Abstract

Neurodegeneration has multiscalar impacts, including behavioral, neuroanatomical, and neurofunctional disruptions. Can disease-differential alterations be captured across such dimensions using naturalistic stimuli? To address this question, we assessed comprehension of four naturalistic stories, highlighting action, nonaction, social, and nonsocial events, in Parkinson’s disease (PD) and behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) relative to Alzheimer’s disease patients and healthy controls. Text-specific correlates were evaluated via voxel-based morphometry, spatial (fMRI), and temporal (hd-EEG) functional connectivity. PD patients presented action–text deficits related to the volume of action–observation regions, connectivity across motor-related and multimodal-semantic hubs, and frontal hd-EEG hypoconnectivity. BvFTD patients exhibited social–text deficits, associated with atrophy and spatial connectivity patterns along social-network hubs, alongside right frontotemporal hd-EEG hypoconnectivity. Alzheimer’s disease patients showed impairments in all stories, widespread atrophy and spatial connectivity patterns, and heightened occipitotemporal hd-EEG connectivity. Our framework revealed disease-specific signatures across behavioral, neuroanatomical, and neurofunctional dimensions, highlighting the sensitivity and specificity of a single naturalistic task. This investigation opens a translational agenda combining ecological approaches and multimodal cognitive neuroscience for the study of neurodegeneration.

Introduction

The multiscalar impact of neurodegeneration encompasses behavioral, anatomical, and connectivity levels (Arvanitakis et al. 2019). These dimensions are differentially affected depending on the disease and its higher-order impairments (García-Cordero et al. 2016; Melloni et al. 2016). In particular, Parkinson’s disease (PD) and behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) patients exhibit deficits in grasping action-related and social verbal meanings, respectively, together with structural and functional brain abnormalities (Gregory et al. 2002; Birba et al. 2017). However, no existing framework captures such disease-specific patterns via naturalistic stimuli, creating a gap between ecological neurocognitive models and translational research on neurodegeneration. Here we combined behavioral, neuroanatomical (voxel-based morphometry [VBM]), and resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) measures (derived from fMRI and high-density EEG [hd-EEG]) to examine core signatures of naturalistic action- and social-discourse comprehension in PD and bvFTD, vis-à-vis healthy controls and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients.

Beyond dominant motor symptoms and diverse neurocognitive dysfunctions (Helmich et al. 2012), PD involves deficits in processing action language—verbal materials denoting bodily movement (Boulenger et al. 2008; Fernandino et al. 2013; Gallese and Cuccio 2018; Cervetto et al. 2021). Indeed, comprehension of actions is affected early, selectively, and primarily in PD (Birba et al. 2017). These deficits entail alterations in movement-related circuits, such as atrophy of motor regions (Birba et al. 2017), altered activation (Péran et al. 2009) and connectivity (Moguilner et al. 2021a) patterns along motor networks, and motor-related temporal (hd-EEG) rsFC abnormalities (Melloni et al. 2015). Thus, verbal tasks evoking bodily actions could afford sensitive multidimensional signatures of PD (Birba et al. 2017).

Regarding bvFTD, patients exhibit various social cognition disorders (Piguet et al. 2011), including early difficulties in linguistic tasks taxing emotional, empathic, and mentalizing skills (Hsieh et al. 2012; Ibáñez et al. 2018), as well as faux pas detection (Gregory et al. 2002) and social coordination (McMillan et al. 2012). These domains are subserved by fronto-insulo-temporal networks and frontotemporal oscillatory (hd-EEG) dynamics that are specifically affected in bvFTD (Piguet et al. 2011; Ibáñez et al. 2017) and underpin processing of verbal social stimuli (Perdikis et al. 2017; Rice and Hoffman 2018). Therefore, linguistic paradigms tapping social information comprehension may provide multiscalar markers of bvFTD.

Yet, these antecedents mainly rely on single-item tasks. Though critical to capture selective disruptions and potential markers of neurodegenerative disorders (Hardy et al. 2016; Birba et al. 2017; Geraudie et al. 2021), such paradigms neglect the contextual and cohesive richness of real-life discourse, offering low ecological validity (Hasson et al. 2018). Importantly, those alterations cannot be a priori assumed to hold in discourse-level materials, as contextual information influences category-specific word processing (Van Dam et al. 2010), favors the retrieval of task-relevant information (Ledoux et al. 2006), and facilitates performance in persons with brain disorders (Aviezer et al. 2009; García et al. 2018). Furthermore, findings of category-specific deficits in PD and bvFTD lack an integrated theoretical rationale enabling their joint conceptualization. Promisingly, action-related deficits in PD (García et al. 2018) and sociocognitive difficulties in bvFTD (McMillan et al. 2012) can be synergistically interpreted via embodied cognition models, which posit that language understanding depends on reactivations of sensorimotor and affective networks (Pulvermüller 2013). Strategically, our framework uses a naturalistic embodied paradigm including an action text (AT), a nonaction text (nAT), a social text (ST), and a nonsocial text (nST). This allows tackling differential patterns of deficit in each disorder. To further test for specificity, we also included AD patients, whose broad memory impairments and widespread neural alterations would potentially affect all text categories (Piguet et al. 2011; Melloni et al. 2015; García-Cordero et al. 2016; Melloni et al. 2016). This way, we aimed to ascertain the ecological validity, nosological specificity, and neurobiological signatures of these disturbances.

Considering previous findings, we raised three predictions. First, PD patients would exhibit selective action comprehension deficits related to atrophy and spatial rsFC alterations along action-related circuits, together with altered connectivity signatures. Second, bvFTD patients would be selectively impaired in grasping social information, with this pattern being linked to disruptions of areas and networks subserving social cognition, as well as frontotemporal hd-EEG hypoconnectivity. Finally, AD should entail cross-categorical deficits associated to widespread neural disturbances and memory deficits. Briefly, our naturalistic framework aims to track discriminatory embodied signatures of PD and bvFTD across multiple neurocognitive dimensions.

Experimental design and data analysis. (A) Sample collection and neuropsychological assessment. PD patients, bvFTD patients, AD patients, and healthy controls were included in the study upon completion of a neuropsychological assessment that complemented their clinical evaluation. (B) Discourse-level assessment. In the discourse-level task, participants listened to four naturalistic texts, namely an action text (AT), a nonaction text (nAT), a social text (ST), and a nonsocial text (nST). After each one, they answered a 14-item multiple-choice comprehension questionnaire tapping on verb-related and circumstantial information. Results were first subjected to within-text analyses, to evaluate whether a specific condition (verbs or circumstances) in each text yielded impairments in each patient sample compared with controls. Then, we performed a cross-textual analysis, including “text” as a factor and evaluating all its possible interactions with “group” and “condition.” After checking that the variable “condition” did not interact with “group,” we averaged the two conditions per text and generated four aggregate scores (AT, nAT, ST, nST), thus reducing dimensionality while increasing variance across texts for the regressions with MRI, fMRI, and hd-EEG data. (C) Neuroimaging data collection. Participants underwent a resting-state MRI and fMRI session. Then, a subsample of participants completed a 10-min resting-state protocol while hd-EEG signals were recorded. (D) Data analysis. The aggregate text scores were regressed in a VBM analysis and associated with the functional connectivity metrics derived from the resting-state fMRI and hd-EEG sessions. HCs: healthy controls; PD: Parkinson’s disease patients; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia patients; AD: Alzheimer’s disease patients; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; IFS: INECO Frontal Screening battery; AV: action verbs; AC: action circumstances; nAV: nonaction verbs; nAC: nonaction circumstances; SV: social verbs; nSV: nonsocial verbs; MRI: magnetic resonance imaging; VBM: voxel-based morphometry; fMRI: functional magnetic resonance imaging; hd-EEG: high-density electroencephalography.
Figure 1

Experimental design and data analysis. (A) Sample collection and neuropsychological assessment. PD patients, bvFTD patients, AD patients, and healthy controls were included in the study upon completion of a neuropsychological assessment that complemented their clinical evaluation. (B) Discourse-level assessment. In the discourse-level task, participants listened to four naturalistic texts, namely an action text (AT), a nonaction text (nAT), a social text (ST), and a nonsocial text (nST). After each one, they answered a 14-item multiple-choice comprehension questionnaire tapping on verb-related and circumstantial information. Results were first subjected to within-text analyses, to evaluate whether a specific condition (verbs or circumstances) in each text yielded impairments in each patient sample compared with controls. Then, we performed a cross-textual analysis, including “text” as a factor and evaluating all its possible interactions with “group” and “condition.” After checking that the variable “condition” did not interact with “group,” we averaged the two conditions per text and generated four aggregate scores (AT, nAT, ST, nST), thus reducing dimensionality while increasing variance across texts for the regressions with MRI, fMRI, and hd-EEG data. (C) Neuroimaging data collection. Participants underwent a resting-state MRI and fMRI session. Then, a subsample of participants completed a 10-min resting-state protocol while hd-EEG signals were recorded. (D) Data analysis. The aggregate text scores were regressed in a VBM analysis and associated with the functional connectivity metrics derived from the resting-state fMRI and hd-EEG sessions. HCs: healthy controls; PD: Parkinson’s disease patients; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia patients; AD: Alzheimer’s disease patients; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; IFS: INECO Frontal Screening battery; AV: action verbs; AC: action circumstances; nAV: nonaction verbs; nAC: nonaction circumstances; SV: social verbs; nSV: nonsocial verbs; MRI: magnetic resonance imaging; VBM: voxel-based morphometry; fMRI: functional magnetic resonance imaging; hd-EEG: high-density electroencephalography.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study comprised 109 native Spanish speakers: 25 PD patients, 20 bvFTD patients, 23 AD patients, and 41 controls. This sample size reaches a power of 0.96 (Supplementary Material 1). Participants were recruited in three centers from the Multi-Partner Consortium to Expand Dementia Research in Latin America (ReDLat) (Ibáñez, Pina-Escudero, et al. 2021b; Ibáñez, Yokoyama, et al. 2021c). Unified procedures ensure consistency across sites so that participants can be reliably framed as an integrated multicentric sample (Ibáñez, Pina-Escudero, et al. 2021b; Ibáñez, Yokoyama, et al. 2021c). These same procedures have been reported in previous works (Baez et al. 2014; Melloni et al. 2016; García et al. 2017; Donnelly-Kehoe et al. 2019; Moguilner et al. 2020; Legaz et al. 2021; Salamone et al. 2021; Ibáñez, Fittipaldi, et al. 2021a). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal hearing. Patients were diagnosed by expert neurologists following UKPD-SBB standards for PD (Hughes et al. 1992), current criteria for probable bvFTD (Rascovsky et al. 2011), and NINCDS-ADRDA clinical criteria for AD (McKhann et al. 2011). Diagnoses were supported by extensive neurological, neuropsychiatric, and neuropsychological examinations (Piguet et al. 2011) (Fig. 1A). No patient reported a history of other neurological disorders, psychiatric conditions, primary language deficits, or substance abuse.

PD patients had no symptoms of Parkinson-plus or significant atrophy patterns (Supplementary Material 2). They were assessed during the “on” phase of antiparkinsonian medication. Available UPDRS-III scores fell below the cutoff for mild/moderate symptoms. BvFTD exhibited sociobehavioral impairments as defined by caregivers (Piguet et al. 2011) and predominantly frontotemporal atrophy (Supplementary Material 2). AD patients presented executive dysfunction and working memory (WM) deficits, as established through the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS) battery (Torralva et al. 2009), as well as predominant temporohippocampal atrophy (Supplementary Material 2). Available CDR scores for bvFTD and AD patients fell slightly below and above the cutoff for mild dementia, respectively. All patient groups had mild cognitive impairment, based on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al. 2005). Controls were cognitively preserved, functionally autonomous, and had no background of neuropsychiatric disease or alcohol/drug abuse.

All groups were matched for age and education. A significant difference in sex emerged between bvFTD patients and controls. Thus, although no other group pair exhibited differences, all analyses were covaried for sex. For demographic and neuropsychological details, see Table 1.

Table 1

Demographic and neuropsychological information

HC
N = 41
PD
N = 25
bvFTD
N = 20
AD
N = 23
Main effectPairwise comparisons
GroupsestimateP-value
Demographic data
Sex (F:M)30:1112:136:1416:7χ2 = 12.64
P = 0.005a
HC-PD3.230.07b
HC-bvFTD8.650.003b
HC-AD0.00040.98b
Years of age72.05
(5.84)
7.40
(7.00)
68.20
(9.41)
73.30
(4.94)
F = 2.40
P = 0.07a
HC-PD0.980.33c
HC-bvFTD1.670.15c
HC-AD-0.910.36c
Years of education13.37
(3.56)
13.32
(4.54)
14.95
(5.18)
11.87
(4.38)
F = 1.83
P = 0.14a
HC-PD0.040.96c
HC-bvFTD-1.230.22c
HC-AD1.400.17c
Neuropsychological data
MoCA (cutoff: 21e)25.65
(3.3)
23.32
(5.19)
21.79
(5.38)
17.43
(4.19)
F = 16.17
P < 0.001a
HC-PD2.000.05c
HC-bvFTD2.870.008c
HC-AD7.77<0.001c
IFS battery (cutoff: 25f)21.26
(4.07)
20.16
(5.22)
18.89
(5.56)
14.50
(5.29)
F = 9.68
P < 0.001a
HC-PD0.890.81c
HC-bvFTD1.610.12c
HC-AD5.27<0.001c
UPDRS-IIId (cutoff: 32g)20.41
(14.70)
CDRd (cutoff: 1h)0.84
(0.85)
1.14
(0.65)
HC
N = 41
PD
N = 25
bvFTD
N = 20
AD
N = 23
Main effectPairwise comparisons
GroupsestimateP-value
Demographic data
Sex (F:M)30:1112:136:1416:7χ2 = 12.64
P = 0.005a
HC-PD3.230.07b
HC-bvFTD8.650.003b
HC-AD0.00040.98b
Years of age72.05
(5.84)
7.40
(7.00)
68.20
(9.41)
73.30
(4.94)
F = 2.40
P = 0.07a
HC-PD0.980.33c
HC-bvFTD1.670.15c
HC-AD-0.910.36c
Years of education13.37
(3.56)
13.32
(4.54)
14.95
(5.18)
11.87
(4.38)
F = 1.83
P = 0.14a
HC-PD0.040.96c
HC-bvFTD-1.230.22c
HC-AD1.400.17c
Neuropsychological data
MoCA (cutoff: 21e)25.65
(3.3)
23.32
(5.19)
21.79
(5.38)
17.43
(4.19)
F = 16.17
P < 0.001a
HC-PD2.000.05c
HC-bvFTD2.870.008c
HC-AD7.77<0.001c
IFS battery (cutoff: 25f)21.26
(4.07)
20.16
(5.22)
18.89
(5.56)
14.50
(5.29)
F = 9.68
P < 0.001a
HC-PD0.890.81c
HC-bvFTD1.610.12c
HC-AD5.27<0.001c
UPDRS-IIId (cutoff: 32g)20.41
(14.70)
CDRd (cutoff: 1h)0.84
(0.85)
1.14
(0.65)

Notes: Data presented as mean (SD), with the exception of sex. HC: healthy controls; PD: Parkinson’s disease; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; AD: Alzheimer’s disease; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; IFS: INECO Frontal Screening battery; UPDRS-III: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, part III; CDR: Cognitive Dementia Rating.

aP-values calculated via independent measures ANOVA.

bP-values calculated via chi-squared test (χ2).

cP-values calculated via unpaired t-test.

dData from patients who completed the full clinical protocol.

eCutoff for MCI (Delgado et al. 2019).

fCutoff for mild effects (Torralva et al. 2009).

gCutoff for moderate effects (Martinez-Martin et al. 2015).

hCutoff for mild dementia (Morris 1991).

Table 1

Demographic and neuropsychological information

HC
N = 41
PD
N = 25
bvFTD
N = 20
AD
N = 23
Main effectPairwise comparisons
GroupsestimateP-value
Demographic data
Sex (F:M)30:1112:136:1416:7χ2 = 12.64
P = 0.005a
HC-PD3.230.07b
HC-bvFTD8.650.003b
HC-AD0.00040.98b
Years of age72.05
(5.84)
7.40
(7.00)
68.20
(9.41)
73.30
(4.94)
F = 2.40
P = 0.07a
HC-PD0.980.33c
HC-bvFTD1.670.15c
HC-AD-0.910.36c
Years of education13.37
(3.56)
13.32
(4.54)
14.95
(5.18)
11.87
(4.38)
F = 1.83
P = 0.14a
HC-PD0.040.96c
HC-bvFTD-1.230.22c
HC-AD1.400.17c
Neuropsychological data
MoCA (cutoff: 21e)25.65
(3.3)
23.32
(5.19)
21.79
(5.38)
17.43
(4.19)
F = 16.17
P < 0.001a
HC-PD2.000.05c
HC-bvFTD2.870.008c
HC-AD7.77<0.001c
IFS battery (cutoff: 25f)21.26
(4.07)
20.16
(5.22)
18.89
(5.56)
14.50
(5.29)
F = 9.68
P < 0.001a
HC-PD0.890.81c
HC-bvFTD1.610.12c
HC-AD5.27<0.001c
UPDRS-IIId (cutoff: 32g)20.41
(14.70)
CDRd (cutoff: 1h)0.84
(0.85)
1.14
(0.65)
HC
N = 41
PD
N = 25
bvFTD
N = 20
AD
N = 23
Main effectPairwise comparisons
GroupsestimateP-value
Demographic data
Sex (F:M)30:1112:136:1416:7χ2 = 12.64
P = 0.005a
HC-PD3.230.07b
HC-bvFTD8.650.003b
HC-AD0.00040.98b
Years of age72.05
(5.84)
7.40
(7.00)
68.20
(9.41)
73.30
(4.94)
F = 2.40
P = 0.07a
HC-PD0.980.33c
HC-bvFTD1.670.15c
HC-AD-0.910.36c
Years of education13.37
(3.56)
13.32
(4.54)
14.95
(5.18)
11.87
(4.38)
F = 1.83
P = 0.14a
HC-PD0.040.96c
HC-bvFTD-1.230.22c
HC-AD1.400.17c
Neuropsychological data
MoCA (cutoff: 21e)25.65
(3.3)
23.32
(5.19)
21.79
(5.38)
17.43
(4.19)
F = 16.17
P < 0.001a
HC-PD2.000.05c
HC-bvFTD2.870.008c
HC-AD7.77<0.001c
IFS battery (cutoff: 25f)21.26
(4.07)
20.16
(5.22)
18.89
(5.56)
14.50
(5.29)
F = 9.68
P < 0.001a
HC-PD0.890.81c
HC-bvFTD1.610.12c
HC-AD5.27<0.001c
UPDRS-IIId (cutoff: 32g)20.41
(14.70)
CDRd (cutoff: 1h)0.84
(0.85)
1.14
(0.65)

Notes: Data presented as mean (SD), with the exception of sex. HC: healthy controls; PD: Parkinson’s disease; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; AD: Alzheimer’s disease; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; IFS: INECO Frontal Screening battery; UPDRS-III: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, part III; CDR: Cognitive Dementia Rating.

aP-values calculated via independent measures ANOVA.

bP-values calculated via chi-squared test (χ2).

cP-values calculated via unpaired t-test.

dData from patients who completed the full clinical protocol.

eCutoff for MCI (Delgado et al. 2019).

fCutoff for mild effects (Torralva et al. 2009).

gCutoff for moderate effects (Martinez-Martin et al. 2015).

hCutoff for mild dementia (Morris 1991).

All participants provided written informed consent pursuant to the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the Ethics Committees of the involved institutions.

Experimental Protocol

All participants completed a multidimensional protocol involving neuropsychological and discourse-level assessments, as well as MRI, fMRI, and hd-EEG recordings (Fig. 1).

Discourse-Level Task

Naturalistic Texts

All narratives were created through a systematic protocol (Trevisan and García 2019) for establishing semantic distinctions between text sets Table 2; García et al. 2018). The number of critical items manifesting the semantic contrasts between AT, nAT, ST, and nST was statistically controlled across texts, and so were relevant linguistic variables (Table 3). For details, see Supplementary Material 3.

Table 2

General features of the texts

ATnATSTnST
ThemeA day at the playgroundA nightly outingTwo neighbors interactingA peaceful day at home
ContentHigh action contentLow action contentHigh social contentLow social content
VerbsMostly motor processesMostly nonmotor processesMostly social nonmotoric processesMostly nonsocial motor process
Verb exampleRunningWaitingAccepting (a request)Exercising
CircumstancesGeneral circumstantial informationGeneral circumstantial informationSocially laden circumstantial informationNonsocially laden circumstantial information
Circumstance exampleSaturday afternoonThe streetKindlyOn his nightstand
ATnATSTnST
ThemeA day at the playgroundA nightly outingTwo neighbors interactingA peaceful day at home
ContentHigh action contentLow action contentHigh social contentLow social content
VerbsMostly motor processesMostly nonmotor processesMostly social nonmotoric processesMostly nonsocial motor process
Verb exampleRunningWaitingAccepting (a request)Exercising
CircumstancesGeneral circumstantial informationGeneral circumstantial informationSocially laden circumstantial informationNonsocially laden circumstantial information
Circumstance exampleSaturday afternoonThe streetKindlyOn his nightstand
Table 2

General features of the texts

ATnATSTnST
ThemeA day at the playgroundA nightly outingTwo neighbors interactingA peaceful day at home
ContentHigh action contentLow action contentHigh social contentLow social content
VerbsMostly motor processesMostly nonmotor processesMostly social nonmotoric processesMostly nonsocial motor process
Verb exampleRunningWaitingAccepting (a request)Exercising
CircumstancesGeneral circumstantial informationGeneral circumstantial informationSocially laden circumstantial informationNonsocially laden circumstantial information
Circumstance exampleSaturday afternoonThe streetKindlyOn his nightstand
ATnATSTnST
ThemeA day at the playgroundA nightly outingTwo neighbors interactingA peaceful day at home
ContentHigh action contentLow action contentHigh social contentLow social content
VerbsMostly motor processesMostly nonmotor processesMostly social nonmotoric processesMostly nonsocial motor process
Verb exampleRunningWaitingAccepting (a request)Exercising
CircumstancesGeneral circumstantial informationGeneral circumstantial informationSocially laden circumstantial informationNonsocially laden circumstantial information
Circumstance exampleSaturday afternoonThe streetKindlyOn his nightstand
Table 3

Linguistic features of the texts

ATnATSTnSTStatisticP-value*Pairwise comparisons**
Charactersa936974959949X2 = 0.810.85
Words207203199199X2 = 0.210.97
Nouns48444340X2 = 0.740.86
Verbs32323232X2 = 10.00
Action verbs241715X2 = 25.42<0.001AT-nAT: X2 = 21.16, P < 0.001
AT-ST: X2 = 9.33, P = 0.002
nST-nAT: X2 = 12.25, P < 0.001
nAT-ST: X2 = 4.5, P = 0.03.
Nonaction verbs8312517X2 = 14.750.002AT-nAT: X2 = 13.56, P < 0.001
AT-ST: X2 = 8.75, P = 0.003
nST-nAT: X2 = 4.1, P = 0.04.
Social verbs56240X2 = 37.8<0.001ST-AT: X2 = 12.48, P < 0.001
ST-nAT: X2 = 1.8, P = 0.001
ST-nST: X2 = 24, P < 0.001
nST-AT: X2 = 5, P = 0.02
nST-nAT: X2 = 6, P = 0.01.
Nonsocial verbs2726832X2 = 13.130.004ST-AT: X2 = 1.31, P = 0.001
ST-nAT: X2 = 9.5, P = 0.002
ST-nST: X2 = 14.4, P < 0.001.
CAs27222830X2 = 1.290.72
Social CAs42150X2 = 25.67<0.001ST-AT: X2 = 6.36, P = 0.01
ST-nAT: X2 = 9.9, P = 0.001
ST-nST: X2 = 15, P < 0.001.
Nonsocial CAs23201330X2 = 6.930.07ST-nST: X2 = 6.72, P = 0.009.
Frequencyb1.641.791.731.82F = 1.230.29
Familiarityb6.176.286.336.23F = 0.850.46
Imageabilityc5.174.964.855.00F = 0.820.48
Syllabic lengthc2.502.452.592.42F = 0.760.52
Graphemic lengthc5.956.036.295.81F = 1.090.35
Sentences22222223X2 = 0.030.99
Minor sentences3334X2 = 0.230.97
Simple sentences8888X2 = 10.99
Compound sentences3445X2 = 0.050.91
Complex sentences8776X2 = 0.280.96
Grammaticalityd4.454.243.744.24F = 1.900.12
Coherenced4.004.003.744.00F = 0.650.63
Comprehensibilityd4.504.384.254.38F = 0.370.83
Readabilitye77.3079.9274.8172.26
Reading difficultyfFairly easyFairly easyFairly easyFairly easy
Emotional valencegMain effect of text: F = 0.03, P = 0.99.
Text-by-emotion interaction: F = 2.98, P = 0.008
Tukey’s HSD tests showed no significant between-text effects in any emotion (P > 0.05).
 Positive55.4556.066.7161.18
 Negative5.6111.971.62.31
 Neutral39.0931.9736.0439.29
ATnATSTnSTStatisticP-value*Pairwise comparisons**
Charactersa936974959949X2 = 0.810.85
Words207203199199X2 = 0.210.97
Nouns48444340X2 = 0.740.86
Verbs32323232X2 = 10.00
Action verbs241715X2 = 25.42<0.001AT-nAT: X2 = 21.16, P < 0.001
AT-ST: X2 = 9.33, P = 0.002
nST-nAT: X2 = 12.25, P < 0.001
nAT-ST: X2 = 4.5, P = 0.03.
Nonaction verbs8312517X2 = 14.750.002AT-nAT: X2 = 13.56, P < 0.001
AT-ST: X2 = 8.75, P = 0.003
nST-nAT: X2 = 4.1, P = 0.04.
Social verbs56240X2 = 37.8<0.001ST-AT: X2 = 12.48, P < 0.001
ST-nAT: X2 = 1.8, P = 0.001
ST-nST: X2 = 24, P < 0.001
nST-AT: X2 = 5, P = 0.02
nST-nAT: X2 = 6, P = 0.01.
Nonsocial verbs2726832X2 = 13.130.004ST-AT: X2 = 1.31, P = 0.001
ST-nAT: X2 = 9.5, P = 0.002
ST-nST: X2 = 14.4, P < 0.001.
CAs27222830X2 = 1.290.72
Social CAs42150X2 = 25.67<0.001ST-AT: X2 = 6.36, P = 0.01
ST-nAT: X2 = 9.9, P = 0.001
ST-nST: X2 = 15, P < 0.001.
Nonsocial CAs23201330X2 = 6.930.07ST-nST: X2 = 6.72, P = 0.009.
Frequencyb1.641.791.731.82F = 1.230.29
Familiarityb6.176.286.336.23F = 0.850.46
Imageabilityc5.174.964.855.00F = 0.820.48
Syllabic lengthc2.502.452.592.42F = 0.760.52
Graphemic lengthc5.956.036.295.81F = 1.090.35
Sentences22222223X2 = 0.030.99
Minor sentences3334X2 = 0.230.97
Simple sentences8888X2 = 10.99
Compound sentences3445X2 = 0.050.91
Complex sentences8776X2 = 0.280.96
Grammaticalityd4.454.243.744.24F = 1.900.12
Coherenced4.004.003.744.00F = 0.650.63
Comprehensibilityd4.504.384.254.38F = 0.370.83
Readabilitye77.3079.9274.8172.26
Reading difficultyfFairly easyFairly easyFairly easyFairly easy
Emotional valencegMain effect of text: F = 0.03, P = 0.99.
Text-by-emotion interaction: F = 2.98, P = 0.008
Tukey’s HSD tests showed no significant between-text effects in any emotion (P > 0.05).
 Positive55.4556.066.7161.18
 Negative5.6111.971.62.31
 Neutral39.0931.9736.0439.29

Notes: AT: action text; nAT: nonaction text; ST: social text; nST: nonsocial text; CAs: circumstantial adjuncts.

aCharacter count was performed without counting spaces.

bData extracted from the LEXESP database, through B-Pal (Davis and Perea 2005). Results based on the mean of all content words in each text.

cData extracted from B-Pal (Davis and Perea 2005). Results based on the mean of all content words in each text.

dData collected from a panel of 13 Spanish-speaking undergraduates, based on Likert scales ranging from 1 (very low) to 5 (very high).

eBased on the Szigriszt-Pazos Index (Szigrist Pazos 1993).

fBased on the Inflesz scale rating (Barrio-Cantalejo et al. 2008).

gData collected from a panel of 17 native Spanish speakers, who rated each sentence in the texts in terms of overall emotional content (positive, negative, or neutral).

*Alpha level set at P < 0.05.

**Only significant differences are shown (omitted contrasts were all nonsignificant).

Table 3

Linguistic features of the texts

ATnATSTnSTStatisticP-value*Pairwise comparisons**
Charactersa936974959949X2 = 0.810.85
Words207203199199X2 = 0.210.97
Nouns48444340X2 = 0.740.86
Verbs32323232X2 = 10.00
Action verbs241715X2 = 25.42<0.001AT-nAT: X2 = 21.16, P < 0.001
AT-ST: X2 = 9.33, P = 0.002
nST-nAT: X2 = 12.25, P < 0.001
nAT-ST: X2 = 4.5, P = 0.03.
Nonaction verbs8312517X2 = 14.750.002AT-nAT: X2 = 13.56, P < 0.001
AT-ST: X2 = 8.75, P = 0.003
nST-nAT: X2 = 4.1, P = 0.04.
Social verbs56240X2 = 37.8<0.001ST-AT: X2 = 12.48, P < 0.001
ST-nAT: X2 = 1.8, P = 0.001
ST-nST: X2 = 24, P < 0.001
nST-AT: X2 = 5, P = 0.02
nST-nAT: X2 = 6, P = 0.01.
Nonsocial verbs2726832X2 = 13.130.004ST-AT: X2 = 1.31, P = 0.001
ST-nAT: X2 = 9.5, P = 0.002
ST-nST: X2 = 14.4, P < 0.001.
CAs27222830X2 = 1.290.72
Social CAs42150X2 = 25.67<0.001ST-AT: X2 = 6.36, P = 0.01
ST-nAT: X2 = 9.9, P = 0.001
ST-nST: X2 = 15, P < 0.001.
Nonsocial CAs23201330X2 = 6.930.07ST-nST: X2 = 6.72, P = 0.009.
Frequencyb1.641.791.731.82F = 1.230.29
Familiarityb6.176.286.336.23F = 0.850.46
Imageabilityc5.174.964.855.00F = 0.820.48
Syllabic lengthc2.502.452.592.42F = 0.760.52
Graphemic lengthc5.956.036.295.81F = 1.090.35
Sentences22222223X2 = 0.030.99
Minor sentences3334X2 = 0.230.97
Simple sentences8888X2 = 10.99
Compound sentences3445X2 = 0.050.91
Complex sentences8776X2 = 0.280.96
Grammaticalityd4.454.243.744.24F = 1.900.12
Coherenced4.004.003.744.00F = 0.650.63
Comprehensibilityd4.504.384.254.38F = 0.370.83
Readabilitye77.3079.9274.8172.26
Reading difficultyfFairly easyFairly easyFairly easyFairly easy
Emotional valencegMain effect of text: F = 0.03, P = 0.99.
Text-by-emotion interaction: F = 2.98, P = 0.008
Tukey’s HSD tests showed no significant between-text effects in any emotion (P > 0.05).
 Positive55.4556.066.7161.18
 Negative5.6111.971.62.31
 Neutral39.0931.9736.0439.29
ATnATSTnSTStatisticP-value*Pairwise comparisons**
Charactersa936974959949X2 = 0.810.85
Words207203199199X2 = 0.210.97
Nouns48444340X2 = 0.740.86
Verbs32323232X2 = 10.00
Action verbs241715X2 = 25.42<0.001AT-nAT: X2 = 21.16, P < 0.001
AT-ST: X2 = 9.33, P = 0.002
nST-nAT: X2 = 12.25, P < 0.001
nAT-ST: X2 = 4.5, P = 0.03.
Nonaction verbs8312517X2 = 14.750.002AT-nAT: X2 = 13.56, P < 0.001
AT-ST: X2 = 8.75, P = 0.003
nST-nAT: X2 = 4.1, P = 0.04.
Social verbs56240X2 = 37.8<0.001ST-AT: X2 = 12.48, P < 0.001
ST-nAT: X2 = 1.8, P = 0.001
ST-nST: X2 = 24, P < 0.001
nST-AT: X2 = 5, P = 0.02
nST-nAT: X2 = 6, P = 0.01.
Nonsocial verbs2726832X2 = 13.130.004ST-AT: X2 = 1.31, P = 0.001
ST-nAT: X2 = 9.5, P = 0.002
ST-nST: X2 = 14.4, P < 0.001.
CAs27222830X2 = 1.290.72
Social CAs42150X2 = 25.67<0.001ST-AT: X2 = 6.36, P = 0.01
ST-nAT: X2 = 9.9, P = 0.001
ST-nST: X2 = 15, P < 0.001.
Nonsocial CAs23201330X2 = 6.930.07ST-nST: X2 = 6.72, P = 0.009.
Frequencyb1.641.791.731.82F = 1.230.29
Familiarityb6.176.286.336.23F = 0.850.46
Imageabilityc5.174.964.855.00F = 0.820.48
Syllabic lengthc2.502.452.592.42F = 0.760.52
Graphemic lengthc5.956.036.295.81F = 1.090.35
Sentences22222223X2 = 0.030.99
Minor sentences3334X2 = 0.230.97
Simple sentences8888X2 = 10.99
Compound sentences3445X2 = 0.050.91
Complex sentences8776X2 = 0.280.96
Grammaticalityd4.454.243.744.24F = 1.900.12
Coherenced4.004.003.744.00F = 0.650.63
Comprehensibilityd4.504.384.254.38F = 0.370.83
Readabilitye77.3079.9274.8172.26
Reading difficultyfFairly easyFairly easyFairly easyFairly easy
Emotional valencegMain effect of text: F = 0.03, P = 0.99.
Text-by-emotion interaction: F = 2.98, P = 0.008
Tukey’s HSD tests showed no significant between-text effects in any emotion (P > 0.05).
 Positive55.4556.066.7161.18
 Negative5.6111.971.62.31
 Neutral39.0931.9736.0439.29

Notes: AT: action text; nAT: nonaction text; ST: social text; nST: nonsocial text; CAs: circumstantial adjuncts.

aCharacter count was performed without counting spaces.

bData extracted from the LEXESP database, through B-Pal (Davis and Perea 2005). Results based on the mean of all content words in each text.

cData extracted from B-Pal (Davis and Perea 2005). Results based on the mean of all content words in each text.

dData collected from a panel of 13 Spanish-speaking undergraduates, based on Likert scales ranging from 1 (very low) to 5 (very high).

eBased on the Szigriszt-Pazos Index (Szigrist Pazos 1993).

fBased on the Inflesz scale rating (Barrio-Cantalejo et al. 2008).

gData collected from a panel of 17 native Spanish speakers, who rated each sentence in the texts in terms of overall emotional content (positive, negative, or neutral).

*Alpha level set at P < 0.05.

**Only significant differences are shown (omitted contrasts were all nonsignificant).

Comprehension Questionnaires

Each text had a 14-item multiple-choice questionnaire featuring wh- questions (Trevisan and García 2019). Half the questions pointed to verb-related information and half to circumstances, realized by adverbial or prepositional phrases. Questions were presented following the stories’ sequence of events. Successive questions were independent from each other. Each question was accompanied by five options: a correct response, three incorrect responses, and “I don’t remember.” The latter option always appeared last; the other three were randomized across questions. Correct responses received one point; incorrect responses received zero. Each questionnaire had a maximum of 14 points (7 for verb-related questions, 7 for circumstantial questions). See Supplementary Material 4.

Procedure

First, a different narrative was administered for familiarization purposes. Each text was presented via stereo headphones. Subjects were instructed to close their eyes and listen attentively to answer some questions. Following each narration, the examiner overtly read each question and its options. When necessary, questions were read twice; if additional repetitions were requested, the item received zero points. Participants answered orally. The texts were counterbalanced across participants (Fig. 1B, top).

Discourse-Level Task Analyses

We performed two types of hypothesis-driven analyses. Based on the results of García et al. (2018), we first predicted specific deficits in action verbs for PD and, by extension, anticipated selective social language impairments in bvFTD. As in previous multimodal text-level research with multiple patient groups (Moguilner et al. 2021b), we carried out a within-text analysis via 4 × 2 mixed-effects ANOVAs, with a between-subject factor (“group”) and a within-subject factor (“condition”: verbs, circumstances). Second, to increase stringency, we implemented a cross-textual analysis via a 4x4x2 mixed-effects ANOVA, adding “text” as a within-subject factor. Since no group-by-condition interaction emerged, we averaged the two conditions per text and generated four aggregate scores (Fig. 1B, bottom), thus reducing dimensionality while increasing variance for brain–behavior associations. All analyses were covaried for sex. Contrasts of groups were inspected via Tukey’s HSD tests. Note that, although our naturalistic stimuli are matched for multiple variables, comparisons across texts are not recommended for this paradigm, given that other important aspects were not controlled among narratives (e.g., mean utterance length, propositional density, and metaphoricity, as well as articulatory, phonetic, and prosodic aspects of the recordings) (García et al. 2018; Moguilner et al. 2021b). To control for mnesic skills, all text-level analyses were replicated covarying for the IFS WM index (added scores of the backward digits span, verbal WM, and spatial WM subtests). Alpha was set at P < 0.05. Effect sizes were calculated through partial eta squared (η2) for ANOVA results and Cohen’s d for pairwise comparisons. Analyses were performed on R (v.3.5.2).

MRI/fMRI Methods

Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

MRI/fMRI acquisition and preprocessing steps are reported following OHBM recommendations (Nichols et al. 2017). We acquired three-dimensional volumetric and 10-min-long resting-state MRI sequences (Fig. 1C, left). Twenty dimensional volumetric images (from eight controls and four patients per pathological group) and twenty-four functional images (from eight controls, five PD patients, six bvFTD patients, and five AD patients) were excluded due to artifacts. The resulting samples remained demographically matched (Supplementary Material 5). For specific parameters and MRI preprocessing details, see Supplementary Material 6.

VBM Analysis

For each control–patient tandem, we examined associations between whole-brain GM volume and the four aggregate text scores (AT, nAT, ST, nST) via SPM12’s multiple regression module (Fig. 1D, left). As this is an exploratory study with a modest sample size per group, statistical significance was set to P < 0.001, uncorrected, with an extent threshold of 50 voxels, as in previous imaging studies (Melloni et al. 2016; Santamaria-García et al. 2017; Sedeño et al. 2017; de la Fuente et al. 2019). These parameters match recommended limits for the study’s power levels (Woo et al. 2014), circumventing biases of liberal primary thresholds on false positives and achieving good balance between Type-I and Type-II errors (Lieberman and Cunningham 2009). See Supplementary Material 7.

fMRI rsFC Analysis

Based on the resting-state fMRI recordings, we examined positive associations between each text’s aggregate score and rsFC patterns (Fig. 1C, middle). See Supplementary Material 6.

We used Pearson’s correlations (García-Cordero et al. 2016) to establish rsFC between each pair of areas from the adjacency matrix, and then, for each control–patient set, we examined Spearman’s correlations between connectivity patterns and each text’s aggregate score. We used an uncorrected threshold of P < 0.001 (Jabbi et al. 2008; Kanske et al. 2016), which avoids detrimental effects of liberal primary thresholds on false positives (Fig. 1D, middle; Lieberman and Cunningham 2009). See Supplementary Material 8.

hd-EEG Methods

Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

Sixty-three participants (21 controls, 12 PD patients, 15 bvFTD patients, 15 AD patients) completed a 10-min-long hd-EEG rsFC protocol (Fig. 1C, right). These subsamples remained sociodemographically matched. See Supplementary Material 9.

hd-EEG rsFC Analysis

hd-EEG rsFC analysis was performed through the weighted symbolic mutual information (wSMI) metric, which captures nonlinear information sharing during cognitive operations (Hesse et al. 2016), including embodied semantic processes (Melloni et al. 2015). See Supplementary Material 9.1.

To identify disease-specific wSMI patterns, we performed nonparametric cluster-based permutations (1000 iterations) for independent samples (Maris and Oostenveld 2007), comparing controls to each patient group (Fig. 1D, upper right). Significant connections were plotted on a 3D head model (Fig. 1D, lower right). See Supplementary Material 10.

Next, for each control–patient set, we tested Pearson’s correlations between the significant cluster’s mean connectivity and the participants’ aggregate scores in each text. Multiple comparisons among the four texts in each set of correlations were corrected via the false discovery rate (FDR) method. See Supplementary Material 10.

Results

Behavioral Results

Within-Text Analyses

The AT revealed a group-by-condition interaction (Fig. 2A, first inset) [F(3,105) = 3.1, P = 0.03, η2 = 0.08]. PD patients exhibited action verb deficits (P < 0.001, d = 5.94) and preserved outcomes on circumstances (P = 0.76, d = 2.33; Fig. 2A, second inset). The nAT yielded significant effects of group [F(3,104) = 31.5, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.48]. ST outcomes revealed significant effects of group [F(3,104) = 26.50, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.43], with controls outperforming bvFTD (P = 0.01, d = 4.11) but not PD (P = 0.25, d = 2.74) patients (Fig. 2A, third inset). The nST yielded a significant group effect [F(3,104) = 15.43, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.31], with controls performing similar to bvFTD patients (P = 0.10, d = 4) and better than PD patients (P < 0.005, d = 5.33; Fig. 2A, fourth inset). AD patients were outperformed by all three groups in the four texts (all P-values < 0.05). For full results, see Supplementary Material 11. All significant results remained so after covariation with WM outcomes. See Supplementary Material 12.

Behavioral results and associations with VBM outcomes. (A) Within-text analyses. Results from the action text showed that, relative to controls, PD patients were impaired in verbs but not in circumstances; no such deficit was observed in bvFTD patients. Neutral text outcomes showed similar performance among controls and PD and bvFTD patients. Social text scores revealed a specific deficit for bvFTD patients relative to controls, in both conditions; no such deficit was observed in PD patients. Outcomes in the nonsocial text yielded a significant impairment of PD patients relative to controls; no such deficit was observed in bvFTD patients. AD patients were impaired in the two conditions of each text compared with all three other groups. Lateral asterisks denote condition-specific deficits relative to controls. Midline asterisks indicate significant differences for an entire text relative to controls. (B) Cross-textual analyses. Aggregate text scores confirmed the patterns observed in the within-text analyses. Relative to controls, PD patients were selectively impaired in the action and nonsocial texts, while bvFTD patients showed a selective impairment in the social text. AD patients were impaired in all four texts. Asterisks indicate significant differences relative to controls. (C) Associations with VBM. Each patient group was analyzed in tandem with controls to identify regions associated with the aggregate score of each text (P < 0.001 uncorrected, extent threshold = 50 voxels). For PD, significant associations were found for the action and nonsocial texts with action observation areas. For bvFTD, the social text was mainly associated with frontotemporal hubs of the so-called social network, whereas the action text was associated with action-related and multimodal semantic regions—the other two texts yielded sparser, unspecific associations. For AD, all four texts were associated with temporohippocampal regions typically underlying memory deficits in this disorder. HC: healthy controls; PD: Parkinson’s disease; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; AD: Alzheimer’s dementia. VBM: voxel-based morphometry.
Figure 2

Behavioral results and associations with VBM outcomes. (A) Within-text analyses. Results from the action text showed that, relative to controls, PD patients were impaired in verbs but not in circumstances; no such deficit was observed in bvFTD patients. Neutral text outcomes showed similar performance among controls and PD and bvFTD patients. Social text scores revealed a specific deficit for bvFTD patients relative to controls, in both conditions; no such deficit was observed in PD patients. Outcomes in the nonsocial text yielded a significant impairment of PD patients relative to controls; no such deficit was observed in bvFTD patients. AD patients were impaired in the two conditions of each text compared with all three other groups. Lateral asterisks denote condition-specific deficits relative to controls. Midline asterisks indicate significant differences for an entire text relative to controls. (B) Cross-textual analyses. Aggregate text scores confirmed the patterns observed in the within-text analyses. Relative to controls, PD patients were selectively impaired in the action and nonsocial texts, while bvFTD patients showed a selective impairment in the social text. AD patients were impaired in all four texts. Asterisks indicate significant differences relative to controls. (C) Associations with VBM. Each patient group was analyzed in tandem with controls to identify regions associated with the aggregate score of each text (P < 0.001 uncorrected, extent threshold = 50 voxels). For PD, significant associations were found for the action and nonsocial texts with action observation areas. For bvFTD, the social text was mainly associated with frontotemporal hubs of the so-called social network, whereas the action text was associated with action-related and multimodal semantic regions—the other two texts yielded sparser, unspecific associations. For AD, all four texts were associated with temporohippocampal regions typically underlying memory deficits in this disorder. HC: healthy controls; PD: Parkinson’s disease; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; AD: Alzheimer’s dementia. VBM: voxel-based morphometry.

Cross-Textual Analysis

The cross-textual analysis (Fig. 2B) revealed a significant group-by-text interaction [F(9,735) = 2.36, P = 0.01, η2 = 0.06]. The group-by-condition and the group-by-text-by-condition interactions were nonsignificant (P-values > 0.18). Post hoc comparisons (Tukey’s HSD: MSE = 0.02, df = 223.4) showed that, compared with controls, PD patients were selectively impaired in the AT (P < 0.005, d = 4 0.31) and the nST (P < 0.005, d = 5.88), while bvFTD patients presented selective deficits in the ST (P = 0.005, d = 5.88)—with no significant differences between these pathological groups. AD patients were outperformed by all groups in all texts (all P-values < 0.05). For full results, see Supplementary Material 13. All significant results remained so after covariation with WM outcomes (Supplementary Material 14), and they were not driven by category-specific patterns of forgetfulness or fatigue (Supplementary Material 15).

Briefly, relative to controls, PD patients showed difficulties in the AT (and in the nST, which features high motility) and bvFTD patients presented ST deficits, whereas AD patients were impaired in all texts.

MRI/fMRI Results

Associations between VBM and Performance

In the PD–control tandem (Fig. 2C, top), AT scores were associated with extrastriate body area (EBA) regions (e.g., right FG). Performance on nST outcomes was associated with regions subserving body-motion imagery (e.g., LG). No significant associations emerged for nAT or ST scores (Supplementary Material 16.1).

In the bvFTD–control tandem (Fig. 2C, middle), ST scores were associated with frontal (e.g., OFC), temporoparietal (e.g., MTG), and occipital regions. Second, nST scores were related to frontostriatal (caudate), temporoparietal, occipital, paracentral, and right hippocampal areas. A similar pattern emerged for AT scores, associated with frontal (e.g., OFC), temporoparietal (precuneus), and occipital regions. Finally, nAT scores were associated with the right inferior temporal and occipital cortices (Supplementary Material 16.2).

In the AD–control tandem (Fig. 2C, bottom), a significant association emerged between AT scores and temporohippocampal (e.g., MTG), parietal (e.g., IPG), frontal (e.g., SFG), and occipital regions, as well as the right cerebellum. The nAT was associated with the left hippocampus as well as the right parahippocampus, MTG, amygdala, and IPG. ST scores were associated with temporohippocampal, parietal (IPG), and frontal (insula) regions, as well as the left cerebellum. Finally, the nST was associated with temporohippocampal (e.g., MTG), parietal, and frontal (e.g., OFC) regions, as well as the right occipital cortex (Supplementary Material 16.3).

Associations between Spatial (fMRI-Derived) rsFC and Performance

In the PD–control tandem (Fig. 3A, top), AT scores correlated with rsFC across frontostriatal (piriform cortex), and occipital regions, as well as the cerebellum. No significant association emerged for nAT outcomes. ST scores correlated with rsFC between the right hippocampus and posterior (e.g., STG) hubs, and between the piriform cortex and the right cerebellum. Finally, nST scores were associated with rsFC between the hippocampus and the left STG and right inferior occipital cortex, as well as between the left cuneus and vermis (Supplementary Material 17.1).

rsFC results. (A) fMRI-derived rsFC. A whole-brain analysis, over 116 regions of interest, at P < 0.001 (uncorrected), was performed to test the association between the functional connectivity of each pair of brain areas and each of the four aggregate text scores. Link thickness indicates connectivity strength. ANG: angular gyrus; AMYG: amygdala; CRBL: cerebellum; CUN: cuneus; FFG: fusiform gyrus; HIP: hippocampus; IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; INS: insula; IOG: inferior occipital gyrus; ITG: inferior temporal gyrus; MFG: middle frontal gyrus; MOG: middle occipital gyrus; OLF: olfactory cortex; ORB: orbitofrontal; PAL: pallidus; PCUN: precuneus; PHG: parahippocampal gyrus; preCG: precentral gyrus; PSG: posterior cingulum; REC: rectus gyrus; SMA: supplementary motor area; SOG: superior orbital gyrus; STG: superior temporal gyrus; THA: thalamus; TPO: temporoparietal occipital junction. For better visualization, see Supplementary material 17.4. (B) hd-EEG-derived rsFC. (B1) Significant clusters per patient group. The three insets show the topographic wSMI patterns of the subtracted connectivity between each group of patients and controls (in a range of 4–10 Hz). Between-group comparisons were performed via cluster-based nonparametric permutation tests, at P < 0.05. The panel shows reduced connectivity for PD (left inset) and bvFTD (middle inset) patients relative to controls, as well as enhanced connectivity for AD patients (right inset). The color map indicates ranges from orange (indicating lower connectivity for the patient group) to violet (indicating lower connectivity for controls). The 3D head model represents the links of the cluster, with the arc size indicating connectivity strength. (B2) Associations between significant hd-EEG rsFC clusters and behavioral outcomes. Pearson’s correlations between the mean connectivity of each significant cluster and the four aggregate scores. Significant correlations are highlighted. All P-values are FDR-corrected. HC: healthy controls; PD: Parkinson’s disease; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; AD: Alzheimer’s disease; rsFC: resting-state functional connectivity; wSMI: weighted symbolic mutual information.
Figure 3

rsFC results. (A) fMRI-derived rsFC. A whole-brain analysis, over 116 regions of interest, at P < 0.001 (uncorrected), was performed to test the association between the functional connectivity of each pair of brain areas and each of the four aggregate text scores. Link thickness indicates connectivity strength. ANG: angular gyrus; AMYG: amygdala; CRBL: cerebellum; CUN: cuneus; FFG: fusiform gyrus; HIP: hippocampus; IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; INS: insula; IOG: inferior occipital gyrus; ITG: inferior temporal gyrus; MFG: middle frontal gyrus; MOG: middle occipital gyrus; OLF: olfactory cortex; ORB: orbitofrontal; PAL: pallidus; PCUN: precuneus; PHG: parahippocampal gyrus; preCG: precentral gyrus; PSG: posterior cingulum; REC: rectus gyrus; SMA: supplementary motor area; SOG: superior orbital gyrus; STG: superior temporal gyrus; THA: thalamus; TPO: temporoparietal occipital junction. For better visualization, see Supplementary material 17.4. (B) hd-EEG-derived rsFC. (B1) Significant clusters per patient group. The three insets show the topographic wSMI patterns of the subtracted connectivity between each group of patients and controls (in a range of 4–10 Hz). Between-group comparisons were performed via cluster-based nonparametric permutation tests, at P < 0.05. The panel shows reduced connectivity for PD (left inset) and bvFTD (middle inset) patients relative to controls, as well as enhanced connectivity for AD patients (right inset). The color map indicates ranges from orange (indicating lower connectivity for the patient group) to violet (indicating lower connectivity for controls). The 3D head model represents the links of the cluster, with the arc size indicating connectivity strength. (B2) Associations between significant hd-EEG rsFC clusters and behavioral outcomes. Pearson’s correlations between the mean connectivity of each significant cluster and the four aggregate scores. Significant correlations are highlighted. All P-values are FDR-corrected. HC: healthy controls; PD: Parkinson’s disease; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; AD: Alzheimer’s disease; rsFC: resting-state functional connectivity; wSMI: weighted symbolic mutual information.

In the bvFTD–control tandem (Fig. 3A, middle), ST scores were associated with rsFC between frontal (e.g., amygdala) and right hippocampal/parahippocampal hubs. Also, AT scores correlated with rsFC between frontostriatal (e.g., piriform) and posterior (e.g., cerebellar) regions. No significant associations emerged for either nST or nAT scores (Supplementary Material 17.2).

In the AD–control tandem (Fig. 3A, bottom), AT scores were associated with rsFC across multiple default-mode-network (DMN) hubs, as well as the SMA and the occipital cortex. Also, nAT outcomes were associated with rsFC between bilateral OFC, cerebellar, and occipital hubs. An association also emerged between ST scores and rsFC between the right insula and the bilateral STG, and between the right angular gyrus and the left precuneus. Finally, nST outcomes correlated with rsFC across DMN hubs, among others (Supplementary Material 17.3).

hd-EEG Results

Disease-Specific wSMI Clusters

Relative to controls, PD patients presented bilateral frontal hypoconnectivity (P = 0.04, cluster-corrected; Fig. 3B1, left), whereas bvFTD patients featured right frontotemporal hypoconnectivity (P = 0.04, cluster-corrected) (Fig. 3B1, middle) and AD patients presented higher right occipitotemporal connectivity (P = 0.04, cluster-corrected; Fig. 3B1, right).

Associations between Temporal (hd-EEG-Derived) rsFC and Performance

In the PD–control tandem (Fig. 3B2, left), mean frontal-cluster connectivity correlated positively with AT (r = 0.41, P = 0.04, FDR-corrected) and ST (r = 0.41, P = 0.04, FDR-corrected) scores. In the bvFTD–control tandem (Fig. 3B2, middle), the right frontotemporal cluster’s mean connectivity correlated with ST outcomes (r = 0.45, P = 0.02, FDR-corrected). For the AD–control tandem (Fig. 3B2, right), the right occipitotemporal cluster correlated negatively with AT (r = 0.56, P < 0.001, FDR-corrected), nAT (r = −44, P = 0.01, FDR-corrected), and ST (r = −.39, P = 0.03, FDR-corrected) scores. Every other association was nonsignificant (all P-values > 0.06). These patterns were specific to each patient group, as shown by null results in most control correlations—except for AD (Supplementary Material 18).

Discussion

Our multidimensional framework tracked discriminatory markers of PD, bvFTD, and AD via a naturalistic language paradigm. PD and bvFTD presented selective deficits in action-related and socially laded texts, respectively, associated with putative motor and social regions and networks, respectively. Instead, AD patients were impaired in all discourse types, with outcomes related to unspecific and widespread neural alterations. Below we discuss these findings in turn.

PD patients were impaired in the AT, with no deficits in the nAT or the ST. This replicates evidence of action–language difficulties for PD with preservation of multiple categories in single-word (Bocanegra et al. 2015) and discourse-level (García et al. 2018) tasks, corroborating that this domain distinctively recruits motor mechanisms (Pulvermüller 2013). The specificity of action-related difficulties in PD patients is strengthened by their deficits in the nST (which depicted an individual’s bodily actions), alongside preserved AT (and nST) comprehension in bvFTD. Accordingly, this domain may discriminate between PD and nonmotor disorders, as surmised in recent embodied accounts (Birba et al. 2017).

VBM results reinforce the embodied nature of such selective difficulties, which were associated with the volume of core EBA hubs subserving observation and imagery of body parts and their motion (Astafiev et al. 2004). Indeed, damage to these regions in PD (Potgieser et al. 2014) entails motion perception deficits (Weil et al. 2016). Moreover, the association with such posterior areas supports the view that, unlike controls (Pulvermüller 2018), PD patients over-rely on alternative (nonmotor) areas for action–language (Birba et al. 2017) and action–imagery (Helmich et al. 2007) processes.

These impairments did not depend solely on the disruption of motion-related networks, but also on their cross-regional connections. For the AT, frontostriatal hubs comprised within (e.g., SMA; Morris et al. 2016) or projecting to (e.g., GR, PC; Diodato et al. 2016) motor regions subserving action language (Pulvermüller 2013) were distinctively connected with temporo-(para)hippocampal hubs implicated in general semantic (Binder et al. 2009) and motor imagery (Vingerhoets et al. 2002) skills—some of which play secondary roles during action–language processing (Hauk et al. 2004). Compatibly, reduced motor imagery in PD has been associated with connectivity between motor and temporal (EBA) regions (Helmich et al. 2007). Together, these findings endorse the interplay of embodied and multimodal systems for action–language processing in PD.

Additionally, AT outcomes correlated with frontal hd-EEG hypoconnectivity. Crucially, the topography of this cluster (and, partially, its frequency range) has been linked to signatures of action-related processes, like event-related beta desynchronization during object grasping (Ewen et al. 2016); oscillatory abnormalities during motor imagery (Pfurtscheller et al. 2006); and mu-rhythm desynchronization (Vukovic and Shtyrov 2014); and rsFC modulations (Melloni et al. 2015) during action–language processing. Moreover, this pattern was distinctive of PD, as AT outcomes were not associated with the significant hd-EEG cluster of bvFTD. Therefore, hd-EEG signatures of action–discourse outcomes may also constitute nosologically specific markers of PD.

Unlike PD, bvFTD was characterized by selective difficulties in grasping social-related discourse (ST). Conceivably, this deficit stems from broader social cognition impairments in these patients (Piguet et al. 2011), who typically fail at inferring others’ thoughts and interpreting social cues (Hsieh et al. 2012; Ibáñez et al. 2018). Indeed, bvFTD patients exhibit poor faux pas detection (Gregory et al. 2002) and social coordination (McMillan et al. 2012) in text-level tasks, suggesting that social-discourse skills are grounded in more general sociocognitive mechanisms.

In fact, whereas preserved nST outcomes in bvFTD yielded volumetric associations across functionally unspecific sites, ST deficits correlated with core social-network regions. These areas are affected in bvFTD (Piguet et al. 2011), leading to undesirable social behavior (Mychack et al. 2001), theory-of-mind deficits (Gregory et al. 2002), and impaired social-concept processing (Rice and Hoffman 2018). The relation between specific ST deficits and the integrity of these areas further supports the reliance of socially laden language on more basic sociocognitive systems.

Also, whereas nST outcomes yielded no associations with spatial rsFC in bvFTD, ST outcomes correlated with the coupling between frontoamygdalar hubs subserving social cognition (Hesse et al. 2016) and hippocampal/parahippocampal hubs acknowledged by social cognition (Tavares et al. 2015) and multimodal semantic (Binder et al. 2009) models. Given that disrupted frontotemporal connectivity underpins interpersonal coordination outcomes in bvFTD (Melloni et al. 2016), this ST-specific pattern also seems grounded in sociocognitive alterations.

Furthermore, ST outcomes in bvFTD correlated with hd-EEG hypoconnectivity over right frontotemporal sites. Topographically and frequentially similar clusters differentiate bvFTD patients from controls (Dottori et al. 2017) and capture signatures of sociocognitive processes, like delta–theta synchronization during facial emotion recognition (Perdikis et al. 2017) and oscillatory abnormalities during interpersonal cooperation in bvFTD (Melloni et al. 2016). Indeed, atypical frontotemporal oscillations constitute a hallmark of social inappropriateness in this disease (Ibáñez 2018). Since no other group showed frontotemporal hypoconnectivity and selective ST deficits, this pattern seems distinctive of bvFTD.

The nosological specificity of the patterns in PD and bvFTD is supported by results from AD. As in previous works, this group provides a more specific (neurodegenerative) benchmark than healthy participants (Piguet et al. 2011; Melloni et al. 2015; García-Cordero et al. 2016; Melloni et al. 2016). Such patients were outperformed in all texts by every other group, with widespread, unspecific neuroanatomical and spatial connectivity correlates involving all four lobes and the cerebellum. This could reflect pervasive semantic memory, working memory, and long-term declarative memory deficits in AD, related to early temporohippocampal atrophy as well as parietal, insular, and perisylvian degeneration (Whitwell et al. 2012). Moreover, AD patients exhibited hd-EEG hyperconnectivity within a temporooccipital cluster, which correlated negatively with performance in three texts and nearly reached significance for the remaining one. Compatibly, posterior hyperconnectivity in AD (Knyazeva et al. 2010) correlates negatively with domain-general cognitive skills (Knyazeva et al. 2010) underlying processing of any stimulus type.

Although results from the AD sample prove less informative because of their nonselectivity, they emphasize the disease specificity of the patterns observed in PD and bvFTD. Moreover, this contrast reinforces the sensitivity of embodied deficits for disease stages that spare higher-order domains, given that the AD patients were more cognitively impaired (even in mnesic aspects). Indeed, previous evidence indicates that action semantic deficits are selective in cognitively preserved PD patients, but that they are accompanied by general semantic difficulties in cognitively impaired patients (Bocanegra et al. 2017; García et al. 2018). Briefly, the distinctively nonselective nature of these patterns verifies the specificity of the multidimensional signatures observed in PD and bvFTD.

Yet, although our task reveals differential patterns of deficit for each patient group relative to HCs, direct comparisons among the patient groups only revealed significantly lower performance across texts for AD than both PD and bvFTD, without discriminating between the latter two groups. Potentially, this might reflect the fact that nonprimary but moderate alterations in action semantics and social semantics may occasionally emerge in bvFTD (Silveri et al. 2003; Cotelli et al. 2006) and PD (Baez et al. 2020), respectively. Nevertheless, note that behavioral performance on the AT for PD and the ST for bvFTD was selectively correlated with anatomofunctional alterations in each group. Though indirectly, this attests to the potential disease discriminability of the deficits observed in each disorder, although more specific studies with more items, and more controlled texts, would be necessary to better address this issue.

Our findings bear clinical implications. Most linguistic assessments target general, isolated aspects of phonology, morphosyntax or lexicosemantics, which often reveal similar deficits in PD, bvFTD, and AD (McMillan et al. 2012; García et al. 2018). Though certainly useful to inform disease characterization and delineate intervention areas, these approaches rarely reveal fine-grained signatures that could differentiate among disorders. Our framework seems to circumvent this limitation employing context-rich, cohesive, coherent narratives that capture key properties of everyday discourse and target sensitive embodied domains (Halliday et al. 2014).

Limitations and Avenues for Further Research

Yet, our work features some limitations. First, the patient samples had moderate sizes. Nevertheless, we matched or surpassed the sample size of other robust multimodal studies (García-Cordero et al. 2016; Melloni et al. 2016), with a strict control clinical variables, detailed diagnostic procedures, systematic assessments, and power estimations that corroborated the adequacy of our Ns. Looking forward, however, this approach should be tested with more participants per site to tackle novel questions hinging on cross-center comparisons. Second, while we focused on patients with relatively preserved cognitive status, our protocol could be run on bvFTD and PD patients with more advanced cognitive impairment. This would be useful to establish whether observed deficits remain selective under higher cognitive dysfunction thresholds. Third, testing items were limited to 14 questions per text. Although previous versions of this paradigm yielded robust condition-specific effects with similar numbers of trials (García et al. 2018), future studies should increase this figure. Fourth, given the multimodal nature of our work, we only focused on specific, hypothesis-driven metrics for each neuroscientific technique. However, future studies could implement different measures, such as cortical thickness or surface-based morphometry for structural imaging (Fischl 2012), weighted symbolic dependence metric for fMRI (Moguilner et al. 2018), and weighed symbolic phase index for hd-EEG (Imperatori et al. 2019). In this sense, our hd-EEG analyses targeted hypothesis-driven frequencies known to capture distinct patterns in neurodegenerative diseases (Melloni et al. 2015; Dottori et al. 2017) and embodied language domains (Melloni et al. 2015; García et al. 2020). Still, future studies could explore other frequency ranges and additional connectivity features, such as network distance—as in (García-Cordero et al. 2016). Finally, although our findings provide a multimodal picture across behavioral, anatomic, electrophysiological, and FC dimensions, all brain–behavior correlations were based on offline neural measures. Further works should replicate this protocol with online recordings during text-level processing, as in Birba et al. (2020).

Conclusion

In conclusion, the potential of multidimensional approaches to discriminate among neurodegenerative diseases can be boosted through the use of simple, theoretically driven naturalistic tasks. As shown here, deliberate semantic manipulations in ecological narratives can reveal behavioral, neuroanatomical, and neurofunctional signatures of PD and bvFTD relative to controls and AD patients. This study thus lays the groundwork for establishing discriminatory embodied markers across neurodegenerative subtypes.

Author contributions

Agustina Birba: Methodology, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Writing—original draft preparation, Visualization. Sol Fittipaldi: Software, Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing—original draft preparation, Visualization. Judith C. Cediel Escobar: Formal analysis, Data curation. Cecilia Gonzalez Campo: Software, Formal analysis, Data curation, Visualization. Agustina Legaz: Formal analysis, Data curation. Agostina Galiani: Formal analysis, Data curation. Mariano N. Díaz Rivera: Data curation, Writing—review & editing. Miquel Martorell Caro: Resources, Data curation. Florencia Alifano: Resources, Data curation. Stefanie D. Piña-Escudero: Resources, Data curation. Juan Felipe Cardona: Resources, Data curation. Alejandra Neely: Resources, Data curation. Gonzalo Forno: Resources, Data curation. Mariela Carpinella: Resources, Data curation. Andrea Slachevsky: Resources, Data curation. Cecilia Serrano: Resources, Data curation. Lucas Sedeño: Investigation, Writing—review & editing. Agustín Ibáñez: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources; Writing—review & editing, Project administration, Funding acquisition. Adolfo M. García: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing—original draft preparation, Writing—review & editing, Visualization, Supervision.

Data Availability

All experimental data and the scripts used are available online via the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/vb4tf/?view_only=ced6492ca7d749a996733b213d82d9b2 (data from “Discourse markers of neurodegeneration”).

Funding

This work was supported by CONICET and FONCYT-PICT [grant numbers 2017-1818, 2017-1820]. Andrea Slachevsky is supported by grants from ANID, FONDAP [15150012]; ANID, FONDEF [18I10113]; and ANID, FONDECYT [1191726]. Agustín Ibáñez is supported by grants of Alzheimer's Association GBHI ALZ UK-20-639295; Takeda CW2680521; ANIDANID/FONDECYT Regular (1210195); ANID/FONDAP 15150012, Sistema General de Regalías [BPIN2018000100059], Universidad del Valle [CI 5316], and the Multi-Partner Consortium to Expand Dementia Research in Latin America (ReDLat), funded by the National Institutes of Aging (NIA) of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under award number R01AG057234, an Alzheimer's Association grant (SG-20-725707-ReDLat), the Rainwater Foundation, and the Global Brain Health Institute. Adolfo García is an Atlantic Fellow at the Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI) and is supported with funding from GBHI, Alzheimer's Association, and Alzheimer's Society (Alzheimer's Association GBHI ALZ UK-22-865742); ANID, FONDECYT Regular [1210176]; and Programa Interdisciplinario de Investigación Experimental en Comunicación y Cognición (PIIECC), Facultad de Humanidades, USACH. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health, Alzheimer's Association, Rainwater Charitable Foundation, or Global Brain Health Institute.

Notes

We extend our deepest gratitude to all patients and their families, who disinterestedly offered their valuable time to the study. We thankfully acknowledge the collaboration of Instituto Conci Carpinella (Córdoba, Argentina) and Hospital Nacional de Clínicas (Facultad de Ciencias Médicas, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, Argentina). Conflict of Interest: The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

Arvanitakis
Z
,
Shah
RC
,
Bennett
DA
.
2019
.
Diagnosis and management of dementia: review
.
JAMA
.
322
:
1589
1599
.

Astafiev
SV
,
Stanley
CM
,
Shulman
GL
,
Corbetta
M
.
2004
.
Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions
.
Nat Neurosci
.
7
:
542
548
.

Aviezer
H
,
Bentin
S
,
Hassin
RR
,
Meschino
WS
,
Kennedy
J
,
Grewal
S
,
Esmail
S
,
Cohen
S
,
Moscovitch
M
.
2009
.
Not on the face alone: perception of contextualized face expressions in Huntington's disease
.
Brain
.
132
:
1633
1644
.

Baez
S
,
Herrera
E
,
Trujillo
C
,
Cardona
JF
,
Diazgranados
JA
,
Pino
M
,
Santamaría-García
H
,
Ibáñez
A
,
García AMJFian.
2020
.
Classifying Parkinson’s disease patients with syntactic and socio-emotional verbal measures
.
Front Aging Neurosci
.
12
:
586233
.

Baez
S
,
Manes
F
,
Huepe
D
,
Torralva
T
,
Fiorentino
N
,
Richter
F
,
Huepe-Artigas
D
,
Ferrari
J
,
Montañes
P
,
Reyes
P
, et al.
2014
.
Primary empathy deficits in frontotemporal dementia
.
Front Aging Neurosci
.
6
:
262
.

Barrio-Cantalejo
IM
,
Simón-Lorda
P
,
Melguizo
M
,
Escalona
I
,
Marijuán
MI
,
Hernando
P
.
2008
.
Validación de la Escala INFLESZ Para evaluar la legibilidad de los textos dirigidos a pacientes
.
An Sist Sanit Navar
.
31
:
135
152
.

Binder
JR
,
Desai
RH
,
Graves
WW
,
Conant
LL
.
2009
.
Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies
.
Cereb Cortex
.
19
:
2767
2796
.

Birba
A
,
Beltrán
D
,
Caro
MM
,
Trevisan
P
,
Kogan
B
,
Sedeño
L
,
Ibáñez
A
,
García
AM
.
2020
.
Motor-system dynamics during naturalistic reading of action narratives in first and second language
.
NeuroImage
.
216
:
116820
.

Birba
A
,
García-Cordero
I
,
Kozono
G
,
Legaz
A
,
Ibáñez
A
,
Sedeño
L
,
García
AM
.
2017
.
Losing ground: frontostriatal atrophy disrupts language embodiment in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
80
:
673
687
.

Bocanegra
Y
,
García
AM
,
Lopera
F
,
Pineda
D
,
Baena
A
,
Ospina
P
,
Alzate
D
,
Buriticá
O
,
Moreno
L
,
Ibáñez
A
, et al.
2017
.
Unspeakable motion: selective action-verb impairments in Parkinson’s disease patients without mild cognitive impairment
.
Brain Lang
.
168
:
37
46
.

Bocanegra
Y
,
García
AM
,
Pineda
D
,
Buriticá
O
,
Villegas
A
,
Lopera
F
,
Gómez
D
,
Arias
C
,
Cardona
J
,
Trujillo
N
, et al.
2015
.
Syntax, action verbs, action semantics, and object semantics in Parkinson's disease: dissociability, progression, and executive influences
.
Cortex
.
69
:
237
254
.

Boulenger
V
,
Mechtouff
L
,
Thobois
S
,
Broussolle
E
,
Jeannerod
M
,
Nazir
TA
.
2008
.
Word processing in Parkinson's disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns
.
Neuropsychologia
.
46
:
743
756
.

Cervetto
S
,
Díaz-Rivera
M
,
Petroni
A
,
Birba
A
,
Martorell Caro
M
,
Sedeño
L
,
Ibáñez
A
,
García
AM
.
2021
.
The neural blending of words and movement: ERP signatures of semantic and action processes during motor-language coupling
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
33
(
8
):
1413
1427
.

Cotelli
M
,
Borroni
B
,
Manenti
R
,
Alberici
A
,
Calabria
M
,
Agosti
C
,
Arévalo
A
,
Ginex
V
,
Ortelli
P
,
Binetti
GJN
.
2006
.
Action and object naming in frontotemporal dementia, progressive supranuclear palsy, and corticobasal degeneration
.
Neuropsycholgoy
.
20
(
5
):
558
565
.

Davis
CJ
,
Perea
M
.
2005
.
BuscaPalabras: a program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish
.
Behav Res Methods
.
37
:
665
671
.

Delgado C, Araneda A, Behrens MI.

2019
.
Validación del instrumento Montreal Cognitive Assessment en español en adultos mayores de 60 años
.
Neurología
.
34
:
376
385
.

de la
Fuente
A
,
Sedeño
L
,
Vignaga
SS
,
Ellmann
C
,
Sonzogni
S
,
Belluscio
L
,
García-Cordero
I
,
Castagnaro
E
,
Boano
M
,
Cetkovich
M
, et al.
2019
.
Multimodal neurocognitive markers of interoceptive tuning in smoked cocaine
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
.
44
:
1425
1434
.

Diodato
A
,
De Brimont
MR
,
Yim
YS
,
Derian
N
,
Perrin
S
,
Pouch
J
,
Klatzmann
D
,
Garel
S
,
Choi
GB
,
Fleischmann
A
.
2016
.
Molecular signatures of neural connectivity in the olfactory cortex
.
Nat Commun
.
7
:
1
10
.

Donnelly-Kehoe
PA
,
Pascariello
GO
,
García
AM
,
Hodges
JR
,
Miller
B
,
Rosen
H
,
Manes
F
,
Landin-Romero
R
,
Matallana
D
,
Serrano
C
, et al.
2019
.
Robust automated computational approach for classifying frontotemporal neurodegeneration: multimodal/multicenter neuroimaging
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
11
:
588
598
.

Dottori
M
,
Sedeño
L
,
Caro
MM
,
Alifano
F
,
Hesse
E
,
Mikulan
E
,
García
AM
,
Ruiz-Tagle
A
,
Lillo
P
,
Slachevsky
A
, et al.
2017
.
Towards affordable biomarkers of frontotemporal dementia: a classification study via network’s information sharing
.
Sci Rep
.
7
:
3822
.

Ewen
JB
,
Lakshmanan
BM
,
Pillai
AS
,
McAuliffe
D
,
Nettles
C
,
Hallett
M
,
Crone
NE
,
Mostofsky
SH
.
2016
.
Decreased modulation of EEG oscillations in high-functioning autism during a motor control task
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
10
:
198
.

Fernandino
L
,
Conant
LL
,
Binder
JR
,
Blindauer
K
,
Hiner
B
,
Spangler
K
,
Desai
RHJB
, language.
2013
.
Parkinson’s disease disrupts both automatic and controlled processing of action verbs
.
Brain Lang
.
127
:
65
74
.

Fischl
B
.
2012
.
FreeSurfer
.
NeuroImage
.
62
:
774
781
.

Gallese
V
,
Cuccio
V
.
2018
.
The neural exploitation hypothesis and its implications for an embodied approach to language and cognition: insights from the study of action verbs processing and motor disorders in Parkinson's disease
.
Cortex
.
100
:
215
225
.

García-Cordero
I
,
Sedeño
L
,
de la
Fuente
L
,
Slachevsky
A
,
Forno
G
,
Klein
F
,
Lillo
P
,
Ferrari
J
,
Rodriguez
C
,
Bustin
J
, et al.
2016
.
Feeling, learning from and being aware of inner states: interoceptive dimensions in neurodegeneration and stroke
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci
.
371
(
1708
):
20160006
.

García
AM
,
Bocanegra
Y
,
Herrera
E
,
Moreno
L
,
Carmona
J
,
Baena
A
,
Lopera
F
,
Pineda
D
,
Melloni
M
,
Legaz
A
, et al.
2018
.
Parkinson's disease compromises the appraisal of action meanings evoked by naturalistic texts
.
Cortex
.
100
:
111
126
.

García
AM
,
Hesse
E
,
Birba
A
,
Adolfi
F
,
Mikulan
E
,
Caro
MM
,
Petroni
A
,
Bekinschtein
TA
,
del
Carmen
GM
,
Silva
W
, et al.
2020
.
Time to face language: embodied mechanisms underpin the inception of face-related meanings in the human brain
.
Cereb Cortex
.
30
:
6051
6068
.

García
AM
,
Sedeño
L
,
Trujillo
N
,
Bocanegra
Y
,
Gomez
D
,
Pineda
D
,
Villegas
A
,
Muñoz
E
,
Arias
W
,
Ibáñez
A
.
2017
.
Language deficits as a preclinical window into Parkinson’s disease: evidence from asymptomatic parkin and dardarin mutation carriers
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc
.
23
:
150
158
.

Geraudie
A
,
Rivera
MD
,
Montembeault
M
,
García
AM
.
2021
.
Language in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: another stone to be turned in Latin America
.
Front Neurol
.
12
:
702770
.

Gregory
C
,
Lough
S
,
Stone
V
,
Erzinclioglu
S
,
Martin
L
,
Baron-Cohen
S
,
Hodges
JR
.
2002
.
Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: theoretical and practical implications
.
Brain
.
125
:
752
764
.

Halliday
MAK
,
Matthiessen
C
,
Halliday
M
.
2014
.
An introduction to functional grammar
.
London
:
Routledge
.

Hardy
CJ
,
Buckley
AH
,
Downey
LE
,
Lehmann
M
,
Zimmerer
VC
,
Varley
RA
,
Crutch
SJ
,
Rohrer
JD
,
Warrington
EK
,
Warren
JD
.
2016
.
The language profile of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia
.
J Alzheimers Dis
.
50
:
359
371
.

Hasson
U
,
Egidi
G
,
Marelli
M
,
Willems
RM
.
2018
.
Grounding the neurobiology of language in first principles: the necessity of non-language-centric explanations for language comprehension
.
Cognition
.
180
:
135
157
.

Hauk
O
,
Johnsrude
I
,
Pulvermüller
F
.
2004
.
Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex
.
Neuron
.
41
:
301
307
.

Helmich
RC
,
de
Lange
FP
,
Bloem
BR
,
Toni
I
.
2007
.
Cerebral compensation during motor imagery in Parkinson's disease
.
Neuropsychologia
.
45
:
2201
2215
.

Helmich
RC
,
Hallett
M
,
Deuschl
G
,
Toni
I
,
Bloem
BR
.
2012
.
Cerebral causes and consequences of parkinsonian resting tremor: a tale of two circuits?
Brain
.
135
:
3206
3226
.

Hesse
E
,
Mikulan
E
,
Decety
J
,
Sigman
M
,
García
MC
,
Silva
W
,
Ciraolo
C
,
Vaucheret
E
,
Baglivo
F
,
Huepe
D
.
2016
.
Early detection of intentional harm in the human amygdala
.
Brain
.
139
:
54
61
.

Hsieh
S
,
Foxe
D
,
Leslie
F
,
Savage
S
,
Piguet
O
,
Hodges
JR
.
2012
.
Grief and joy: emotion word comprehension in the dementias
.
Neuropsychology
.
26
:
624
630
.

Hughes
AJ
,
Daniel
SE
,
Kilford
L
,
Lees
AJ
.
1992
.
Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
.
55
:
181
184
.

Ibáñez
A
,
García
AM
,
Esteves
S
,
Yoris
A
,
Munoz
E
,
Reynaldo
L
,
Pietto
ML
,
Adolfi
F
,
Manes
F
.
2018
.
Social neuroscience: undoing the schism between neurology and psychiatry
.
Soc Neurosci
.
13
:
1
39
.

Ibáñez
A
.
2018
.
Brain oscillations, inhibition and social inappropriateness in frontotemporal degeneration
.
Brain
.
141
:
73
73
.

Ibáñez
A
,
Billeke
P
,
de la
Fuente
L
,
Salamone
P
,
García
AM
,
Melloni
M
.
2017
.
Reply: towards a neurocomputational account of social dysfunction in neurodegenerative disease
.
Brain
.
140
:
e15
e15
.

Ibáñez
A
,
Fittipaldi
S
,
Trujillo
C
,
Jaramillo
T
,
Torres
A
,
Cardona
JF
,
Rivera
R
,
Slachevsky
A
,
García
A
,
Bertoux
M
, et al.
2021a
.
Predicting and characterizing neurodegenerative subtypes with multimodal neurocognitive signatures of social and cognitive processes
.
J Alzheimers Dis
.
83
(
1
):
227
248
.

Ibáñez
A
,
Pina-Escudero
SD
,
Possin
KL
,
Quiroz
YT
,
Peres
FA
,
Slachevsky
A
,
Sosa
AL
,
Brucki
SM
,
Miller
BL
.
2021b
.
Dementia caregiving across Latin America and the Caribbean and brain health diplomacy
.
Lancet Healthy Longev
.
2
:
e222
e231
.

Ibáñez
A
,
Yokoyama
JS
,
Possin
KL
,
Matallana
DL
,
Lopera
F
,
Nitrini
R
,
Takada
LT
,
Custodio
N
,
Sosa Ortiz
AL
,
Avila-Funes
JA
, et al.
2021c
.
The multi-partner consortium to expand dementia research in Latin America (ReDLat): driving multicentric research and implementation science
.
Front Neurol
.
12
:
303
.

Imperatori
LS
,
Betta
M
,
Cecchetti
L
,
Canales-Johnson
A
,
Ricciardi
E
,
Siclari
F
,
Pietrini
P
,
Chennu
S
,
Bernardi
G
.
2019
.
EEG functional connectivity metrics wPLI and wSMI account for distinct types of brain functional interactions
.
Sci Rep
.
9
:
8894
.

Jabbi
M
,
Bastiaansen
J
,
Keysers
C
.
2008
.
A common anterior insula representation of disgust observation, experience and imagination shows divergent functional connectivity pathways
.
PLoS One
.
3
:
e2939
.

Kanske
P
,
Böckler
A
,
Trautwein
F-M
,
Parianen Lesemann
FH
,
Singer
T
.
2016
.
Are strong empathizers better mentalizers? Evidence for independence and interaction between the routes of social cognition
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
.
11
:
1383
1392
.

Knyazeva
MG
,
Jalili
M
,
Brioschi
A
,
Bourquin
I
,
Fornari
E
,
Hasler
M
,
Meuli
R
,
Maeder
P
,
Ghika
J
.
2010
.
Topography of EEG multivariate phase synchronization in early Alzheimer's disease
.
Neurobiol Aging
.
31
:
1132
1144
.

Ledoux
K
,
Camblin
CC
,
Swaab
TY
,
Gordon
PC
.
2006
.
Reading words in discourse: the modulation of lexical priming effects by message-level context
.
Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev
.
5
:
107
127
.

Legaz
A
,
Abrevaya
S
,
Dottori
M
,
Campo
CG
,
Birba
A
,
Caro
MM
,
Aguirre
J
,
Slachevsky
A
,
Aranguiz
R
,
Serrano
C
, et al.
2021
.
Multimodal mechanisms of human socially reinforced learning across neurodegenerative diseases
.
Brain
.

Lieberman
MD
,
Cunningham
WA
.
2009
.
Type I and type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
.
4
:
423
428
.

Maris
E
,
Oostenveld
R
.
2007
.
Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data
.
J Neurosci Methods
.
164
:
177
190
.

Martínez-Martín P, Rodríguez-Blázquez C, Alvarez M, Arakaki T, Arillo VC, Chaná P, Fernández W, Garretto N, Martínez-Castrillo JC, Rodríguez-Violante M, et al.

2015
.
Parkinson's disease severity levels and MDS-Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale
.
Parkinsonism Relat Disord
.
21
:
50
54
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.10.026

McKhann
GM
,
Knopman
DS
,
Chertkow
H
,
Hyman
BT
,
Jack
CR
Jr
,
Kawas
CH
,
Klunk
WE
,
Koroshetz
WJ
,
Manly
JJ
,
Mayeux
R
, et al.
2011
.
The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
7
:
263
269
.

McMillan
CT
,
Rascovsky
K
,
Khella
MC
,
Clark
R
,
Grossman
M
.
2012
.
The neural basis for establishing a focal point in pure coordination games
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
.
7
:
881
887
.

Melloni
M
,
Billeke
P
,
Baez
S
,
Hesse
E
,
de la
Fuente
L
,
Forno
G
,
Birba
A
,
García-Cordero
I
,
Serrano
C
,
Plastino
A
, et al.
2016
.
Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining
.
Brain
.
139
:
1
19
.

Melloni
M
,
Sedeño
L
,
Hesse
E
,
García-Cordero
I
,
Mikulan
E
,
Plastino
A
,
Marcotti
A
,
López
JD
,
Bustamante
C
,
Lopera
F
, et al.
2015
.
Cortical dynamics and subcortical signatures of motor-language coupling in Parkinson’s disease
.
Sci Rep
.
5
:
11899
.

Moguilner
S
,
Birba
A
,
Fino
D
,
Isoardi
R
,
Huetagoyena
C
,
Otoya
R
,
Tirapu
V
,
Cremaschi
F
,
Sedeño
L
,
Ibáñez
A
, et al.
2021a
.
Structural and functional motor-network disruptions predict selective action-concept deficits: evidence from frontal lobe epilepsy
.
Cortex
.
144
:
43
55
.

Moguilner
S
,
Birba
A
,
Fino
D
,
Isoardi
R
,
Huetagoyena
C
,
Otoya
R
,
Tirapu
V
,
Cremaschi
F
,
Sedeño
L
,
Ibáñez
A
, et al.
2021b
.
Multimodal neurocognitive markers of frontal lobe epilepsy: insights from ecological text processing
.
NeuroImage
.
235
:
117998
.

Moguilner
S
,
García
AM
,
Mikulan
E
,
Hesse
E
,
García-Cordero
I
,
Melloni
M
,
Cervetto
S
,
Serrano
C
,
Herrera
E
,
Reyes
P
, et al.
2018
.
Weighted symbolic dependence metric (wSDM) for fMRI resting-state connectivity: a multicentric validation for frontotemporal dementia
.
Sci Rep
.
8
:
1
15
.

Moguilner
S
,
García
AM
,
Perl
YS
,
Tagliazucchi
E
,
Piguet
O
,
Kumfor
F
,
Reyes
P
,
Matallana
D
,
Sedeño
L
,
Ibáñez
A
.
2021
.
Dynamic brain fluctuations outperform connectivity measures and mirror pathophysiological profiles across dementia subtypes: a multicenter study
.
NeuroImage
.
225
:
117522
.

Morris JC.

1993
.
The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules
.
Neurology
.
43
:2412–2414.

Morris
LS
,
Kundu
P
,
Dowell
N
,
Mechelmans
DJ
,
Favre
P
,
Irvine
MA
,
Robbins
TW
,
Daw
N
,
Bullmore
ET
,
Harrison
NA
.
2016
.
Fronto-striatal organization: defining functional and microstructural substrates of behavioural flexibility
.
Cortex
.
74
:
118
133
.

Mychack
P
,
Kramer
J
,
Boone
K
,
Miller
B
.
2001
.
The influence of right frontotemporal dysfunction on social behavior in frontotemporal dementia
.
Neurology
.
56
:
S11
S15
.

Nasreddine
ZS
,
Phillips
NA
,
Bedirian
V
,
Charbonneau
S
,
Whitehead
V
,
Collin
I
,
Cummings
JL
,
Chertkow
H
.
2005
.
The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
53
:
695
699
.

Nichols
TE
,
Das
S
,
Eickhoff
SB
,
Evans
AC
,
Glatard
T
,
Hanke
M
,
Kriegeskorte
N
,
Milham
MP
,
Poldrack
RA
,
Poline
J-B
.
2017
.
Best practices in data analysis and sharing in neuroimaging using MRI
.
Nat Neurosci
.
20
:
299
.

Péran
P
,
Cardebat
D
,
Cherubini
A
,
Piras
F
,
Luccichenti
G
,
Peppe
A
,
Caltagirone
C
,
Rascol
O
,
Démonet
J-F
,
Sabatini
U
.
2009
.
Object naming and action-verb generation in Parkinson's disease: a fMRI study
.
Cortex
.
45
:
960
971
.

Perdikis
D
,
Volhard
J
,
Müller
V
,
Kaulard
K
,
Brick
TR
,
Wallraven
C
,
Lindenberger
U
.
2017
.
Brain synchronization during perception of facial emotional expressions with natural and unnatural dynamics
.
PLoS One
.
12
:e0181225.

Pfurtscheller
G
,
Brunner
C
,
Schlögl
A
,
Da Silva
FL
.
2006
.
Mu rhythm (de) synchronization and EEG single-trial classification of different motor imagery tasks
.
NeuroImage
.
31
:
153
159
.

Piguet
O
,
Hornberger
M
,
Mioshi
E
,
Hodges
JR
.
2011
.
Behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: diagnosis, clinical staging, and management
.
Lancet Neurol
.
10
:
162
172
.

Potgieser
AR
,
van der
Hoorn
A
,
Meppelink
AM
,
Teune
LK
,
Koerts
J
,
de
Jong
BM
.
2014
.
Anterior temporal atrophy and posterior progression in patients with Parkinson's disease
.
J Neurodegener Dis
.
14
:
125
132
.

Pulvermüller
F
.
2013
.
How neurons make meaning: brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
17
:
458
470
.

Pulvermüller
F
.
2018
.
Neural reuse of action perception circuits for language, concepts and communication
.
Prog Neurobiol
.
160
:
1
44
.

Rascovsky
K
,
Hodges
JR
,
Knopman
D
,
Mendez
MF
,
Kramer
JH
,
Neuhaus
J
,
Van Swieten
JC
,
Seelaar
H
,
Dopper
EGP
,
Onyike
CU
, others.
2011
.
Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia
.
Brain
134
:
2456
2477
.

Rice
GE
,
Hoffman
P
.
2018
.
Concrete versus abstract forms of social concept: an fMRI comparison of knowledge about people versus social terms
.
Philos Trans R Soc B
.
373
:20170136.

Salamone
PC
,
Legaz
A
,
Sedeño
L
,
Moguilner
S
,
Fraile-Vazquez
M
,
Campo
CG
,
Fittipaldi
S
,
Yoris
A
,
Miranda
M
,
Birba
A
, et al.
2021
.
Interoception primes emotional processing: multimodal evidence from neurodegeneration
.
J Neurosci
.
41
:
4276
4292
.

Santamaria-García
H
,
Baez
S
,
Reyes
P
,
Santamaria-García
JA
,
Santacruz-Escudero
JM
,
Matallana
D
,
Arevalo
A
,
Sigman
M
,
García
AM
,
Ibáñez
A
.
2017
.
A lesion model of envy and schadenfreude: legal, deservingness and moral dimensions as revealed by neurodegeneration
.
Brain
.
140
:
3357
3377
.

Sedeño
L
,
Piguet
O
,
Abrevaya
S
,
Desmaras
H
,
García-Cordero
I
,
Baez
S
,
Alethia de la Fuente
L
,
Reyes
P
,
Tu
S
,
Moguilner
S
, et al.
2017
.
Tackling variability: a multicenter study to provide a gold-standard network approach for frontotemporal dementia
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
38
:
3804
3822
.

Silveri
MC
,
Salvigni
BL
,
Cappa
A
,
Della Vedova
C
,
Puopolo
M
.
2003
.
Impairment of verb processing in frontal variant-frontotemporal dementia: a dysexecutive symptom
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
.
16
:
296
300
.

Szigrist
PF
.
1993
.
Sistemas de legibilidad del mensaje escrito: fómula de perspicuidad
.
Madird
:
Universidad Complutense de Madrid
.

Tavares
RM
,
Mendelsohn
A
,
Grossman
Y
,
Williams
CH
,
Shapiro
M
,
Trope
Y
,
Schiller
D
.
2015
.
A map for social navigation in the human brain
.
Neuron
.
87
:
231
243
.

Torralva
T
,
Roca
M
,
Gleichgerrcht
E
,
Lopez
P
,
Manes
F
.
2009
.
INECO frontal screening (IFS): a brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc
.
15
:
777
786
.

Trevisan
P
,
García
AM
.
2019
.
Systemic functional grammar as a tool for experimental stimulus design: new appliable horizons in psycholinguistics and neurolinguistics
.
Lang Sci
.
75
:
35
46
.

Van Dam
WO
,
Rueschemeyer
S-A
,
Lindemann
O
,
Bekkering
H
.
2010
.
Context effects in embodied lexical-semantic processing
.
Front Psychol
.
1
:
150
.

Vingerhoets
G
,
De Lange
FP
,
Vandemaele
P
,
Deblaere
K
,
Achten
E
.
2002
.
Motor imagery in mental rotation: an fMRI study
.
NeuroImage
.
17
:
1623
1633
.

Vukovic
N
,
Shtyrov
Y
.
2014
.
Cortical motor systems are involved in second-language comprehension: evidence from rapid mu-rhythm desynchronisation
.
NeuroImage
.
102
:
695
703
.

Weil
RS
,
Schrag
AE
,
Warren
JD
,
Crutch
SJ
,
Lees
AJ
,
Morris
HR
.
2016
.
Visual dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease
.
Brain
.
139
:
2827
2843
.

Whitwell
JL
,
Dickson
DW
,
Murray
ME
,
Weigand
SD
,
Tosakulwong
N
,
Senjem
ML
,
Knopman
DS
,
Boeve
BF
,
Parisi
JE
,
Petersen
RC
, et al.
2012
.
Neuroimaging correlates of pathologically defined subtypes of Alzheimer's disease: a case-control study
.
Lancet Neurol
.
11
:
868
877
.

Woo
C-W
,
Krishnan
A
,
Wager
TD
.
2014
.
Cluster-extent based thresholding in fMRI analyses: pitfalls and recommendations
.
NeuroImage
.
91
:
412
419
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)

Supplementary data