780.0300.0140.1420.019
CBS at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 13)
 Time−0.1340.051−0.239−0.0300.014
 Time × time0.0013.47E-043.89E-050.0010.040
SNQ at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.2330.066−0.366−0.1010.001
 Time × time0.0014.39E-044.93E-040.0020.003

aβ coefficients are shown in reference to the active group.

Abbreviations: BTT, baking tray task; CBS, Catherine Bergego scale; CI, confidence interval; CVDT, computerized visual detection task; EWB, endpoint weightings bias; MLBT-d, McIntosh line bisection task-digitized; QoS, quality of search; SCT, star cancellation task; SLBT, Schenkenberg line bisection task; SNQ, subjective neglect questionnaire.

Table 2

Final model of fixed-effect predictors and covariates for predicting primary and secondary outcomes

PredictorβaSEβ95% CI lower bound95% CI higher boundP value
QoS, contralesional side of screen (SCT) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time0.0010.0012.84E-040.0030.016
 Group−0.1610.155−0.4820.1600.310
 Group × time−0.0020.001−0.004−3.12E-040.021
Weighted hits, contralesional condition (CVDT) across T0 to T5 (n = 21)
 Time0.0320.018−0.0050.0690.088
 Group−1.3703.322−8.2575.5170.684
 Group × time−0.0740.026−0.126−0.0230.005
 Gender−9.4203.482−16.747−2.0940.015
Weighted hits, bilateral condition (CVDT) across T0 to T5 (n = 21)
 Time0.1980.0500.0980.297<0.001
 Time × time−0.0013.22E-04−0.002−3.06E-040.005
 Group−2.1304.621−11.7507.4900.650
 Group × time−0.0710.027−0.124−0.0170.010
 Gender−14.1484.977−24.581−3.7150.011
EWB (MLBT-d) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.0020.001−0.003−2.92E-040.018
 Time × time1.08E-054.51E-061.77E-061.99E-050.020
Relative deviation on contralesional lines (SLBT) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.0470.020−0.088−0.0070.023
Mean x-coordinate (BTT) at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 21)
 Group0.0780.0300.0140.1420.019
CBS at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 13)
 Time−0.1340.051−0.239−0.0300.014
 Time × time0.0013.47E-043.89E-050.0010.040
SNQ at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.2330.066−0.366−0.1010.001
 Time × time0.0014.39E-044.93E-040.0020.003
PredictorβaSEβ95% CI lower bound95% CI higher boundP value
QoS, contralesional side of screen (SCT) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time0.0010.0012.84E-040.0030.016
 Group−0.1610.155−0.4820.1600.310
 Group × time−0.0020.001−0.004−3.12E-040.021
Weighted hits, contralesional condition (CVDT) across T0 to T5 (n = 21)
 Time0.0320.018−0.0050.0690.088
 Group−1.3703.322−8.2575.5170.684
 Group × time−0.0740.026−0.126−0.0230.005
 Gender−9.4203.482−16.747−2.0940.015
Weighted hits, bilateral condition (CVDT) across T0 to T5 (n = 21)
 Time0.1980.0500.0980.297<0.001
 Time × time−0.0013.22E-04−0.002−3.06E-040.005
 Group−2.1304.621−11.7507.4900.650
 Group × time−0.0710.027−0.124−0.0170.010
 Gender−14.1484.977−24.581−3.7150.011
EWB (MLBT-d) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.0020.001−0.003−2.92E-040.018
 Time × time1.08E-054.51E-061.77E-061.99E-050.020
Relative deviation on contralesional lines (SLBT) across T0 to T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.0470.020−0.088−0.0070.023
Mean x-coordinate (BTT) at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 21)
 Group0.0780.0300.0140.1420.019
CBS at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 13)
 Time−0.1340.051−0.239−0.0300.014
 Time × time0.0013.47E-043.89E-050.0010.040
SNQ at T0, T2, T4 and T5 (n = 22)
 Time−0.2330.066−0.366−0.1010.001
 Time × time0.0014.39E-044.93E-040.0020.003

aβ coefficients are shown in reference to the active group.

Abbreviations: BTT, baking tray task; CBS, Catherine Bergego scale; CI, confidence interval; CVDT, computerized visual detection task; EWB, endpoint weightings bias; MLBT-d, McIntosh line bisection task-digitized; QoS, quality of search; SCT, star cancellation task; SLBT, Schenkenberg line bisection task; SNQ, subjective neglect questionnaire.

For the sake of completeness, we conducted a second analysis for the ipsilesional side of the screen, revealing no group by time interaction (P values for linear, quadratic and cubic functions ≥ 0.655). This outcome aligns with the underlying theory of our interventional approach, which predicts that contralesional stimulation does not affect (or negatively affects) performance in the ipsilesional side compared to sham stimulation. The final model is depicted in Supplementary Table 6.

Note that Figs 3 and 4 plot model-predicted means, and not observed means, since the actual time-points (days) of measurement vary between patients (i.e. unbalanced longitudinal data).

Mean model-predicted scores of secondary outcomes. Linear mixed regression analysis including post hoc contrasts with Bonferroni correction was performed to probe the interaction between time and group by testing for a difference between active and sham tACS groups at specific time-points. Predicted scores of (A) sum of weighted hits for contralesional stimuli, (B) sum of weighted hits for bilateral stimuli, (C) EWB, (D) relative deviation of contralesional lines, (E) CBS and (F) SNQ, are based on the models that include linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms. 95% confidence intervals for the mean in the active group (dashed orange line) and the mean in the sham group (solid blue line) are included at the time-points of interest at which the active versus sham contrasts were tested. Neglect is less severe when scores are higher (CVDT), or closer to zero (EWB, SLBT), or lower (CBS, SNQ). Asterisks (*) depict significant difference (P < 0.05). The active tACS group showed a significantly higher mean visual detection performance in the contralesional condition of the CVDT (A) at Day 46 (t(25) = 2.146, P = 0.042), at Day 53 (t(27) = 2.343, P = 0.027) and at Day 138 (t(43) = 2.514, P = 0.016), as well as in the bilateral condition of the CVDT (B) at Day 138 (t(34) = 2.288, P = 0.028). Please note that test statistics presented here deviate slightly from the test statistics as mentioned in the text, as the predicted scores shown here are based on the regression model that includes linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms, whereas in the text the comparisons are based on the final regression models including only a linear group by time interaction term. Asterisks in green only apply to the regression model that includes both linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms, but do not apply to the regression model that includes only a linear group by time interaction term. Abbreviations: CBS, Catherine Bergego scale; CVDT, computerized visual detection task; EWB, endpoint weightings bias; MLBT-d, McIntosh line bisection task-digitized; SLBT, Schenkenberg line bisection task; SNQ, subjective neglect questionnaire.
Figure 4

Mean model-predicted scores of secondary outcomes. Linear mixed regression analysis including post hoc contrasts with Bonferroni correction was performed to probe the interaction between time and group by testing for a difference between active and sham tACS groups at specific time-points. Predicted scores of (A) sum of weighted hits for contralesional stimuli, (B) sum of weighted hits for bilateral stimuli, (C) EWB, (D) relative deviation of contralesional lines, (E) CBS and (F) SNQ, are based on the models that include linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms. 95% confidence intervals for the mean in the active group (dashed orange line) and the mean in the sham group (solid blue line) are included at the time-points of interest at which the active versus sham contrasts were tested. Neglect is less severe when scores are higher (CVDT), or closer to zero (EWB, SLBT), or lower (CBS, SNQ). Asterisks (*) depict significant difference (P < 0.05). The active tACS group showed a significantly higher mean visual detection performance in the contralesional condition of the CVDT (A) at Day 46 (t(25) = 2.146, P = 0.042), at Day 53 (t(27) = 2.343, P = 0.027) and at Day 138 (t(43) = 2.514, P = 0.016), as well as in the bilateral condition of the CVDT (B) at Day 138 (t(34) = 2.288, P = 0.028). Please note that test statistics presented here deviate slightly from the test statistics as mentioned in the text, as the predicted scores shown here are based on the regression model that includes linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms, whereas in the text the comparisons are based on the final regression models including only a linear group by time interaction term. Asterisks in green only apply to the regression model that includes both linear and quadratic group by time interaction terms, but do not apply to the regression model that includes only a linear group by time interaction term. Abbreviations: CBS, Catherine Bergego scale; CVDT, computerized visual detection task; EWB, endpoint weightings bias; MLBT-d, McIntosh line bisection task-digitized; SLBT, Schenkenberg line bisection task; SNQ, subjective neglect questionnaire.

Secondary outcomes

Sum of weighted hits (CVDT)

One patient of the active tACS group displayed a very high variability of weighted hits scores and was identified as statistical outlier (> 3.0*IQR from Q1 and Q3), thus the data presented here includes a total sample of 21 patients. As expected, the baseline scores were significantly lower (i.e. worse performance) of the contralesional and bilateral stimulus conditions compared to baseline scores of the ipsilesional condition (contralesional versus ipsilesional: Z = 4.02, P < 0.001; bilateral versus ipsilesional: Z = 3.980, P < 0.001).

The final model of the contralesional condition (Table 2) included a linear group by time interaction (F(1, 52) = 8.493, P = 0.005), but not a quadratic (F(1, 44) = 3.760, P = 0.059) nor a cubic (F(1, 78) = 0.913, P = 0.342) group by time interaction. Furthermore, only gender was included as a covariate (F(1, 18) = 7.319, P = 0.015). The significant group by time interaction indicates that the time effect was different between the active and sham group (Fig. 4A); there was a positive linear effect of time in the active group (P = 0.088), and a negative linear effect of time in the sham group (P = 0.020). Follow-up contrasts showed a significant higher mean visual detection performance at Day 138 in the active compared to the sham group (t(40) = 2.933, P = 0.006; P values at all other time-points were ≥ 0.108).

The final model of the bilateral condition (Table 2) included a linear group by time interaction (F(1, 68) = 6.940, P = 0.010), but not a quadratic (F(1, 60) = 0.086, P = 0.770) nor a cubic (F(1, 82) = 1.194, P = 0.278) group by time interaction. Also, there was a negative quadratic effect of time (F(1, 55) = 8.739, P = 0.005), and gender was included as a covariate (F(1, 19) = 8.082, P = 0.011). The significant group by time interaction indicates that the time effect was different between the active and sham group. There was an initial improvement in both groups (Days 0–53; Fig. 4B), but in the sham group this was followed by a decline in scores (Days 53–138). Additional contrasts showed significant better mean performance at Day 138 in the active compared to the sham group (t(32) = 2.283, P = 0.029; P values at all other time-points were ≥ 0.208).

For the sake of completeness, we conducted a final analysis for the ipsilesional condition, revealing no group by time interaction (P values for linear, quadratic and cubic functions ≥ 0.459). Again, this outcome aligns with the underlying theory of our interventional approach, which predicts that contralesional stimulation does not affect (or negatively affects) visual detection performance in the ipsilesional hemifield compared to sham stimulation. The final model is depicted in Supplementary Table 6.

EWB (MLBT-d)

The final model of the EWB (Table 2) did not include any group by time interaction (linear: F(1, 78) = 2.610, P = 0.110; quadratic: F(1, 58) = 0.314, P = 0.577; cubic: F(1, 88) = 3.354, P = 0.070). The final model included not only a linear main effect of time (F(1, 50) = 5.954, P = 0.018), but also a quadratic main effect of time (F(1, 54) = 5.744, P = 0.020). Figure 4C shows that, over the course of 6 weeks training and one-week follow-up (Days 0–53), regardless of whether tACS was involved or not, patients showed less bias towards the right endpoints of the lines; however, subsequent to that period (Days 53–138), any further improvement stagnated (active group) or even reversed (sham group).

Relative deviation (SLBT)

As expected, the baseline relative deviation was significantly higher (i.e. worse performance) of the lines positioned contralesional compared to lines either positioned in the middle or ipsilesional (contralesional versus middle: Z = 3.652, P < 0.001; contralesional versus ipsilesional: t(21) = 4.205, P < 0.001). The final model of the contralesional lines (Table 2) included no group by time interaction (linear: F(1, 82) = 1.730, P = 0.192; quadratic: F(1, 68) = 1.751, P = 0.190; cubic: F(1, 92) = 2.529, P = 0.115), and included only a linear main effect of time (F(1, 81) = 5.348, P = 0.023). This means that, over the course of time (Days 0–138), regardless of whether patients received tACS, patients showed less bias towards the ipsilesional side of the lines (Fig. 4D).

Mean x-coordinate (BTT)

One patient of the active tACS group did not understand the instructions of the BTT, and was excluded from the BTT analyses. The final model (Table 2) did not include a group by time interaction (linear: F(1, 58) = 0.487, P = 0.488; quadratic: F(1, 56) = 2.431, P = 0.125; cubic: F(1, 57) = 0.479, P = 0.492), indicating that patterns of effects over time were similar for both groups. Furthermore, no time effect was found (linear, quadratic and cubic; all P values > 0.602).

CBS

In eight out of 22 patients (36%), there was no therapist involved in the patient’s care to fill out the CBS. Of the remaining fourteen patients (64%), there were seven forms that were not reliable (i.e. < five valid items), four missing forms due to practical concerns (such as therapist on leave or employed elsewhere), and five missing forms due to dropout. This eventually led to a sample size of thirteen patients (59%; seven sham group, six active group), with a mean number of filled out forms per patient of 3.08 (SD = 1.12).

The final model for the CBS (Table 2) did not include a group by time interaction (linear: F(1, 27) = 0.063, P = 0.804; quadratic: F(1, 28) = 0.417, P = 0.524; cubic: F(1, 28) = 0.183, P = 0.672), indicating that the time effect was similar for both groups (Fig. 4E). There was, however, evidence not only for a linear main effect of time (F(1, 26) = 6.948, P = 0.014), but also a quadratic main effect of time (F(1, 26) = 4.706, P = 0.040). Over the course of 6 weeks training, regardless of whether tACS was involved or not, patients showed less VSN behavior in daily life activities (Days 0–53); however, subsequent to that period, any further enhancement in effects seemed to stagnate (Days 53–138; Fig. 4E).

SNQ

Besides the eight forms that were missing due to the three patients that terminated participation prematurely (dropouts), there was only one form that was not reliable (i.e. < 10 valid items). Thus, sample size remained at 22, with a mean number of filled out forms per patient of 3.59 (SD = 0.96).

The final model for the SNQ (Table 2) did not include a group by time interaction (linear: F(1, 55) = 0.494, P = 0.485; quadratic: F(1, 54) = 0.341, P = 0.562; cubic: F(1, 54) = 0.002, P = 0.965), indicating that the time effect was similar for both groups (Fig. 4F). As was seen for the CBS, there was evidence not only for a linear main effect of time (F(1, 53) = 12.741, P = 0.001), but also a quadratic main effect of time (F(1, 52) = 9.989, P = 0.003). Figure 4F shows that, during the training trajectory of 6 weeks, regardless of stimulation group, patients experienced less problems due to VSN in their daily lives (Days 0–53); however, this initial phase of progress was followed by a plateau in effects (Days 53–138).

Discussion

This study evaluated the additive effects of multi-session tACS at alpha frequency, combined with VST, on alleviating attention deficits in chronic stroke patients suffering from VSN. We found that patients receiving active tACS with VST showed a significantly stronger improvement in their visual search performance on the contralesional side measured with a computerized cancellation task (SCT, primary outcome measure), as compared to patients receiving sham (placebo) stimulation with VST. Additionally, our novel tACS approach resulted in significantly stronger improvements in the allocation of attention towards the contralesional side measured with a computerized visual detection paradigm (CVDT), also compared to sham stimulation. Furthermore, although no differences in performance were found between active and sham tACS on the line bisection tasks (MLBT-d and SLBT) and the measures of neglect behavior in basic ADL (BTT, CBS and SNQ), significant time-dependent improvements were observed, emphasizing the potential for recovery through rehabilitation in the later phases following a stroke.

Our findings closely parallel those of our prior single-session tACS study in subacute stroke patients.31 There too, improvements were found specific to active tACS on a cancellation task (bells task) and the same visual detection task, but not on a line bisection task. The repeatedly observed divergent effects are likely due to the varying cognitive demands of distinct tasks. For example, cancellation tasks require visual search and tap into a different type of cognitive process than line bisection tasks.55,56 Cancellation tasks may therefore better correspond with the skills and cognitive processes trained by VST, and, consequently, are more likely to capture the accurate underlying cognitive process.

Our current results are the first to show that multi-session tACS complemented with VST leads to long-term benefits of up to three months post treatment. Stimulation effects were seen in QoS on the contralesional side and in visual detection in the contralesional and bilateral conditions. The bilateral condition directly relates to the visual extinction phenomenon, a neurological syndrome closely associated with VSN. Extinction is characterized by the failure to process or attend to a contralesional event when a second competing stimulus is simultaneously presented in the ipsilesional hemifield.57,58 These results suggest that our tACS therapy enhances perception in the neglected side, also/even in the presence of distractors in the non-neglected side. Additionally, this enhancement of attention in the neglected side was not accompanied by an impairment of attention in the non-neglected side, because stimulation did not affect performance in the ipsilesional side/condition of the SCT and CVDT, as may be the case when simply reducing cortical excitability of the contralesional hemisphere using conventional NIBS approaches.59,60

No tACS effects were found on questionnaires/tasks requiring more dynamic interactions (i.e. ADL-related measures: BTT, CBS and SNQ). This may be because ADL measures do not, typically, assess the ‘efficiency’ with which daily-life activities are carried out, but merely measure the severity or frequency of occurrence of neglect-related behavior. In this sense, these measures are different from the SCT and CVDT where a time component or time restriction is included, and are less capable of detecting changes in performance efficiency, such as a better quality or effectiveness of the process to perform a daily life activity (e.g. less steps needed) or less time needed to complete an activity. Another explanation for the lack of a generalization effect could be the digitalized format of the VSN training. Possibly, training on a computer screen does not affect daily life tasks. Nonetheless, significant time-dependent improvements were observed on the CBS and SNQ, irrespective of stimulation group, suggesting that patients implemented the acquired visual scanning strategies of the VST in daily life. The digitized VST brings advantages as it is easily usable on a touchscreen and adapts to the patient’s performance ability. Also, the training program encompasses a variety of engaging tasks and provides data-driven feedback, ensuring a varied and stimulating experience to foster commitment and adherence among patients. Results of the CBS should, however, be interpreted with caution as sample size was reduced to a mere thirteen patients (59% of 22), reducing statistical power and increasing the likelihood of Type II errors.

The lack of stimulation effects on measures of ADL could have (also) been caused by the large performance variability seen in our patient sample. Patients were included based on the presence of VSN symptoms on several conventional, paper-and-pencil neglect tests (not including any measure of ADL), which, evidently, does not necessarily imply that they (also) suffered from neglect in dynamic daily living situations. Indeed, dissociations have been found between patients who displayed symptoms of VSN on conventional, static measures but not on measures of daily functioning, and vice versa.9,61,62

Overall, assessing the transfer of treatment effects to daily life remains a considerable challenge. For instance, tools used to evaluate ADL can be significantly influenced by other VSN-related issues; motor deficits, for instance, have demonstrated notable effects on measures like the Barthel index or functional independence measure.9 Additionally, both the quantity and duration of treatment sessions can influence effectiveness. There remains a gap in the literature regarding systematic exploration of the optimal combination of treatment intensity and duration necessary for effective transfer of treatment effects to daily life.9,13

Shortcomings and strengths

A limitation is the study’s response rate of 17.6% (22 included patients out of 125 eligible patients), which may raise concerns about the generalizability of findings to the broader neglect population. The low response rate may have been the result of the rather strict criteria that we use for research purposes while the clinical stroke population is much more diverse. Also, some patients were deceased or were unreachable since their discharge from rehabilitation.

A second limitation regards the novel approach for administering and analyzing the line bisection task (i.e. ‘endpoint weightings analysis’) that has recently been proposed by McIntosh et al.46,47 The EWB, representing the lateral attentional bias, has proven to be more sensitive to right-sided brain damage than the ‘classical’ bisection error, and relates more strongly to cancellation and copying measures.46 However, also on this new, more sensitive measure we did not observe tACS effects in the current study. We speculate that the means of assessment employed in this study may not have been the optimal choice; the ‘touch’ of the finger on a touchscreen laptop may have resulted in a less precise bisection mark compared to when the mark would be placed with pencil on paper (as was done in McIntosh et al.46) or even with pencil on touchscreen laptop or tablet.

While we did not explicitly assess patients’ ability to distinguish between active and sham tACS, it is important to note that tACS does not generate audible signals and somatosensory sensations during active stimulation.63 Furthermore, we included a ramp-up period in both conditions so patients could (potentially) perceive the onset of stimulation; however, in the sham condition, this was followed by a ramp-down phase after a brief interval. Blinding effectiveness has previously been demonstrated in comparable studies involving healthy volunteers, utilizing identical tACS devices and stimulation parameters.24,25,64

Several important strengths of this study are in reference to the double-blind, randomized controlled study design. All patients, researchers and therapists were blinded to treatment allocation, and the outcomes of the assessments did not affect therapists in any way. Furthermore, we used minimization as randomization method, to ensure balance across important patient characteristics that could have affected the study outcomes. Lastly, we adopted an interdisciplinary approach where brain-based NIBS is combined with behavior-based rehabilitation techniques combined with function-based and clinically relevant outcome measures, both in the short term and the long term.

Future research and clinical applications

The combined tACS-VST approach should be further tested in a rehabilitation setting, with subacute patients, and explore patterns of recovery within specific patient profiles. For instance, VSN involves different clinical subtypes that vary in frame of reference (egocentric and allocentric), sensory modality (visual, auditory, haptic and tactile) and region of space (personal, peripersonal and extrapersonal),65-67 and different clinical subtypes have been associated with different lesion sites.68,69 Evaluating at subgroup level, or even at individual level, with due consideration for distinct clinical subtypes and lesion location, will bring to light which patients are likely to benefit (most) from the treatment. Also, it is necessary to explore the most cost-effective setting for implementing the intervention. For example, although VST is traditionally offered in the clinical setting, our digitized VST program, in combination with a portable, low-cost tACS, would lend well to be used in a home-setting.70

As our VST composed of both bottom–up and top–down strategies, it remains unclear what component of the training induces the strongest neuronal plasticity when combined with tACS. Determining the most influential element, whether it is the strengthening of exogenous orienting towards external cues (associated with the ventral attention network71,72) or the enhancement of systematic learning by top–down mechanisms (linked to endogenous attention regulated by the dorsal attention network71,72), warrants further exploration.

Regarding optimization of the tACS protocol itself, significant efforts have been made towards individualized stimulation parameters (e.g. personalized stimulation montage/location, dose and waveform) using individual brain morphology (with computational head modeling) and neuroimaging (with EEG and fMRI).17 For example, we demonstrated in healthy individuals that stimulation at intrinsic individual frequencies, compared to stimulation at flanker frequencies leads to larger alpha power lateralization after stimulation.25 Personalizing tACS frequencies to individual brain rhythms could indeed improve tACS efficacy in a healthy population group,73 yet how such approach would be most effectively implemented to work in clinical populations where brain rhythms are disrupted after brain damage,74,75 is clearly less straightforward and should be addressed through forthcoming research.

Conclusion

In conclusion, multi-session tACS at alpha frequency complemented with VST, led to significantly stronger improvement in visual search performance and more enhanced perception in the neglected side in chronic stroke patients with VSN up to three months post treatment, compared to sham tACS with VST. While we did not find additive effects of stimulation on other measures (line bisection and ADL), it is noteworthy that time-dependent improvements on all but one of these measures were observed, regardless of stimulation group. Future research should focus on specific clinical neglect profiles to account for the heterogeneous nature of the neglect syndrome, and create stimulation protocols customized for VSN patient groups to allow enhanced tACS efficacy, that ultimately transfers to beneficial effects in patients’ daily living.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all patients who participated in this study and all therapists of the healthcare organizations who helped recruit patients and fill-out questionnaires throughout the study. In particular, we thank Dianne de Wilde for help with patient inclusion, and all students who helped with training and data collection.

Funding

No funding was received towards this work.

Competing interests

The authors report no competing interests.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request. The codes that were used for statistical analyses are provided within the supplementary material (Supplementary Table 7).

References

1

Buxbaum
 
L
,
Ferraro
 
M
,
Veramonti
 
T
, et al.  
Hemispatial neglect: Subtypes, neuroanatomy, and disability
.
Neurology
.
2004
;
62
:
749
756
.

2

Heilman
 
KM
,
Valenstein
 
E
,
Watson
 
RT
.
Neglect and related disorders
.
Semin Neurol
.
2000
;
20
(
4
):
463
470
.

3

Nijboer
 
TCW
,
Winters
 
C
,
Kollen
 
BJ
,
Kwakkel
 
G
.
Impact of clinical severity of stroke on the severity and recovery of visuospatial neglect
.
PLoS One
.
2018
;
13
(
7
):
e0198755
.

4

Nijboer
 
TCW
,
Van de Port
 
I
,
Schepers
 
V
,
Post
 
M
,
Visser-Meily
 
A
.
Predicting functional outcome after stroke: The influence of neglect on basic activities in daily living
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2013
;
7
:
182
.

5

Di Monaco
 
M
,
Schintu
 
S
,
Dotta
 
M
,
Barba
 
S
,
Tappero
 
R
,
Gindri
 
P
.
Severity of unilateral spatial neglect is an independent predictor of functional outcome after acute inpatient rehabilitation in individuals with right hemispheric stroke
.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
.
2011
;
92
(
8
):
1250
1256
.

6

Stone
 
S
,
Patel
 
P
,
Greenwood
 
R
,
Halligan
 
P
.
Measuring visual neglect in acute stroke and predicting its recovery: The visual neglect recovery index
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
.
1992
;
55
(
6
):
431
436
.

7

Bosma
 
MS
,
Caljouw
 
MAA
,
Achterberg
 
WP
,
Nijboer
 
TCW
.
Prevalence, severity and impact of visuospatial neglect in geriatric stroke rehabilitation: A cross-sectional study
.
J Am Med Dir Assoc
.
2023
;
24
:
1798
1805
.

8

Embrechts
 
E
,
Van Criekinge
 
T
,
Schröder
 
J
, et al.  
The association between visuospatial neglect and balance and mobility post-stroke onset: A systematic review
.
Ann Phys Rehabil Med
.
2021
;
64
(
4
):
101449
.

9

Azouvi
 
P
,
Jacquin-Courtois
 
S
,
Luauté
 
J
.
Rehabilitation of unilateral neglect: Evidence-based medicine
.
Ann Phys Rehabil Med
.
2017
;
60
(
3
):
191
197
.

10

Pizzamiglio
 
L
,
Antonucci
 
G
,
Judica
 
A
,
Montenero
 
P
,
Razzano
 
C
,
Zoccolotti
 
P
.
Cognitive rehabilitation of the hemineglect disorder in chronic patients with unilateral right brain damage
.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
.
1992
;
14
(
6
):
901
923
.

11

Van Kessel
 
ME
,
Geurts
 
ACH
,
Brouwer
 
WH
,
Fasotti
 
L
.
Visual scanning training for neglect after stroke with and without a computerized lane tracking dual task
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2013
;
7
:
358
.

12

Longley
 
V
,
Hazelton
 
C
,
Heal
 
C
, et al.  
Non-pharmacological interventions for spatial neglect or inattention following stroke and other non-progressive brain injury
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev.
 
2021
;
7
:
CD003586
.

13

Kerkhoff
 
G
,
Schenk
 
T
.
Rehabilitation of neglect: An update
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2012
;
50
(
6
):
1072
1079
.

14

Kinsbourne
 
M
.
Hemi-neglect and hemisphere rivalry
.
Adv Neurol
.
1977
;
18
:
41
49
.

15

Lefaucheur
 
JP
,
Aleman
 
A
,
Baeken
 
C
, et al.  
Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): An update (2014–2018)
.
Clin Neurophysiol
.
2020
;
131
(
2
):
474
528
.

16

Elsner
 
B
,
Kugler
 
J
,
Pohl
 
M
,
Mehrholz
 
J
.
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for improving activities of daily living, and physical and cognitive functioning, in people after stroke
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev.
 
2020
;
11
(
11
):
CD009645
.

17

Wischnewski
 
M
,
Alekseichuk
 
I
,
Opitz
 
A
.
Neurocognitive, physiological, and biophysical effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2023
;
27
(
2
):
189
205
.

18

Lakatos
 
P
,
Gross
 
J
,
Thut
 
G
.
A new unifying account of the roles of neuronal entrainment
.
Curr Biol.
 
2019
;
29
:
R890
R905
.

19

Gould
 
IC
,
Rushworth
 
MF
,
Nobre
 
AC
.
Indexing the graded allocation of visuospatial attention using anticipatory alpha oscillations
.
J Neurophysiol
.
2011
;
105
(
3
):
1318
1326
.

20

Händel
 
BF
,
Haarmeier
 
T
,
Jensen
 
O
.
Alpha oscillations correlate with the successful inhibition of unattended stimuli
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2011
;
23
(
9
):
2494
2502
.

21

Thut
 
G
,
Nietzel
 
A
,
Brandt
 
SA
,
Pascual-Leone
 
A
.
α-Band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection
.
J Neurosci
.
2006
;
26
(
37
):
9494
9502
.

22

Sauseng
 
P
,
Klimesch
 
W
,
Stadler
 
W
, et al.  
A shift of visual spatial attention is selectively associated with human EEG alpha activity
.
Eur J Neurosci
.
2005
;
22
(
11
):
2917
2926
.

23

Worden
 
MS
,
Foxe
 
JJ
,
Wang
 
N
,
Simpson
 
GV
.
Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex
.
J Neurosci.
 
2000
;
20
(
6
):
RC63
.

24

Schuhmann
 
T
,
Kemmerer
 
SK
,
Duecker
 
F
, et al.  
Left parietal tACS at alpha frequency induces a shift of visuospatial attention
.
PLoS One
.
2019
;
14
(
11
):
e0217729
.

25

Kemmerer
 
SK
,
Sack
 
AT
,
De Graaf
 
TA
,
Ten Oever
 
S
,
De Weerd
 
P
,
Schuhmann
 
T
.
Frequency-specific transcranial neuromodulation of alpha power alters visuospatial attention performance
.
Brain Res
.
2022
;
1782
:
147834
.

26

Kasten
 
FH
,
Wendeln
 
T
,
Stecher
 
HI
,
Herrmann
 
CS
.
Hemisphere-specific, differential effects of lateralized, occipital–parietal α- versus γ-tACS on endogenous but not exogenous visual-spatial attention
.
Sci Rep
.
2020
;
10
:
12270
.

27

Radecke
 
JO
,
Fiene
 
M
,
Misselhorn
 
J
, et al.  
Personalized alpha-tACS targeting left posterior parietal cortex modulates visuo-spatial attention and posterior evoked EEG activity
.
Brain Stimul
.
2023
;
16
(
4
):
1047
1061
.

28

Veniero
 
D
,
Benwell
 
CSY
,
Ahrens
 
MM
,
Thut
 
G
.
Inconsistent effects of parietal α-tACS on pseudoneglect across two experiments: A failed internal replication
.
Front Psychol
.
2017
;
8
:
952
.

29

Van Schouwenburg
 
MR
,
Sörensen
 
LKA
,
De Klerk
 
R
,
Reteig
 
LC
,
Slagter
 
HA
.
No differential effects of two different alpha-band electrical stimulation protocols over fronto-parietal regions on spatial attention
.
Front Neurosci
.
2018
;
12
:
433
.

30

Coldea
 
A
,
Morand
 
S
,
Veniero
 
D
,
Harvey
 
M
,
Thut
 
G
.
Parietal alpha tACS shows inconsistent effects on visuospatial attention
.
PLoS One
.
2021
;
16
(
8
):
e0255424
.

31

Schuhmann
 
T
,
Duecker
 
F
,
Middag-van Spanje
 
M
, et al.  
Transcranial alternating brain stimulation at alpha frequency reduces hemispatial neglect symptoms in stroke patients
.
Int J Clin Health Psychol.
 
2022
;
22
(
3
):
100326
.

32

Perera
 
T
,
George
 
MS
,
Grammer
 
G
,
Janicak
 
PG
,
Pascual-Leone
 
A
,
Wirecki
 
TS
.
The clinical TMS society consensus review and treatment recommendations for TMS therapy for major depressive disorder
.
Brain Stimul
.
2016
;
9
(
3
):
336
346
.

33

Kasten
 
FH
,
Herrmann
 
CS
.
The hidden brain-state dynamics of tACS aftereffects
.
Neuroimage
.
2022
;
264
:
119713
.

34

Neuling
 
T
,
Rach
 
S
,
Herrmann
 
CS
.
Orchestrating neuronal networks: Sustained after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation depend upon brain states
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2013
;
7
:
161
.

35

Nguyen
 
J
,
Deng
 
Y
,
Reinhart
 
RMG
.
Brain-state determines learning improvements after transcranial alternating-current stimulation to frontal cortex
.
Brain Stimul
.
2018
;
11
(
4
):
723
726
.

36

Middag-van Spanje
 
M
,
Schuhmann
 
T
,
Nijboer
 
T
,
Van der Werf
 
O
,
Sack
 
AT
,
Van Heugten
 
C
.
Study protocol of transcranial electrical stimulation at alpha frequency applied during rehabilitation: A randomized controlled trial in chronic stroke patients with visuospatial neglect
.
BMC Neurol
.
2022
;
22
(
1
):
402
.

37

Rodrigues
 
EM
,
Isolan
 
GR
,
Becker
 
LG
,
Dini
 
LI
,
Vaz
 
MAS
,
Frigeri
 
TM
.
Anatomy of the optic radiations from the white matter fiber dissection perspective: A literature review applied to practical anatomical dissection
.
Surg Neurol Int
.
2022
;
13
:
309
.

38

Kim
 
BR
,
Chun
 
MH
,
Kim
 
DY
,
Lee
 
SJ
.
Effect of high- and low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on visuospatial neglect in patients with acute stroke: A double-blind, sham-controlled trial
.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
.
2013
;
94
(
5
):
803
807
.

39

Yang
 
W
,
Liu
 
TT
,
Bin Song
 
X
, et al.  
Comparison of different stimulation parameters of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for unilateral spatial neglect in stroke patients
.
J Neurol Sci
.
2015
;
359
(
1-2
):
219
225
.

40

Fu
 
W
,
Song
 
W
,
Zhang
 
Y
, et al.  
Long-term effects of continuous theta-burst stimulation in visuospatial neglect
.
J Int Med Res.
 
2015
;
43
(
2
):
196
203
.

41

Bang
 
DH
,
Bong
 
SY
.
Effect of combination of transcranial direct current stimulation and feedback training on visuospatial neglect in patients with subacute stroke: A pilot randomized controlled trial
.
J Phys Ther Sci
.
2015
;
27
(
9
):
2759
2761
.

42

Wilson
 
B
,
Cockburn
 
J
,
Halligan
 
PW
.
Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect
.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
.
1987
;
68
(
2
):
98
102
.

43

Dalmaijer
 
ES
,
Van der Stigchel
 
S
,
Nijboer
 
TCW
,
Cornelissen
 
THW
,
Husain
 
M
.
CancellationTools: All-in-one software for administration and analysis of cancellation tasks
.
Behav Res Methods
.
2015
;
47
(
4
):
1065
1075
.

44

Duecker
 
F
,
Schuhmann
 
T
,
Bien
 
N
,
Jacobs
 
C
,
Sack
 
AT
.
Moving beyond attentional biases: Shifting the interhemispheric balance between left and right posterior parietal cortex modulates attentional control processes
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2017
;
29
(
7
):
1267
1278
.

45

Bien
 
N
,
Goebel
 
R
,
Sack
 
AT
.
Extinguishing extinction: Hemispheric differences in the modulation of TMS-induced visual extinction by directing covert spatial attention
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2012
;
24
(
4
):
809
818
.

46

McIntosh
 
RD
,
Ietswaart
 
M
,
Milner
 
AD
.
Weight and see: Line bisection in neglect reliably measures the allocation of attention, but not the perception of length
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2017
;
106
:
146
158
.

47

McIntosh
 
RD
,
Schindler
 
I
,
Birchall
 
D
,
Milner
 
AD
.
Weights and measures: A new look at bisection behaviour in neglect
.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res.
 
2005
;
25
(
3
):
833
850
.

48

Schenkenberg
 
T
,
Bradford
 
D
,
Ajax
 
E
.
Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment
.
Neurology
.
1980
;
30
(
5
):
509
517
.

49

Tham
 
K
.
The baking tray task: A test of spatial neglect
.
Neuropsychol Rehabil
.
1996
;
6
(
1
):
19
26
.

50

Azouvi
 
P
,
Olivier
 
S
,
De Montety
 
G
,
Samuel
 
C
,
Louis-Dreyfus
 
A
,
Tesio
 
L
.
Behavioral assessment of unilateral neglect: Study of the psychometric properties of the Catherine Bergego scale
.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
.
2003
;
84
:
51
57
.

51

Ten Brink
 
A
,
Nijboer
 
T
,
Van Beekum
 
L
, et al.  
De Nederlandse Catherine Bergego schaal: Een bruikbaar en valide instrument in de CVA-zorg
.
Wetenschappelijk Tijdschrift voor Ergotherapie
.
2013
;
6
(
3
):
27
36
.

52

Towle
 
D
,
Lincoln
 
NB
.
Development of a questionnaire for detecting everyday problems in stroke patients with unilateral visual neglect
.
Clin Rehabil
.
1991
;
5
:
135
140
.

53

Nasreddine
 
ZS
,
Phillips
 
NA
,
Bédirian
 
V
, et al.  
The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2005
;
53
:
695
699
.

54

Saghaei
 
M
,
Saghaei
 
S
.
Implementation of an open-source customizable minimization program for allocation of patients to parallel groups in clinical trials
.
J Biomed Sci Eng
.
2011
;
4
(
11
):
734
739
.

55

Ferber
 
S
,
Karnath
 
HO
.
How to assess spatial neglect—Line bisection or cancellation tasks?
 
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
.
2001
;
23
(
5
):
599
607
.

56

Van der Stigchel
 
S
,
Nijboer
 
TCW
.
Temporal order judgements as a sensitive measure of the spatial bias in patients with visuospatial neglect
.
J Neuropsychol
.
2017
;
12
(
3
):
427
441
.

57

Vossel
 
S
,
Eschenbeck
 
P
,
Weiss
 
PH
, et al.  
Visual extinction in relation to visuospatial neglect after right-hemispheric stroke: Quantitative assessment and statistical lesion-symptom mapping
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
.
2011
;
82
(
8
):
862
868
.

58

Riddoch
 
MJ
,
Rappaport
 
SJ
,
Humphreys
 
GW
.
Extinction: A window into attentional competition
.
Prog Brain Res
.
2009
;
176
:
149
159
.

59

Dambeck
 
N
,
Sparing
 
R
,
Meister
 
IG
, et al.  
Interhemispheric imbalance during visuospatial attention investigated by unilateral and bilateral TMS over human parietal cortices
.
Brain Res
.
2006
;
1072
(
1
):
194
199
.

60

Hilgetag
 
CC
,
Théoret
 
H
,
Pascual-Leone
 
A
.
Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced ‘virtual lesions’ of human parietal cortex
.
Nat Neurosci
.
2001
;
4
(
9
):
953
957
.

61

Huisman
 
K
,
Visser-Meily
 
A
,
Eijsackers
 
A
,
Nijboer
 
T
.
Hoe kan de diagnostiek van visueel neglect verbeterd worden?
 
Tijdschrift Voor Neuropsychologie
.
2013
;
8
(
3
):
134
140
.

62

Spreij
 
LA
,
Ten Brink
 
AF
,
Visser-Meily
 
JMA
,
Nijboer
 
TCW
.
Increasing cognitive demand in assessments of visuo-spatial neglect: Testing the concepts of static and dynamic tests
.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
.
2020
;
42
(
7
):
675
689
.

63

Herrmann
 
CS
,
Rach
 
S
,
Neuling
 
T
,
Strüber
 
D
.
Transcranial alternating current stimulation: A review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive processes
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2013
;
7
:
279
.

64

Kemmerer
 
SK
,
De Graaf
 
TA
,
Ten Oever
 
S
,
Erkens
 
M
,
De Weerd
 
P
,
Sack
 
AT
.
Parietal but not temporoparietal alpha-tACS modulates endogenous visuospatial attention
.
Cortex
.
2022
;
154
:
149
166
.

65

Corbetta
 
M
.
Hemispatial neglect: Clinic, pathogenesis, and treatment
.
Semin Neurol
.
2014
;
34
(
5
):
514
523
.

66

Rode
 
G
,
Pagliari
 
C
,
Huchon
 
L
,
Rossetti
 
Y
,
Pisella
 
L
.
Semiology of neglect: An update
.
Ann Phys Rehabil Med
.
2017
;
60
(
3
):
177
185
.

67

Van der Stoep
 
N
,
Visser-Meily
 
JMA
,
Kappelle
 
LJ
, et al.  
Exploring near and far regions of space: Distance-specific visuospatial neglect after stroke
.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
.
2013
;
35
(
8
):
799
811
.

68

Molenberghs
 
P
,
Sale
 
MV
,
Mattingley
 
JB
.
Is there a critical lesion site for unilateral spatial neglect? A meta-analysis using activation likelihood estimation
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2012
;
6
:
78

69

Karnath
 
HO
,
Rorden
 
C
.
The anatomy of spatial neglect
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2012
;
50
(
6
):
1010
1017
.

70

Perera
 
MPN
,
Bailey
 
NW
,
Murphy
 
OW
, et al.  
Home-based individualized alpha transcranial alternating current stimulation improves symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder: Preliminary evidence from a randomized, sham-controlled clinical trial
.
Depress Anxiety
.
2023
;
2023
:
1
11
.

71

Corbetta
 
M
,
Shulman
 
GL
.
Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2002
;
3
(
3
):
201
215
.

72

Corbetta
 
M
,
Shulman
 
GL
.
Spatial neglect and attention networks
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
.
2011
;
34
:
569
599
.

73

Zaehle
 
T
,
Rach
 
S
,
Herrmann
 
CS
.
Transcranial alternating current stimulation enhances individual alpha activity in human EEG
.
PLoS One
.
2010
;
5
(
11
):
e13766
.

74

Lasaponara
 
S
,
Pinto
 
M
,
Aiello
 
M
,
Tomaiuolo
 
F
,
Doricchi
 
F
.
The hemispheric distribution of a-band EEG activity during orienting of attention in patients with reduced awareness of the left side of space (spatial neglect)
.
J Neurosci
.
2019
;
39
(
22
):
4332
4343
.

75

Lasaponara
 
S
,
D’Onofrio
 
M
,
Pinto
 
M
, et al.  
EEG correlates of preparatory orienting, contextual updating, and inhibition of sensory processing in left spatial neglect
.
J Neurosci
.
2018
;
38
(
15
):
3792
3808
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Supplementary data