-
PDF
- Split View
-
Views
-
Cite
Cite
Neil Michael Dundon, Mohammad Zia Ul Haq Katshu, Bronson Harry, Daniel Roberts, E Charles Leek, Paul Downing, Ayelet Sapir, Craig Roberts, Giovanni d’Avossa, Human Parahippocampal Cortex Supports Spatial Binding in Visual Working Memory, Cerebral Cortex, Volume 28, Issue 10, October 2018, Pages 3589–3599, https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx231
- Share Icon Share
Abstract
Studies investigating the functional organization of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) suggest that parahippocampal cortex (PHC) generates representations of spatial and contextual information used by the hippocampus in the formation of episodic memories. However, evidence from animal studies also implicates PHC in spatial binding of visual information held in short term, working memory. Here we examined a 46-year-old man (P.J.), after he had recovered from bilateral medial occipitotemporal cortex strokes resulting in ischemic lesions of PHC and hippocampal atrophy, and a group of age-matched healthy controls. When recalling the color of 1 of 2 objects, P.J. misidentified the target when cued by its location, but not shape. When recalling the position of 1 of 3 objects, he frequently misidentified the target, which was cued by its color. Increasing the duration of the memory delay had no impact on the proportion of binding errors, but did significantly worsen recall precision in both P.J. and controls. We conclude that PHC may play a crucial role in spatial binding during encoding of visual information in working memory.
Introduction
The medial temporal lobe (MTL) comprises the hippocampus and parahippocampal regions, that is, entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex (PRC), and parahippocampal cortex (PHC). These structures play a prominent role in episodic memory, as evidenced by the dense anterograde amnesia, which follows damage to MTL (Scoville and Milner 1957; Corkin 1984; Corkin et al. 1997). Modular accounts of MTL function have suggested that the hippocampus synthesizes episodic memories by binding information about the identity and location of objects carried respectively by 2 different streams (Diana et al. 2007; Eichenbaum et al. 2007).
MTL structures have also been implicated in short-term memory processes (Ranganath and Blumenfeld 2005; Graham et al. 2010; Yonelinas 2013). First, animal models have pointed to specific molecular mechanisms in the mammalian MTL dedicated to the storage of short-term memories, and separate from those involved in long term memory (Deacon et al. 2002; Reisel et al. 2002). Single unit recordings and lesion studies in nonhuman primates have further demonstrated that the hippocampus (Friedman and Goldman-Rakic 1988), entorhinal cortex (Suzuki et al. 1997), PRC (Davachi and Goldman-Rakic 2001), and PHC (Bachevalier and Nemanic 2008) contribute to the encoding and recall of information from short-term memory. These animal findings complement neuropsychological studies of patients with amnesia resulting from Korsakoff’s syndrome, encephalitis and colloid cysts (Holdstock et al. 1995), and patients with surgical (Aggleton 1992; Owen et al. 1995) or ischemic (Holdstock et al. 2002) lesions to the MTL, demonstrating retention deficits for novel stimuli over delay intervals as short as 2 s (Ranganath and Blumenfeld 2005).
An increasing body of evidence further suggests that short-term memory exploits the same MTL modules as episodic memory; that is, PRC codes information about an object’s identity and PHC codes an object’s location and its context, and these 2 streams are bound in the hippocampus (Pertzov et al. 2013; Watson et al. 2013; Libby et al. 2014; Yee et al. 2014). Consistent with the idea that in short-term memory identity and location information are processed separately and then bound, patients with hippocampal damage can exhibit deficits recalling object-location conjunctions after 1.0 s delays, even when unimpaired recalling either object identities or locations (Olson et al. 2006a; 2006b). However, other studies report that patients with damage to the hippocampus do not necessarily show deficits in recalling object-location conjunctions, suggesting that spatial binding is preserved (Jeneson et al. 2010; see Yonelinas 2013 for a review).
An alternative possibility is that spatial binding in short-term memory occurs in parahippocampal regions, rather than the hippocampus proper. In support of this view, data in both rats (Burwell and Amaral 1998) and monkeys (Suzuki and Amaral 1994) indicate that PRC and PHC are reciprocally connected, suggesting that the parcellation of identity and spatial information is not absolute, and that there may already be substantial cross-talk between object and spatial/context related information in parahippocampal regions. Further, recordings in rats have demonstrated single unit responses for object-location conjunctions in the PHC homologue (Barker and Warburton 2011).
Behavioral studies in monkeys have provided crucial evidence for the role of PHC in spatial binding. Rhesus monkeys with PHC lesions are impaired in both simple location and object-location conjunction tasks (Malkova and Mishkin 2003). This short-term memory impairment was observed in a delayed match-to-sample task, where the sample contained 2 nonidentical objects. After a 6 s delay, the test array contained one of the objects in its original location (the target), and an identical item either at the location of the sample foil (object-place condition), or at a novel location not previously occupied by either sample object (location condition). Monkeys with PHC lesions were impaired identifying the target in both conditions, while monkeys with lesions in the hippocampus showed no impairment in either task (Malkova and Mishkin 2003). Hippocampectomized monkeys were likewise unimpaired in a later study, using a more difficult task with an increased number of objects and locations (Belcher et al. 2006).
A cross-species homology in the short-term memory functionality of PHC is partly supported by the observation that patients with PHC lesions also exhibit a decrement in spatial recall (Ploner et al. 2000), although this impairment is only observed using delays greater (i.e., >15.0 s) than those used by Malkova and Mishkin (2003). In addition, functional imaging data in healthy subjects demonstrate heightened right PHC activation during both encoding and maintenance of object-location conjunctions, relative to trials where objects or locations are memorized separately (Luck et al. 2010). However, no neuropsychological study has so far demonstrated that PHC contributes to spatial binding in human short-term memory.
In the present study, we examined the nature and extent of spatial and short-term memory deficits associated with focal PHC lesions, by testing a middle-aged man (P.J.) with bilateral posterior circulation strokes involving the PHC, but sparing the hippocampus and PRC. Our experiments were driven by 3 specific research questions: (1) does damage to PHC produce binding difficulties and if so, are the binding problems specifically spatial or do they generalize to other visual dimensions; (2) do binding impairments reflect deficits in memory encoding or maintenance; and (3) is the binding impairment secondary to a loss of positional information either in memory or perception?
Both P.J. and controls showed delay-dependent decrements in the precision of spatial recall, however, P.J.’s recall precision was significantly worse than the controls at longer delays (5.0 s). P.J. also showed impaired spatial binding. This impairment was unaffected by the duration of the memory delay. Finally, P.J.’s binding deficits did not generalize across visual dimensions, since he performed normally when recall involved the conjunction of nonspatial features. We conclude that PHC serves a spatially specific binding function in short-term memory, and that this function appears to be independent of PHC’s role in recall precision.
Methods
P.J.: History and Clinical Assessment
P.J. was first seen by one of the authors (C.R.), 4 months after he had suffered a cerebrovascular accident. P.J. was 45 years old when he developed headaches, visual and mental status changes over the course of a few hours. Two days after the onset of these symptoms, he was admitted to a stroke-unit at a regional hospital. During the admission, he continued to be confused and agitated. The diagnostic work-up revealed bilateral posterior circulation strokes involving the occipitotemporal cortex. No cause for the stroke was identified. P.J. had no significant medical history, except for cluster headaches, which responded well to standard treatment.
Upon returning home, he was not able to resume his full-time occupation as an animal breeder, because of difficulties finding his way around the house and farm, where he had moved 2 years prior. He also relinquished driving, because he could not find his way around familiar streets. He was able to sketch the overall layout of his home, but frequently misidentified rooms and the family resorted to placing signs on internal doors to help him find his way around. His ability to repair equipment around the farm was also diminished, because of difficulty identifying the correct tool in a cluttered environment.
P.J.’s visual perimetry was formally assessed 3 and 5 months following the ischemic injury, with a binocular field test (Esterman 1982). He showed strict upper quadrantanopias, worse on the left than on the right. There was evidence of partial recovery on the second assessment (Fig. S3).
Formal clinical psychometric testing was conducted approximately 6 months following his stroke. The standardized scores are presented in Supplementary Table 1. His general intellectual functioning fell within the average range, as measured with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence scale, fourth edition (WAIS-IV). This was affected negatively by slowed processing speed on visual tasks. He performed similarly on the verbal (Verbal Comprehension Index) and nonverbal scale (Perceptual Reasoning Index) of the WAIS-IV. His expressive and receptive language functions were grossly intact. He did however often require verbal instructions to be repeated. His information-processing speed was in the borderline range on the WAIS-IV. Memory function was significantly impaired for both visual and verbal material. He had difficulties with learning and acquisition of new material and also with delayed recall. Performance was not improved for recognition memory. His errors on a visual memory task were primarily misplacement errors. He demonstrated set-loss errors on a word generation task and also required reminding of rules on a problem-solving task. Performance on executive functioning tasks was mixed; he performed at the expected level on a planning and problem-solving task. His performance on a verbal fluency task was within normal limits. His score on an attention-shifting and inhibition task was in the impaired range of ability. P.J. passed on all subtests of object perception from the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (Warrington and James 1991), except for progressive silhouettes, where he had a raw score of 11, indicating mild impairment. He was also faultless in all subtests of space perception.
P.J. was scanned using a research MRI protocol and tested behaviorally at the Bangor University School of Psychology approximately 1 year and 10 months following the ischemic event, when he was 47 years of age. Testing took place on 2 consecutive days.
Control Participants
Behavioral Comparison
A total of 10 right-handed, healthy male participants were recruited from the local community. Controls were screened for any history of major neuropsychiatric disorders and visual impairments. IQ was measured with the 2-subtest (vocabulary and matrix reasoning) version of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler 1999). Supplementary Table 2 summarizes the characteristics of the control group. The mean age was 48.2 years (standard deviation [SD]: 6.4), the mean IQ was 101.1 (SD: 7.6) and the mean age leaving school was 16.6 (SD: 0.7). On all these variables, P.J. and controls were matched; all P-values were above 0.095 using a modified t-test (Crawford and Howell 1998).
Anatomical Comparison
A convenience sample of 10 healthy male participants was drawn from a Bangor University image register. The mean age was 43.3 years (SD: 4.9).
All participants were compensated for their time and travel expenses. All participants gave written, informed consent prior to initiating any experimental procedure. The testing procedures had been reviewed and approved by the Betsi Cadwaladr University Health Board and the Bangor University School Psychology Ethics committees.
Behavioral Testing: Overview and Material
P.J. and controls performed 3 computer-based behavioral experiments. Testing took place in a dark room; participants sat comfortably, unrestrained, approximately 85 cm from an LCD screen (NEC LCD3210). Participants were encouraged to actively scan the display and foveate individual stimuli. Custom-coded Matlab scripts (Mathworks 2014a), using a set of freely available routines designed to facilitate the coding of visual experiments (Brainard 1997), controlled the experiments and generated the displays. Matlab scripts were run on an Apple iMac 10.
Statistical Comparison of P.J. and Controls
We computed the significance of performance differences between P.J. and the control group in all experiments using a modified t-test (Crawford and Howell 1998). Where performance was measured with a percentage or ratio, we conducted the t-test on logarithmically transformed values.
Imaging
Imaging—Image Acquisition and Analysis
P.J. and the anatomical comparison controls were scanned on a Phillips Achieva 3 T MR scanner with a 32-channel head coil. T1-weighted images (TE = 4.32 ms; 8° flip angle) were acquired axially with a 0.7 mm isotropic voxel-size. P.J.’s T1-weighted anatomical volume was bias corrected and normalized to the atlas representative MNI152 template using SPM12 (Ashburner and Friston 2003). The mapping included a 12-degrees-of-freedom affine transform followed by a local deformation, computed after the lesion had been masked using a hand-drawn region. The normalized anatomy was obtained by interpolation via a fourth degree B-spline, and resampled using a 0.7 mm linear voxel size. Skull stripped anatomy was obtained using a modified version of FSL’s BET, which is optimized for tissue segmentation in the presence of brain pathology (Lutkenhoff et al. 2014). To determine whether P.J.’s stroke encroached onto perirhinal and entorhinal cortex, probabilistic maps of these regions were superimposed on his brain anatomy (Hindy and Turk-Browne 2016). Lesion boundaries were drawn by a board-certified adult neurologist, using the coregistered T1 and FLAIR images.
Lesion Anatomy Results
Figure 1 shows axial and coronal slices from the MNI Atlas coregistered T1-weighted scan of P.J.’s brain. In the left hemisphere the lesion volume is 6.25 cm3, in the right hemisphere 10.71 cm3. Figure 1A shows that the ischemic lesions in medial occipitotemporal cortex (mOTC) of the left and right hemisphere lie posterior to the location of entorhinal and perirhinal cortex (marked respectively in red and green), identified in a previous group study (Hindy and Turk-Browne 2016). Figure S1 provides additional anatomical information about the relationship between lesion and entorhinal and perirhinal cortex. The coronal slices in Figure 1B demonstrate that the fornix is intact, however, sections −23 to −32 suggest hippocampal volume loss on the right. Also, retrosplenial cortex and the adjacent precuneus are spared in both hemispheres. Figure S2 shows sagittal slices through medial brain structures, which highlights the extent of the damage to PHC and lingual gyrus. Given the apparent hippocampal volume loss, we compared P.J.’s left and right hippocampal volumes to those of the anatomical comparison controls. A stereological procedure was used to estimate hippocampal volumes in all participants (Keller and Roberts 2009). The input images were the T1-weighted brain volumes in native scanner space. A regular cubic grid with a step of 3 pixels was superimposed on coronal slices, with a random starting position. The senior author, a board-certified neurologist, outlined the hippocampal formation to determine the number of overlaying grid points. The hippocampal formation included the hippocampus, dentate gyrus and subiculum. The anterior border of the hippocampal formation was the alveus, the posterior border was the crux of the fornix. The hippocampal borders were also identified in axial and sagittal slices. The procedure was implemented using ImageJ (Schneider et al. 2012) and a stereology dedicated plugin (Merzin 2008). This analysis indicated that P.J.’s left (3931 mm3) and right (2530 mm3) hippocampi were not significantly smaller than controls (left: mean = 3561 mm3; t(9) = 0.516, P = 0.618; right: mean = 3816 mm3; t(9) = −1.79, P = 0.108). However, the volumetric difference between the left and right hippocampi was significantly greater for P.J. than for controls (t(9) = 2.641, P = 0.027), suggesting that P.J.’s right hippocampus may have been atrophied.

Lesion anatomy. T1-weighted, MNI atlas registered axial (A) and coronal (B) slices are displayed in neurological coordinates, and illustrate the extent of ischemic damage in the left and right mOTC. (A) The axial slices also highlight the location of entorhinal and perirhinal cortex, in red and green, respectively. These regions lay anteriorly and laterally to the boundaries of the ischemic lesions. (B) Coronal slices highlight parahippocampal and hippocampal structures, including the fornix. The ischemic lesions lay inferiorly and posteriorly to the hippocampus and spare the fornix and the retrosplenial cingulate cortex. The hippocampi appear diminished in volume, more so on the right.
Experiment 1: Spatial Versus Nonspatial Binding in Working Memory
Experiment 1—Rationale
Primate studies (Malkova and Mishkin 2003; Belcher et al. 2006) have suggested that PHC is involved in remembering locations in close peri-personal space as well as spatial binding in working memory. In this first experiment, we examined visual working memory spatial and feature binding in P.J., a man with PHC lesions, and a group of age-matched controls. On each trial, participants had to remember the color, shape and location of 2 objects. After a short delay, participants were cued to recall the color of one of the objects, identified either by its location on the screen, or by its shape. We reasoned that if human PHC is involved in spatial binding, then P.J.’s recall performance should be worse than controls, specifically on location trials.
Experiment 1—Methods
Figure 2A shows a schematic representation of Experiment 1’s trial structure. In each trial, an equilateral triangle and a square, whose side lengths were 2.42° and 1.72°, respectively, appeared side-to-side in the lower half of the screen, at an eccentricity of 4.25° along the main diagonal, for 2.0 s. The shapes were either red, blue or green. A 200 ms pattern mask, and then a 2.0 s blank screen, followed the sample display. The recall screen contained 3 colored rectangles, 1.0° wide and 3.0° high, whose lower edges were aligned 2.5° above the screen center and spaced horizontally 9.0° apart. A bright cross (location cue) or the outline of 1 of the 2 shapes (shape cue) identified the target. The location cues, which also included a dark cross, appeared at the locations occupied by the 2 shapes. The shape cue appeared 3.0° below the screen center. Participants reported the target color by placing a cursor over the corresponding colored rectangle and clicking the mouse button. The mouse click prompted the beginning of a new trial, after a 1.0 s delay, during which the screen was blank. Participants practiced the task over 10 trials and then completed 90 trials, including both shape and location cued recalls. Trial order was randomized, minimizing participants’ ability to predict whether a shape or location cue would follow the sample display. To ensure that P.J. had not forgotten the task instructions, we asked him to describe what he had been doing after each block. In each instance he correctly reported that he had been recalling either the probed shape color, or the color at the location of the white cross.

Spatial versus nonspatial binding in working memory. (A) The trial structure. The sample display for all participants (including P.J.) contained a square and a triangle, placed side by side in the bottom half of the screen. The 2 objects were red, blue or green and never had the same color. After a brief pattern mask and blank delay, 3 vertical colored bars appeared as well as a cursor, which the participant used to report the color of the memory target. In shape trials, targets were identified by a probe whose outline matched the target shape. In location trials, the location of targets were identified by a white cross. (B) Each individual participants’ error rate on a greyscale, with lighter colors representing a higher proportion of errors; the left panel shows generic errors, the right panel shows binding errors. On each panel, the upper row shows errors following shape probes, while the lower row shows errors following location probes, for P.J. (blue outline) and each of the controls (red outline). (C) P.J.’s and the group averaged proportion of generic and binding errors. Error bars are standard error of the mean.
Experiment 1—Data Analysis
We scored trials based on whether participants reported (1) the correct target color (correct response), (2) the color of the nontarget shape (binding error), or (3) neither the target nor the nontarget color, that is, dummy color (generic error). We then calculated the proportion of binding (BE) and generic errors (GE) for each cue condition (location and shape) and compared P.J. and the control group’s recall accuracy using odds ratios. We computed 2 odds ratios: the first was the ratio of the proportion of binding errors in location versus shape cued trials (i.e., [BElocation/BEshape]). The second was the ratio of binding errors over generic errors in location versus shape cued trials (i.e., [BElocation/GElocation]/[BEshape/GEshape]). If a participant’s data cells contained zero counts, a value of 0.5 was added to all cells prior to computing the ratios (Gart and Zweifel 1967).
Experiment 1—Results: Impaired Spatial Binding in Visual Working Memory
The left-hand panels of Figure 2B, C report the proportion of generic errors following location and shape cues, while the right-hand panels show the proportion of binding errors. P.J. made more binding errors when the target was identified by a location than a shape cue (P < 0.001; Fisher exact test). P.J. was also much more likely to make a binding than a generic error following a location (P < 0.001, 2-tailed binomial test), but not a shape cue (P = 0.5), suggesting that his difficulties did not reflect a problem remembering which colors had been shown. For P.J., the odds ratio of making a binding error in the location versus shape cue trials was 60.7, which was significantly greater than the control group average of 0.501 (95% CI: [0.23–1.06], t(9) = 3.72, P = 0.005), suggesting that he was much more likely to make a binding error on location than shape cue trials, while controls were modestly more accurate following a location than a shape cue. Moreover, P.J.’s odds ratio of making a binding rather than a generic error in the location versus shape cue trials was 29.0 which was again significantly greater than the control group average of 0.421 (95% CI: [0.21–0.83], i(9) = 3.46, P = 0.007), confirming that he was much more likely to make a binding than a generic error on location rather than shape cue trials, while controls were more likely to make a binding than a generic error on shape rather than location cue trials.
Experiment 1: Interim Discussion
P.J. showed a remarkable deficit binding objects to their location in a working memory task. When he reported the color of one of 2 objects, he was able to do so accurately for targets cued by their shape. However, when a target was identified by its location, his performance was greatly diminished because of numerous binding errors. Control participants, on the other hand, showed comparable recall accuracy irrespective of the cue type. These findings strongly suggest that P.J.’s impairment cannot be attributed to either diminished memory for the report feature, that is, the target’s color, or a binding deficit that generalizes across visual dimensions. Rather, P.J. shows a binding impairment that is specifically spatial.
Experiment 2: Delayed Spatial Recall
Experiment 2—Rationale
In the previous experiment, we demonstrated that P.J. suffers a specific spatial binding impairment in a working memory task. In Experiment 2, we examined whether spatial binding impairments reflect diminished resolution of spatial data in working memory, or rather disruption of spatial binding. To this end we assessed the effects of the duration of the memory delay on both the precision of spatial recall and the proportion of binding errors.
Experiment 2—Methods
Figure 3A summarizes Experiment 2’s trial structure. The sample stimulus consisted of 3 colored discs, 0.8° in diameter. The discs were red, green and blue, and remained visible for 2.0 s. A 1.0 s long pattern mask followed the sample. A central color cue (a 0.3° wide square) appeared either immediately after the pattern mask, or after an additional 4.0 s interval, during which only a white central fixation point was visible. The cue identified the target of the same color. The participants placed the cursor at the recalled target location and clicked the mouse to record their response and initiate the next trial. The location of the discs included the center of the screen and the vertices of a virtual square, at an eccentricity of 6.0°. 2D Gaussian displacement (SD = 0.9°) jittered the position of each disc. Each participant completed 2 blocks of 120 trials each.

Delayed spatial recall. (A) Structure of immediate and delayed, spatial recall trials. The sample display for all participants (including P.J.) contained 3 colored discs, which could appear in both the upper and lower portion of the screen. The participants had to reproduce the position of one of the discs (the target) using a mouse cursor after either a 1.0 s pattern mask or an additional 4.0 s delay. The target was identified by its color, indicated by a visual probe displayed at the center of the screen. (B) (Left) shows P.J.’s (blue outline) and controls’ (red outline) individual percentage of binding errors on a greyscale, following 1.0 s (upper row) and 5.0 s (lower row) delays, with lighter colors representing a higher proportion of errors. (B) (Right) shows recall precision (95% error ellipses) in 1.0 and 5.0 s delayed recall trials for P.J. (blue) and controls (red). (C) P.J.’s and the group averaged proportion of binding errors and precision. Error bars are standard error of the mean.
Experiment 2—Data Analysis
First, we identified trials in which participants had made a binding error, that is, when the recalled position was closer to the one of the nontarget items than the target, and the distance from the nontarget item was no greater than half the minimum distance between canonical locations, that is, 3.0° (Pertzov et al. 2013). After tabulating and removing binding errors, we estimated recall accuracy and precision. Accuracy reflects how close a participant’s average reported location is to the true target position. Precision reflects the magnitude of trial-to-trial deviations from a participant’s average reported location. Accuracy is diminished by systematic errors, which depend on factors such as display size and memory load (Katshu and d’Avossa 2014), while precision is thought to reflect the resolution of spatial memory (Bays et al. 2009). These 2 variables were computed using linear regressions. We computed 2 regressions whose dependent variables were the azimuth and elevation of the reported target location, respectively. The regressors in each case included a constant and the target’s azimuth and elevation. The results of the regression analysis were used to estimate the systematic biases reporting the target location. The scaling factor was the divergence of the error field, which we previously found to be the main linear component of the systematic error (Katshu and d’Avossa 2014). We quantified recall precision using the standard deviation of the residuals from the model fits. The variance and standard deviations of the variable errors were computed using the same procedure employed in a previous study (Katshu and d’Avossa 2014), and averaged over azimuth and elevation. Precision changes between short and long delays were quantified using an efficiency measure, namely a ratio whose numerator was the recall variance following 1.0 s delays and denominator was recall variance following 5.0 s delays.
Experiment 2—Results: Recall Precision, But not Binding Errors, Affected by Memory Delay
P.J. made more binding errors than controls, following both 1.0 and 5.0 s delays. Otherwise, both P.J. and controls performed similarly in terms of accuracy and precision.
The proportion of binding errors are shown in the left-hand panels of Figure 3B, C. Overall, P.J. made a binding error on 9.44% of trials, which was significantly greater than the control group average of 3.21% (95% CI: [2.24–4.18]; t(9) = 4.02; P = 0.003). Increasing the duration of the memory delay had no effect on the proportion of P.J.’s relative binding errors; P.J.’s odds ratio for making a binding error following 1.0 versus 5.0 s delays was 1.27, which was not significantly different to the control group average of 1.0 (95% CI: [0.72–1.38]; t(9) = 0.462; P = 0.655), and suggested a nonsignificant tendency for more binding errors following short than long memory delays. Further, 40% (6/15) of P.J.’s binding errors on short delay trials, and 50% (6/12) of his binding errors on long delay trials, occurred when the target appeared in the upper portion of the screen; a goodness of fit test reported that his binding errors were not biased toward the target appearing in either the upper or lower half of the screen following either delay (χ2 (3) = 1, P = 0.801). We can therefore conclude that his binding issues are unlikely due to his upper visual field deficit impacting the encoding of the entire sample stimulus.
Both P.J. and controls showed systematic distortions. Following both short and long memory delays, P.J. reported targets displaced leftward (1.0 s: −0.24°; 5.0 s: −0.23°) and upward (1.0 s: 0.15°; 5.0 s: 0.09°). In contrast, controls’ group mean displacement was rightward (1.0 s: 0.09°, 95% CI: [−0.09 to 0.26]; 5.0 s: 0.07°, 95% CI: [−0.12 to 0.27];) and downward (1.0 s: −0.37°, 95% CI: [−0.55 to −0.19]; 5.0 s: −0.28°, 95% CI: [−0.45 to −0.11]). However, P.J.’s displacements were not significantly different from controls for both delays (all P-values > 0.100). P.J. also tended to overestimate the position of targets relative to the screen center, indicated by an error divergence of 0.04 following 1.0 s delays and 0.16 following 5.0 s delays. In contrast, controls underestimated targets relative to the screen center, as indicated by a group average error divergence of −0.26 (95% CI: [−0.36 to −0.15]) following 1.0 s delays and −0.29 (95% CI: [−0.41 to −0.16]) following 5.0 s delays. However, P.J. and controls did not differ significantly (both P-values > 0.055).
Recall precision data are summarized in the right-hand panel of Figure 3B, C. In contrast to binding errors, increasing the delay had a significant effect on recall precision. P.J.’s error standard deviation was 1.33° following 1.0 s delays, which was not statistically different from the control group average of 1.01° (95% CI: [0.91–1.10]; t(9) = 2.11; P = 0.064). P.J.’s error standard deviation following 5.0 s delays (1.78°) was statistically larger than the control group average of 1.18° (95% CI: [1.09–1.27]; t(9) = 4.23; P = 0.002). However, P.J.’s efficiency after a 5.0 s delay compared with a 1.0 s delay was 0.56, which was not significantly smaller than the control group average of 0.73 (95% CI: [0.65–0.82]; t(9) = −1.37; P = 0.203).
Experiment 2: Interim Discussion
The experiment yielded a number of findings. First P.J. made more binding errors than controls, confirming that he exhibited an impairment of spatial binding using a task in which the target location was the report rather than the cue variable. Secondly, following 1.0 s delay the precision recalling the target location was not appreciably different between P.J. and controls, suggesting that his binding impairment did not reflect a problem recalling the target location precisely. Moreover, while increasing the memory delay did not increase the proportion of binding errors, it did significantly diminish both P.J. and controls’ spatial recall precision, providing additional evidence that recall precision did not account for binding errors. In summary, P.J. shows frequent binding errors, but spatial recall precision which is comparable to that of controls. Crucially, changing the duration of the memory delay produces dissociable effects on recall precision and binding.
Experiment 3: Centroid Estimation
Experiment 3—Rationale
In Experiment 3 we ascertained whether P.J.’s diminished recall of a target position may reflect a sensory impairment. While this seems unlikely given the finding that P.J.’s recall precision was not significantly diminished compared with controls (with 1.0 s delay), it was important to establish the extent to which sensory difficulties may have limited his performance. We therefore assessed participants’ spatial accuracy and precision in a perceptual task.
Experiment 3—Methods
This experiment assessed participants’ ability to localize the centroid, namely the average location, of 3 white discs. The discs’ diameter was 0.5° (see Fig. 4A for a schematic representation of the trial structure). The discs remained visible until participants had positioned a crosshair shaped cursor at the desired location and clicked the mouse. Following a blank, 1.0 s-long interval, a novel set of discs appeared and the procedure was repeated. Discs could occupy any of 7 canonical locations. These included the screen center and the vertices of a virtual concentric hexagon, with a side length of 6.87°. All permutations of 3 out of 7 canonical target locations, less any resulting in a collinear configuration, were used as sample arrays. Each possible permutation appeared twice, for a total of 64 trials. A pseudorandom, zero mean, circular Gaussian distribution, with a standard deviation of 0.6°, was used to jitter each disc’s position independently. Prior to testing, instructions were read to the participants. The centroid was defined as the point in space where the triangle, whose vertices coincided with the discs’ locations, would balance in the horizontal plane (Baud-Bovy and Soechting 2001). One of the experimenters also provided a visual demonstration, using a cut-out triangular shape. Prior to testing, participants completed 25 practice trials. At the end of each practice trial, the reported and actual positions of the centroid were shown for 2.0 s.

Centroid estimation. (A) The trial structure. The participants placed a cursor at the centroid of the configuration formed by 3 bright discs. The discs remained visible until the participant made a response by clicking the mouse. (B) Each participant’s constant displacement (arrow vectors), scaling (diamond plot) and precision (uncertainty ellipses) in locating the centroid. The length of the diamond plot’s hemi-axes corresponds to 1.0 scaling factor. (C) P.J.’s and group averaged values of the constant displacement and scaling factor, separately for azimuth (X) and elevation (Y). The precision measure shown is the square root of the mean error variance for azimuth and elevation. Error bars in all cases are standard error of the mean.
Experiment 3—Data Analysis
We estimated the systematic and variable error of participants’ centroid estimations, by fitting a linear model to the azimuth and elevation of the reported centroid location. The model regressors included a constant and the centroid azimuth and elevation. Two metrics were used to characterize the systematic error: (1) the constant displacement, that is the tendency to report the centroid above, below, right or left of its true location, and (2) scaling factor, measuring the linear relationship between reported and actual centroid positions. These are, respectively, the estimated intercept and beta parameters of the linear model. We computed precision as the standard deviation of the variable error, that is, residuals from the model, using the same methods used in Experiment 2.
Experiment 3—Results: Accuracy and Precision of Centroid Estimation
The left-hand panels of Figure 4B, C illustrate the direction of systematic biases in centroid estimates. P.J. and controls respectively reported the centroid −0.07° and −0.10° (95% CI: [−0.15° to −0.04°]) left of its veridical position, suggesting that both showed a similarly small leftward bias, (t(9) = 0.322, P = 0.755). However, P.J. reported the centroid 0.56° above its veridical position. This bias was significantly larger than controls, who showed a group average upward bias of 0.06° (95% CI: [−0.02° to 0.14°]; t(9) = 3.69, P = 0.005). The middle panel of Figure 4B, C summarize the linear scaling for centroid estimates. P.J. varied the reported centroid azimuth by a factor of 0.97, and elevation by a factor of 1.00, in both cases reflecting an almost perfect linear relationship between reported an actual centroid positions. These values were comparable to those shown by controls, namely, 0.99 for azimuth (95% CI: [0.94–1.03]; t(9) = −0.263, P = 0.799), and 0.97 for elevation (95% CI: [0.93–1.01]; t(9) = 0.443, P = 0.668). Finally, P.J.’s azimuth variable error standard deviation, 0.67°, was not significantly different from the control average of 0.69° (95% CI = [0.56°–0.82°]; t(9) = −0.091, P = 0.931), nor was his elevation variable error standard deviation, 0.77°, significantly different from the control average of 0.59° (95% CI = [0.47°–0.70°]; t(9) = 0.925, P = 0.380), suggesting that both the vertical and horizontal precision of his centroid judgements was relatively spared.
Experiment 3—Interim Discussion
P.J. showed a strong tendency to report the centroid above its true location. This probably represents a compensatory strategy for his upper visual field defect. In fact, hemianopic patients display a bias toward their blind field when judging the midpoint of horizontal line (Barton and Black 1998; Kerkhoff and Buchers 2008). However, both P.J.s accuracy and precision estimating the centroid position were within the control group’s range. We conclude that aside from compensatory visual defect biases, P.J.’s ability to localise perceptually is largely spared and unlikely to account for his diminished recall precision.
Discussion
We tested a middle-aged man (P.J.) with bilateral mOTC strokes involving the PHC. Acutely, P.J. had developed a derangement of attention and short-term memory (Horenstein et al. 1967; Medina et al. 1977; Shih et al. 2007). At the time of testing, P.J. was no longer delirious, but continued to have difficulties with his memory as well as navigating familiar environments. The latter is a form of spatial disorientation previously attributed to PHC lesions in humans (Zola-Morgan et al. 1989; Epstein et al. 2001). Animal studies have demonstrated additional deficits in spatial working memory following PHC lesions in nonhuman primates (Malkova and Mishkin 2003; Bachevalier and Nemanic 2008). Whether the same deficits characterize human patients with PHC lesions is not yet known.
We found that P.J. had a profound deficit binding an object to its location in a working memory task. When he recalled the color of 1 of 2 objects, after a short memory delay, he could accurately do so when the target was cued by its shape. However, when the target was cued by its location, his accuracy was greatly diminished because he made numerous binding errors, frequently reporting the color of the nontarget item instead of the color of the target. Control participants, on the other hand, were accurate whether the target was identified by the location or shape cue. These findings strongly suggest that P.J. was impaired only when using a location cue and that this impairment could not be attributed to either diminished memory for the report feature, that is, the target’s color, or a binding deficit that generalizes across spatial and nonspatial visual dimensions. According to a recent study, generalized binding difficulties may instead characterize recall performance in individuals with autoimmune temporal encephalitis, which mainly affects the hippocampal formation (Pertzov et al. 2013).
Some animal and imaging studies have indeed shown that both anterior PHC and hippocampus contribute to object-in-place associations in short-term memory (Milner et al. 1997; Bachevalier and Nemanic 2008). However, animal data suggest that hippocampal involvement in spatial binding is restricted to tasks where spatial relations are incidentally encoded (Bachevalier and Nemanic 2008). These findings, together with ours, suggest that in tasks where spatial information is intentionally encoded and recalled, the role of PHC goes beyond simply providing spatial data to the hippocampus, where general purpose processes bind visual features in working memory. Moreover, our findings confirm that binding in visual working memory is liable to be disrupted by focal brain lesions (Gorgoraptis et al. 2011), supporting the idea that it is a neural function independent from those underpinning the representations of individual features (Wheeler and Treisman 2002; Smyrnis et al. 2005).
Binding Errors do not Reflect the Resolution of Spatial Information
When P.J. reported the location of 1 of 3 objects held in memory he erroneously reported the location of one of the nontarget items more frequently than controls. This finding suggests that P.J. had difficulties with spatial binding, whether space was the cue or report dimension. One might argue that P.J.’s spatial binding impairment simply reflects degraded spatial representations. In other words, diminished ability recalling the location of an object might explain his difficulties using spatial information to identify targets in memory. However, this hypothesis is not supported by our data. P.J. was able to estimate the centroid of simple dot configurations as precisely as controls, indicating that despite the presence of an upper visual field defect, the spatial resolution of visual data was not prominently affected in this perceptual task. Moreover, P.J.’s precision recalling the location of visual targets was not appreciably different from that of controls, even though his proportion of spatial binding errors was much greater. Finally, binding errors did not become more frequent when the delay interval was increased, although the precision of spatial recall did decrease. We conclude that binding errors do not reflect the temporal decay of a memory trace, contrary to previous suggestions (Zhang and Luck 2009). Moreover, our findings are consistent with observations that binding errors are not affected by the duration of the memory delay in either patients with hippocampal pathology (Pertzov et al. 2013) or healthy controls (Gorgoraptis et al. 2011), although whether binding errors may be effected by longer (e.g., >20.0 s) delays remains to be established. Finally, varying the spatial memory demands at the time of recall in a spatial version of the Sternberg working memory task does not change the likelihood of committing a binding error, confirming that binding errors do not reflect confusion among features of the probe dimension (Smyrnis et al. 2005). Taken together, the available evidence in healthy controls and patients instead suggests that binding errors reflect interference with early processes, engaged at the time when visual information is encoded in working memory. However, a recent high-resolution fMRI study has suggested that load dependent signals in PHC during the delay period of a match-to-sample-task may reflect on-going binding processes (Schon et al. 2016).
Delays Affect the Precision of Spatial Recall
P.J.’s spatial recall precision was similar to that of controls when the memory delay lasted 1.0 s. When the memory delay was 5.0 s long, both he and controls suffered a decrement in recall precision. These are not entirely novel findings. Recall precision is known to decrease with longer memory delays in healthy controls (Sheth and Shimojo 2001; Zhang and Luck 2009). Moreover, recall precision disproportionally decreases in patients with PHC lesions, although significantly so only following memory delays greater than 20 s (Ploner et al. 2000). This finding is in keeping with our own: recall efficiency following 5.0 versus 1.0 s delays was lower in P.J. than in controls, however, this difference was not significant. Combined, these data are consistent with the idea that following PHC lesions, spatial recall precision decays more quickly than in healthy controls, as opposed to declining abruptly. More generally, our findings are in keeping with the view that spatial recall draws information from a limited capacity resource (Bays et al. 2009), whose resolution diminishes over time. Therefore, delay dependent changes in spatial recall precision most likely reflect a limited ability to maintain information in working memory rather than impaired encoding, in contrast to the binding deficits discussed above. Finally, P.J.’s performance in our experiments is consistent with his neuropsychological profile, which is principally characterized by impairment on various memory tasks, including those that do not have a spatial binding component, such as the Logical Memory test and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. However, we do not yet know the extent to which diminished recall precision and spatial binding account for the broad memory deficits observed following lesions to PHC.
Could the Hippocampus be the Site for Short-term Memory Spatial Binding?
In the present study we identified impairments resulting from focal lesions to PHC, and found a spatial binding deficit in short-term memory. Our data cannot rule out the possibility that binding takes place outside PHC, for example, in the hippocampus. Indeed, comparison of hippocampal volumes in P.J. and age and gender matched controls suggest hippocampal atrophy in P.J. Lateralized hippocampal atrophy commonly follows distal, ipsilateral stroke, even in young patients unlikely to harbor neurodegenerative processes (Schaapsmeerders et al. 2015a, 2015b), suggesting that the hippocampus may be particularly vulnerable to the effects of deafferentation. P.J.’s hippocampal atrophy raises the possibility that spatial binding deficits reflect diminished function within the hippocampus. Our data cannot refute this alternative hypothesis. As mentioned in the introduction, previous studies in patients with inflammatory and anoxic damage involving the hippocampus (Pertzov et al. 2013; Watson et al. 2013; Yee et al. 2014) have also demonstrated spatial binding impairments, lending support to the hippocampus’ role in feature binding. Nonetheless, the specific spatial nature of P.J.’s binding impairment, which did not generalize to other visual dimensions (i.e., shape), is inconsistent with the proposal that the hippocampus provides a general purpose binding mechanism. Therefore, we conclude that spatial binding is either carried out in hippocampus, using inputs from PHC, or that PHC itself initiates spatial binding processes.
Concluding Remarks
This study provides novel information on the role of MTL, by showing that a man with a lesion involving PHC, hippocampal atrophy, but spared PRC, has a selective deficit in short-term spatial binding. This deficit is not explained by diminished resolution of spatial information. Our findings are consistent with the idea that spatial binding processes in short-term memory may be initiated in the PHC even before visual information reaches the hippocampus.
Funding
Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council grant BB/1007091/1.
Footnotes
The authors thank Paul Mullins for his assistance with MRI data acquisition, and for providing the anatomical control data. Conflict of Interest: None declared.