Abstract

Neuropsychological disturbances in the sense of limb ownership provide unique opportunities to study the neurocognitive basis of body ownership. Previous small sample studies that showed discrete cortical lesions cannot explain why multisensory, affective, and cognitive manipulations alter disownership symptoms. We tested the novel hypothesis that disturbances in the sense of limb ownership would be associated not only with discrete cortical lesions but also with disconnections of white-matter tracts supporting specific functional networks. We drew on an advanced lesion-analysis and Bayesian statistics approach in 49 right-hemisphere patients (23 with and 26 without limb disownership). Our results reveal that disturbances in the sense of ownership are associated with lesions in the supramarginal gyrus and disconnections of a fronto-insular-parietal network, involving the frontal-insular and frontal inferior longitudinal tracts, confirming previous disconnection hypotheses. Together with previous behavioral and neuroanatomical results, these findings lead us to propose that the sense of body ownership involves the convergence of bottom-up, multisensory integration, and top-down monitoring of sensory salience based on contextual demands.

Introduction

A fundamental aspect of self-consciousness is the sense of body ownership (SBO) or the experience of one’s physical body as belonging to the unitary self (Gallagher 2000). The SBO is considered distinct from the attribution of body parts to a conceptual self (Neisser and Jopling 1994) typically studied by projecting one’s body parts as images, or on video displays and testing their self-recognition in extrapersonal space (e.g. Saxe et al. 2006). A different tradition of studies has investigated the SBO as a more intuitive and less doxastic sense of the body (Tsakiris 2010; de Vignemont 2011) using experimental manipulations to identify the mechanisms by which sensations are combined to give rise to the SBO in different temporal, spatial, and affective conditions (Tsakiris 2010; Blanke et al. 2015; Kilteni et al. 2015; Crucianelli et al. 2019). In this tradition, the SBO is regarded less as a fixed mental representation of the body in the brain and more as a dynamic process of multisensory integration, subject to both learned top-down factors and bottom-up sensory signals. Consistently with these insights, functional neuroimaging studies have shown that the SBO is supported by a network of multimodal areas, composed of premotor, temporoparietal, and occipital areas, as well as the insular cortex (Blanke et al. 2015; Tsakiris 2017 for reviews). Early stroke lesion work pointed to the insular cortex as a main component in the sensation of ownership of own extremities (e.g. Cereda et al. 2002; Baier and Karnath 2008). The functional role of the insula in the SBO seems to relate both to its contribution to the salience network of the brain (Menon and Uddin 2010) and to its fundamental role in the central processing of interoceptive signals from the body (Craig 2009). Indeed, several studies have shown that the degree to which certain sensations are integrated to give rise to the SBO relies on their salience and reliability in given spatial, temporal, and even affective and social contexts (Apps and Tsakiris 2014; Fotopoulou 2015; Northoff 2016; Qin et al. 2020). Moreover, it has been increasingly understood that the SBO not only involves the integration of exteroceptive modalities such as vision and touch but also interoceptive modalities such as cardiac and respiratory systems (Walla et al. 2008; Monti et al. 2020; Azzalini et al. 2021), as well as affective touch (Jenkinson et al. 2020). Finally, in accordance to more general computational insights about dynamic brain functions, the SBO has been recently understood as the consequence of probabilistic inferences of the most likely cause of one’s multisensory experience (Fotopoulou et al. 2011; Apps and Tsakiris 2014; Fotopoulou 2015; Samad et al. 2015).

Despite progress in understanding the SBO through such experimental and computational efforts, it remains difficult to tease apart the phenomenally elusive components of body ownership (Longo et al. 2009; de Vignemont 2011). In contrast, neurological patients with symptoms such as asomatognosia (i.e. the lack of recognition regarding the existence or ownership of one’s limbs, Vallar and Ronchi 2009) show a clear and long-lasting, subjective experience of body disownership, denying ownership of their affected body parts (Jenkinson et al. 2018). Sometimes, this denial is accompanied by delusions about the affected arm (i.e. somatoparaphrenia; Gerstmann 1942) and it may take several clinical forms (Jenkinson et al. 2018; see Vallar and Ronchi 2009; Romano and Maravita 2019 for reviews). The term “disturbed sensation of limb ownership” (DSO; Baier and Karnath 2008; Martinaud et al. 2017; Jenkinson et al. 2018) captures any behavioral manifestation that involves abnormal feelings and beliefs regarding the recognition, ownership, and sense of belonging of ones’ limbs in the first person and in peripersonal space.

DSO has been studied primarily via small samples and in standard symptom-lesion mapping analyses (Feinberg et al. 2010; Invernizzi et al. 2013; see Vallar and Ronchi 2009; Gandola et al. 2012; Romano and Maravita 2019 for discussions) with the co-occurrent neuropsychological symptoms being considered in the clinical comparison between DSO and control patients but not directly analyzed as covariate during the anatomical investigation (Gandola et al. 2012; Invernizzi et al. 2013; Moro et al. 2016). As such, the neuroanatomical correlates of DSO are not clearly distinguished from those accounted for by concurrent symptoms such as neglect or anosognosia for hemiplegia (AHP). Furthermore, although these studies were not focused on specific regions of interest and investigated the whole brain, the results identified discreet cortical lesions to areas such as the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) or the ventral prefrontal cortex (Moro et al. 2016) or the insula (Baier and Karnath 2008), thereby encouraging modular explanations of SBO. However, recent studies show that symptoms cannot be explained by discrete lesions as they are subject to modulation by multisensory, affective, and cognitive manipulations (Fotopoulou et al. 2011; Zeller et al. 2011; Jenkinson et al. 2013, 2020; Romano et al. 2014; Salvato et al. 2016; Martinaud et al. 2017; Romano and Maravita 2019) and linked to damage to wider, cortical networks and white matter tracts (Feinberg et al. 2010; Gandola et al. 2012; Invernizzi et al. 2013; Moro et al. 2016). These observations have led some authors to suggest the original hypothesis that lesions prevent the processing and the integration of different afferent information arising from the affected body part (bottom-up processes) with higher order and pre-existing body representations normally computed in higher order cortices. However, this disconnection hypothesis has not been investigated beyond the aforementioned symptom lesion-mapping methods. In this study, we aimed to examine this hypothesis using advanced methods for identification of gray and white matter damage (Thiebaut De Schotten et al. 2014, 2015; Foulon et al. 2018; Fox 2018; Pacella et al. 2019; Monai et al. 2020) and controlling for other neuropsychological symptoms.

Based on the insights of the aforementioned studies on the disconnection hypothesis of DSO and our recent understanding of the disconnections underlying another right hemisphere syndrome, namely AHP (Pacella et al. 2019; Klingbeil et al. 2020; Kirsch et al. 2021), we predicted that DSO would be associated with damage not only to discrete frontal, parietal, and insular cortical regions but also with disconnections of fronto-parietal (superior longitudinal fasciculus) and fronto-insular white matter tracts supporting functional networks for multisensory integration and body awareness (Pacella et al. 2019).

To study this hypothesis and overcome the aforementioned methodological limitations in the field, we drew on a relatively large cohort of right hemisphere stroke patients (n = 135), from which we selected patients with a reliable diagnosis of DSO (n = 23; DSO+ group) and compared them to a group of control patients, matched on all key demographic and clinical variables, apart from a complete absence of DSO (n = 26; DSO− group; see Table 1). To our knowledge, this is the largest sample of patients with DSO ever studied. First, we used an advanced lesion analysis method (BCBtoolkit; Foulon et al. 2018), which generates a probabilistic map of disconnections from each patient’s brain lesion to identify the disconnections that are associated with given neuropsychological deficits at the group level, and has been successfully used to examine the brain disconnections implicated in several other neuropsychological disorders (Thiebaut De Schotten et al. 2014, 2015; Fox 2018; Pacella et al. 2019; Monai et al. 2020). Second, in order to understand the contribution of each single lesion (gray matter) or tract disconnection in the syndrome, beyond the potential effects of the clinical variables, we used advanced Bayesian modeling comparisons to contrast each single lesion site (gray matter) or tract disconnection against clinical variables alone (Hartigan and Wong 1979; Kaufman and Rousseeuw 1990). Finally, we applied cluster analyses on the resultant lesion patterns, to identify whether the gray and white matter lesions associated with DSO symptoms can be classified in different networks of brain areas and to determine the degree of gray matter damage and probability of damage to white matter connections in each area of each network.

Table 1

Results of the clinical and neuropsychological assessment for DSO and control groups.

DSO (n = 23)Controls (n = 26)BF10b
Age (years)66.22 ± 15.33a67.42 ± 13.260.29H0,*
Education (years)11.43 ± 3.510.43 ± 3.460.12H0
Interval (days)32.17 ± 43.3528.62 ± 41.520.59
Lesion Size (n. voxels)166833.57 ± 115216.26117802.18 ± 122448.570.01H0
DSO (range 0–6)4.49 ± 0.730.0 ± 0.0>150H1
MRC-UUL0.0 ± 0.00.0 ± 0.0Scores are equal H0
Proprioception (max = 9)4.96 ± 2.794.65 ± 3.010.02H0
Bisiach scale (max =3)1.63 ± 1.141.67 ± 1.250.03H0
Comb (right bias, cut-off = −0.11)−0.39 ± 0.32−0.23 ± 0.410.02H0
Line crossing (max = 36)19.91 ± 11.2621.19 ± 13.240.27H0
Digit span backward3 ± 1.543.46 ± 1.70.06H0
DSO (n = 23)Controls (n = 26)BF10b
Age (years)66.22 ± 15.33a67.42 ± 13.260.29H0,*
Education (years)11.43 ± 3.510.43 ± 3.460.12H0
Interval (days)32.17 ± 43.3528.62 ± 41.520.59
Lesion Size (n. voxels)166833.57 ± 115216.26117802.18 ± 122448.570.01H0
DSO (range 0–6)4.49 ± 0.730.0 ± 0.0>150H1
MRC-UUL0.0 ± 0.00.0 ± 0.0Scores are equal H0
Proprioception (max = 9)4.96 ± 2.794.65 ± 3.010.02H0
Bisiach scale (max =3)1.63 ± 1.141.67 ± 1.250.03H0
Comb (right bias, cut-off = −0.11)−0.39 ± 0.32−0.23 ± 0.410.02H0
Line crossing (max = 36)19.91 ± 11.2621.19 ± 13.240.27H0
Digit span backward3 ± 1.543.46 ± 1.70.06H0

aMean ± standard deviation of demographic and clinical variables, neurological, and neuropsychological assessments are reported.

bBayes Factors (BF10) were calculated, with BF10 > 3 = alternative hypothesis (there are differences between the groups); a BF10 < 1/3 = null hypothesis (no differences between groups); results within 3 and 1/3 are inconclusive. All comparisons show no differences between the two groups (BF10 < 1/3, acceptance of the null hypothesis), with one exception (indicated in italic within the table) in the Interval since lesion onset in days. In this latter case, BF10 does not allow to accept the null hypothesis, but it represents anecdotal evidence toward the null hypothesis36.

cH1 = the scores between the two groups are different; H0 = the scores between the two groups are equal.

Table 1

Results of the clinical and neuropsychological assessment for DSO and control groups.

DSO (n = 23)Controls (n = 26)BF10b
Age (years)66.22 ± 15.33a67.42 ± 13.260.29H0,*
Education (years)11.43 ± 3.510.43 ± 3.460.12H0
Interval (days)32.17 ± 43.3528.62 ± 41.520.59
Lesion Size (n. voxels)166833.57 ± 115216.26117802.18 ± 122448.570.01H0
DSO (range 0–6)4.49 ± 0.730.0 ± 0.0>150H1
MRC-UUL0.0 ± 0.00.0 ± 0.0Scores are equal H0
Proprioception (max = 9)4.96 ± 2.794.65 ± 3.010.02H0
Bisiach scale (max =3)1.63 ± 1.141.67 ± 1.250.03H0
Comb (right bias, cut-off = −0.11)−0.39 ± 0.32−0.23 ± 0.410.02H0
Line crossing (max = 36)19.91 ± 11.2621.19 ± 13.240.27H0
Digit span backward3 ± 1.543.46 ± 1.70.06H0
DSO (n = 23)Controls (n = 26)BF10b
Age (years)66.22 ± 15.33a67.42 ± 13.260.29H0,*
Education (years)11.43 ± 3.510.43 ± 3.460.12H0
Interval (days)32.17 ± 43.3528.62 ± 41.520.59
Lesion Size (n. voxels)166833.57 ± 115216.26117802.18 ± 122448.570.01H0
DSO (range 0–6)4.49 ± 0.730.0 ± 0.0>150H1
MRC-UUL0.0 ± 0.00.0 ± 0.0Scores are equal H0
Proprioception (max = 9)4.96 ± 2.794.65 ± 3.010.02H0
Bisiach scale (max =3)1.63 ± 1.141.67 ± 1.250.03H0
Comb (right bias, cut-off = −0.11)−0.39 ± 0.32−0.23 ± 0.410.02H0
Line crossing (max = 36)19.91 ± 11.2621.19 ± 13.240.27H0
Digit span backward3 ± 1.543.46 ± 1.70.06H0

aMean ± standard deviation of demographic and clinical variables, neurological, and neuropsychological assessments are reported.

bBayes Factors (BF10) were calculated, with BF10 > 3 = alternative hypothesis (there are differences between the groups); a BF10 < 1/3 = null hypothesis (no differences between groups); results within 3 and 1/3 are inconclusive. All comparisons show no differences between the two groups (BF10 < 1/3, acceptance of the null hypothesis), with one exception (indicated in italic within the table) in the Interval since lesion onset in days. In this latter case, BF10 does not allow to accept the null hypothesis, but it represents anecdotal evidence toward the null hypothesis36.

cH1 = the scores between the two groups are different; H0 = the scores between the two groups are equal.

Materials and methods

Patients

To explore the neural correlates of the sense of body (dis)ownership, a consecutive sample of 135 right hemisphere stroke patients, with dense left-sided hemiplegia, were screened at 2 stroke units located in Italy (Rehabilitation Department, IRCSS Sacro Cuore Hospital in Negrar, Verona, Italy) and the United Kingdom (Acute Stroke Rehabilitation Unit, St. Thomas Hospital, London). These patients included 132 who had previously taken part in 2 studies on AHP (Pacella et al. 2019; Moro et al. 2021). Seventy-five patients met the inclusion criteria: (i) unilateral right hemisphere damage, secondary to a first-ever ischemic stroke, as confirmed by clinical neuroimaging (MRI or CT) and (ii) severe plegia of contralesional upper limb (Medical Research Council—MRC scale (Guarantors of Brain 1976) = 0). Exclusion criteria were: (i) previous history of neurological or psychiatric illness; (ii) medication with severe cognitive or mood side-effects; and (iii) severe language, general cognitive impairment, or mood disturbance that precluded completion of the assessment. Structural neuroimaging and clinical data were collected from the included patients, who were subsequently divided into 2 groups according to the presence/absence of DSO. DSO group classification (presence/DSO+ vs. absence/DSO−) was established according to an interview adapted from previous studies (Feinberg et al. 2010; Moro et al. 2016; Jenkinson et al. 2020) and widely used in the clinical setting. Three questions were asked with reference to the patient’s left hand (moved to the ipsilesional field to reduce neglect): (i) “Is this your hand?”; (ii) “Do you ever feel as if this was not your hand?”; and (iii) “Does it belong to someone else?”. Patients’ responses to each question were scored by 2 expert clinicians, with 0 = patient recognizes the arm as belonging to him/her; 1 = uncertain answers indicating doubts about ownership; and 2 = responses indicating disownership and/or attribution of the arm to somebody else (min score = 0; max score = 6). Patients scoring ≥4 were diagnosed as DSO, while only patients scoring “0” were considered for the control group. This allowed the exclusion of any uncertain cases, where patients did not show clear limb disownership, but gave unclear responses, for example due to somatosensory disorders or affective components (e.g. “I would like the arm was not mine”; “I don’t feel like it is mine as I don’t experience any physical sensations from it”).

The resulting sample (n = 49) comprised 23 DSO+ patients and 26 DSO− controls. A summary of the selection process of the patients is shown in Fig. 1. All patients gave written, informed consent and the research was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki (2013) and approved by the Local Ethical Committees of each location.

A schematic representation of the patient’s selection process by applying the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Uncertain responses were considered when patients did not show clear limb disownership but gave unclear responses that did not allow patients to be clearly classified as DSO+ or DSO-.
Fig. 1

A schematic representation of the patient’s selection process by applying the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Uncertain responses were considered when patients did not show clear limb disownership but gave unclear responses that did not allow patients to be clearly classified as DSO+ or DSO-.

Neurological and neuropsychological assessment

There were no differences in age and education between the 2 groups. As the data were collected from different units, only the scores in the neuropsychological tests that were administrated in both the centers were considered (Table 1).

Proprioception was assessed by asking the patients to state whether or not they felt a passive movement administered to an upper limb joint (i.e. index finger, wrist, and elbow; Vocat et al. 2010). Personal neglect was assessed by means of the “Comb” subtest, of the Comb and Razor test (McIntosh et al. 2000), while patients’ score on the Line crossing subtest of the Behavioral Inattention Test was considered as a measure of extra-personal neglect (Wilson et al. 1987). The backward digit span scores (Baddeley et al. 1975) served as index of working memory. AHP was assessed via the Bisiach’s scale (Bisiach et al. 1986; D’Imperio, Tomelleri, et al. 2017b for scoring).

Lesions drawing and disconnection maps

Neuroimaging data were acquired via Computerized Tomography and Magnetic Resonance (CT = 89%; MRI = 11%) and lesions were segmented and coregistered using an established manual procedure (Moro et al. 2016; Pacella et al. 2019; Jenkinson et al. 2020). The lesion drawing was independently performed by 2 experts, who were blind to the patients’ group classification. In cases of disagreement on the extension or the site of a lesion, a third anatomist’s opinion was consulted.

Scans were registered on the ICBM152 template of the Montreal Neurological Institute, furnished with the MRIcron software (ch2, http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricro/mricron/). First, the standard template was rotated on the 3 plans (size: 181 × 217 × 181 mm, voxel resolution: 1 mm2) to match the orientation of the patient’s MRI or CT scan. Lesions were outlined on the axial slices of the rotated template. The resulting lesion volumes were then rotated back into the canonical orientation, to align the lesion volumes of each patient to the same stereotaxic space. Finally, to remove voxels of lesions outside the white and gray matter brain tissue, lesion volumes were filtered by means of custom masks based on the ICBM152 template.

Disconnectome maps were computed with the “disconnectome map” tool of the BCBToolkit software (Foulon et al. 2018). The first step of the procedure is the tracking of white matter fibers passing through each patient’s lesion, by means of the registration of lesions on the diffusion-weighted imaging dataset of 10 healthy controls (Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2017). This produces a percentage overlap map that takes into account the interindividual variability of tractography in a healthy controls’ dataset. Therefore, in the resulting disconnectome maps computed for each lesion, voxels show the probability of disconnection (from 0% to 100%; Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2015). These disconnection probabilities of each patient were used as a predictor variable in subsequent analyses (see Statistical analyses section).

Statistical analyses

Clinical and neuropsychological variables

A comparison of the clinical and neuropsychological variables between the two groups was executed by means of Bayesian linear models. Unlike traditional frequentist statistics which only allow inferences to be made on the basis of rejecting the null hypothesis, these Bayesian models allow both the null (i.e. no differences between groups) and alternative (i.e. differences between groups) hypotheses to be accepted or rejected (Gelman et al. 2013). Bayes Factors (BF10) were calculated, with BF10 > 3 indicating that the alternative hypothesis should be accepted, a BF10 < 1/3 indicating that the null hypothesis should be accepted, and results within 3 and 1/3 being inconclusive. In this latter case, it is considered as valid to accept the null (simpler) model (for the principle of the Ockham’s razor; Jefferys and Berger 1992). To compare results that are expressed in different scoring ranges, all the scores from neuropsychological tests were transformed to z-scores, with higher scores corresponding to better performances. Missing data (11% of the total clinical and neuropsychological data) were replaced by means of multivariate imputation by chained equations using the mice package (van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn 2011) in R ver. 4.0.0. This algorithm is able to estimate the missing data starting from the existing data, exploiting correlations among variables. The missing values were present in: the Bisiach test, which correlates with the DSO score (Moro et al. 2021); the personal neglect, the line crossing, and the proprioception tests, which correlate with each other (Committeri et al. 2018; Fisher et al. 2020); the digit span backward score, which correlates with age (Hester et al. 2004); and the interval between the lesion onset and neuroimaging recording that correlates with the gravity of the symptom. In order to guarantee that the missing data imputation did not introduce biases, a series of Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests were applied to all the variables with missing data, comparing the data distribution with and without the imputed data. In all cases, the distributions were no different (all D < 0.06 and all P > 0.99).

Sites of lesion and tract disconnections

Two separate linear regression analyses were used to explore differences in lesion sites and tract disconnections between the 2 groups, both using the same procedure via the tool “randomize,” part of FSL package (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/, version 5.0), which performs nonparametric statistics on neuroimaging data. Data on lesion size, lesion onset-neuroimaging interval (de Haan and Karnath 2018), anosognosia, personal and extrapersonal neglect, working memory, and proprioception were used as covariates. Threshold-Free Clusters Enhancement option was applied to boost cluster-like structures of voxels, and results that survived the 5,000 permutations testing were controlled for family-wise error rate (P > 0.95).

To identify the gray matter structures emerging as significant from the regression analysis, results were compared with the probability maps of the correspondent anatomical structures (thresholded at 60%) in the Harvard-Oxford Atlas (Desikan et al. 2006). This allowed us to compute the proportion of gray matter structures affected by each patient’s lesion. Similarly, to identify the disconnected white matter tracts on the disconnection results, the tracts probability maps (thresholded at 80%) of an atlas of human brain connections (Rojkova et al. 2016) were used. The masks were also used to extract the probabilities of disconnection for each tract from each patient’s disconnectome map.

Contribution of gray and white matter structures in DSO

With the purpose of understanding if, in addition to the clinical variables, each single lesion site (gray matter) or tract disconnection was able to explain the DSO symptomatology better than the clinical variables alone, Bayesian Bernoulli models (BBMs) were used. For each structure and tract separately, a null model was fitted with DSO symptoms (1 = present, 0 = absent) regressed on the clinical variables scaled on a [−1, 1] range (lesion size, lesion-onset/neuroimaging interval, anosognosia, visuo-spatial neglect—Line Crossing test, personal neglect—Comb test, working memory—Digit Span Backward, and proprioception scores). The null model was compared with other models where the DSO symptoms were regressed on the probability of disconnection of the white matter tract or on the proportion of the lesioned gray matter structure ([0, 1] range) plus the clinical variables (alternative hypothesis models). When the Bayes Factor was in favor of the alternative hypothesis model (i.e. the structure considered + the clinical variables explained the DSO symptoms better than the clinical variables alone), that white or gray matter structures were used in the following step.

Here, the possibility that the damaged structures found to occur in DSO are organized in neural networks was investigated. For this, the optimal number of clusters was calculated by employing the Average Silhouette method (Kaufman and Rousseeuw 1990) on the gray matter lesions (PrCG, PsCG, and SMG) and disconnection data (AS, SLFIII, SLFII, FIL, and FIT5) that had resulted to be associated with DSO (and thus using only data from DSO patients).

Then, the clusters of lesion and disconnection data of DSO patients were determined via the K-means algorithm (Hartigan and Wong 1979). The rationale behind this procedure was the necessity to verify if the DSO patients were grouped by different patterns of lesion (gray and white matter) independently from the DSO severity. Then, in order to understand whether the clusters represented a different degree of severity of the DSO symptom, a Bayesian Gaussian Model was fitted, with the raw DSO severity score as dependent variable (range: {4, 5, 6}) and the division in clusters and the clinical variables as regressors (scaled in the [−1, 1] range). This model was compared with a null model with the same dependent variable and the scaled clinical variables, but not the cluster factor, as regressors. Finally, the proportion (gray matter) or the probability (white matter) of each damaged structure was compared between the 2 clusters (controlling for the scaled clinical variables) by means of Bayesian Gaussian Models (again, each alternative model—i.e. cluster and control clinical variables—was compared with the null model—control clinical variables alone). The goal of this analysis was to understand which structures were damaged in an equivalent degree in both clusters. A summary of the statistical procedure is shown in SMA.

Cluster and Bayesian analyses were computed in R ver. 4.0.0 (R Core Team 2020). Bayesian models were fitted by means of the brms package (Bürkner 2017), with 4 chains of 100,000 iterations each one and 10,000 burn-in steps, for a total of 360,000 samplings. Comparisons between Bayesian models were carried out using BF10 (Kass and Raftery 1995). BF10 were computed by using the bridgesampling package (Gronau et al. 2020). BBMs had Cauchy distributions with mean 0 and scale 10 as priors for the regressors, and the Bayesian Gaussian models used Cauchy distribution, with mean 0 and scale 10 for the regressors, and mean 0 and scale 1 for the intercept, as prior distributions. For the average silhouette method, the Factoextra package (Kassambara and Mundt 2020) was used.

Results

Results from the clinical and neuropsychological assessment, with comparison between the 2 groups, are shown in Table 1. The 2 groups differed only in their level of DSO, as expected.

In a first step of analysis, we examined the gray and white matter structures involved in DSO, using 2 separate linear regressions, and controlling for extraneous demographic, clinical, and neuropsychological variables. We found that DSO was associated with right-hemisphere gray matter damage to the Pars Opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; BA 6, 45; P = 0.003), the Precentral gyrus (PrCG; BA 4; P = 0.003), the Postcentral gyrus (PsCG; BA 4; P = 0.003), and the SMG (BA 40; P = 0.003) (Fig. 2a). Additionally, damage to several white matter tracts was associated with DSO, comprising the Frontal Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus (FIL; P = 0.008), the Fronto-Insular tract 5 (FIT5; P = 0.008), the Anterior Segment (AS) of the Arcuate Fasciculus (P = 0.009), the second (P = 0.009) and third (P = 0.008) branches of the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus (SLFII; SLFIII), and the Corpus Callosum (CC; P = 0.009) (Figs 2 and 3a–c).

Mapping of lesions and brain disconnections in DSO when compared with the control group. a) Statistical mapping of the lesions in the gray matter. PsCG: Postcentral gyrus; PrCG: Precentral gyrus; Pars Oper: Pars opercularis of the IFG. b) Statistical mapping of the brain disconnections in DSO when compared with the control group. FIT5: Fronto-insular, tract 5; SLFIII: Third branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus; AS: Anterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus; SLFII: Second branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus; FIL: Frontal inferior longitudinal tract of the frontal longitudinal system; CC: Corpus callosum. Color bar represents the P-value statistic; only significant voxels are shown (P < 0.05).
Fig. 2

Mapping of lesions and brain disconnections in DSO when compared with the control group. a) Statistical mapping of the lesions in the gray matter. PsCG: Postcentral gyrus; PrCG: Precentral gyrus; Pars Oper: Pars opercularis of the IFG. b) Statistical mapping of the brain disconnections in DSO when compared with the control group. FIT5: Fronto-insular, tract 5; SLFIII: Third branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus; AS: Anterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus; SLFII: Second branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus; FIL: Frontal inferior longitudinal tract of the frontal longitudinal system; CC: Corpus callosum. Color bar represents the P-value statistic; only significant voxels are shown (P < 0.05).

The white matter tract that contributes to DSO symptoms. a) Lateral and b) medial view of the reconstruction of the white matter tracts contributing to DSO (BF10 > 3). AS = anterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus. SLFIII = third branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus. SLFII = second branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus. FIT5 = fronto-insular tract 5. c) Identification on a brain template of the gray matter structure contributing to DSO (BF10 > 3). PrCG = precentral gyrus. PsCG = postcentral gyrus. d) Bayes factor for each anatomical result’s model representing the hypothesis that the damage to gray matter structure/tract disconnection contributes to the emergence of DSO, against the null model. e) Posterior distributions (PD) from the Bayesian analysis of DSO patients’ grouping along the 2 clusters obtained from the silhouette analysis. The violin plots represent the PDs obtained from the Bayesian analysis; the upper and lower boundaries of the box are the boundaries of the 95% highest density interval of the PD; the midline is the mode of the PD. The first variable on the x-axis (gray-shaded background) represents the DSO mean severity score for each cluster (range 0–6, left-side y-axis). On the right part of the plot, the proportions of gray matter structures and the disconnection probability of the tracts involved in each cluster are represented (range 0–1, right-side y-axis). = Stands for BF10 < 1/3; ≠ Stands for BF10 > 3.
Fig. 3

The white matter tract that contributes to DSO symptoms. a) Lateral and b) medial view of the reconstruction of the white matter tracts contributing to DSO (BF10 > 3). AS = anterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus. SLFIII = third branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus. SLFII = second branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus. FIT5 = fronto-insular tract 5. c) Identification on a brain template of the gray matter structure contributing to DSO (BF10 > 3). PrCG = precentral gyrus. PsCG = postcentral gyrus. d) Bayes factor for each anatomical result’s model representing the hypothesis that the damage to gray matter structure/tract disconnection contributes to the emergence of DSO, against the null model. e) Posterior distributions (PD) from the Bayesian analysis of DSO patients’ grouping along the 2 clusters obtained from the silhouette analysis. The violin plots represent the PDs obtained from the Bayesian analysis; the upper and lower boundaries of the box are the boundaries of the 95% highest density interval of the PD; the midline is the mode of the PD. The first variable on the x-axis (gray-shaded background) represents the DSO mean severity score for each cluster (range 0–6, left-side y-axis). On the right part of the plot, the proportions of gray matter structures and the disconnection probability of the tracts involved in each cluster are represented (range 0–1, right-side y-axis). = Stands for BF10 < 1/3; ≠ Stands for BF10 > 3.

In a second step of analysis, we aimed to identify which of these structures account for DSO symptoms, over and above explaining concurrently observed clinical and neuropsychological deficits. To do this, BBMs of each cortical area and white matter tract were compared with a null model containing only the clinical variables (see Materials and methods for details). All structures except the CC (BF10 = 0.795) and Pars Opercularis (BF10 = 2.692) provided a better explanation for DSO symptoms rather than simply accounting for the concurrent clinical variables (BFs10 > 3, minimum BF10 = 7.044; Fig. 3d). These two lesion sites were not, therefore, considered in the next step of our analyses, in which we aimed to identify the network of structures that form the neuroanatomical basis of core DSO symptoms.

Silhouette analysis (Kaufman and Rousseeuw 1990) was used to identify 2 clusters as the optimal number of clusters needed to explain DSO symptoms. K-means clustering (Hartigan and Wong 1979) then identified the components of these 2 clusters, both of which contained the same anatomical areas, as detailed in Fig. 3e. A Bayesian Gaussian Model subsequently showed that the 2 clusters explained equally well the observed severity of DSO symptoms (BF10 = 0.3; Fig. 3e, left/gray-shaded panel). Thus, as a final step, we used Bayesian Gaussian Models to assess the level of agreement between the 2 clusters in terms of proportion of damage in each gray matter lesion and probability of each of the white matter disconnections. Results of this analysis indicated that FIT5 (BF10 = 0.0519), FIL (BF10 = 0.198), and SMG (BF10 = 0.253) had equal contributions across the clusters (Fig. 2e). In contrast, results for SLFIII and AS were not conclusive (BF10 = 0.459 and 1.099, respectively), and there are differences between the 2 clusters in the involvement of SLFII (BF10 = 20.140), of PsCG (BF10 = 3.420) and of PrCG (BF10 = 5.622) (Fig. 3e).

Discussion

The current study drew on recent ideas that the SBO is a dynamic process relying not only on cortical premotor and parietal areas associated with sensory processing and multisensory integration (Blanke et al. 2015; Kilteni et al. 2015; Tsakiris 2017) but also on wider brain networks responsible for salience monitoring and belief updating (Apps and Tsakiris 2014; Fotopoulou 2015), supported by fronto-parietal and fronto-insular connections (Walla et al. 2008; Gandola et al. 2012; Invernizzi et al. 2013; Moro et al. 2016; Northoff 2016; Romano and Maravita 2019; Qin et al. 2020; Azzalini et al. 2021) as recently found in other areas of body awareness (Pacella et al. 2019; Kirsch et al. 2021). To provide causal evidence for this hypothesis, we used advanced lesion mapping analyses and examined the brain disconnections associated with disturbed sensation of body ownership (DSO) in a large sample of patients with damage to the right-hemisphere. To go beyond the limitations of previous neuroanatomical studies of DSO, we applied Bayesian model comparisons to identify the model that best explains the syndrome over and above clinical scores alone and then we applied cluster analyses on the resultant lesion patterns to identify the possibly dissociable network(s) of structures, and the degree of involvement of each area in each network associated with DSO symptoms and not with other neuropsychological symptoms.

Our results revealed a major role of white matter disconnections in DSO that, when combined with direct gray matter lesions to the parietal cortex (mainly SMG), revealed that DSO is the result of disruption to a fronto-insular-parietal-network, involving several necessary lesioned brain areas, such as SMG, precentral and postcentral gyri, and disconnections of the anterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus (AS), the second and third branches of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLFII, SLFIII), the FIL, and the tract 5 of the fronto-insular tract (FIT5). These structures combine in at least 2 critical ways to cause the syndrome. More specifically, we found strong evidence for the involvement of SMG and disconnections of the fronto-insular (FIT5) and the FIL tracts to this network. We also found evidence that damage to central gyrus areas and disconnections of the SLFII is of smaller, additional contribution to DSO, while the degree of involvement of the anterior segment of the Arcuate Fasciculus (AS) and the third branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLFIII) remains inconclusive. Correspondingly, we conclude that the SBO requires the convergence (i.e. integration) of a number of cognitive processes, rather than being purely the functional role of a segregated cortical area. Indeed, this interpretation is consistent with previous studies on the syndrome that have used standard lesion-mapping approaches and revealed white matter disconnections (Gandola et al. 2012; Invernizzi et al. 2013; Moro et al. 2016). The involvement of the SMG is also consistent with previous results (Feinberg 1990; Martinaud et al. 2017), while in our sample, the insula (Baier and Karnath 2008; Feinberg et al. 2010) and the ventral prefrontal cortex (Gandola et al. 2012; Martinaud et al. 2017) result to be involved in the symptoms because of disconnections rather than direct lesions. Methodological differences may contribute to these divergent results. First, previous studies controlled for clinical and neuropsychological variables based on the comparison of clinical scores between the DSO and control groups, while in this study, these variables have been directly controlled in the analysis of neuroanatomical results. Second, previous evidence regarding white matter disconnection was deduced from lesion mapping approaches and not specifically investigated by means of a tractography as done in the current study.

Our findings are also consistent with the dynamic features of DSO, revealed by experimental manipulations that allow patients to temporarily recognize their own arms, or dummy arms as belonging to the self (Fotopoulou et al. 2011; Jenkinson et al. 2013; Martinaud et al. 2017), including experimental studies that manipulated the salience and emotional value of sensory stimulations (Jenkinson et al. 2013, 2020; Romano et al. 2014; D’Imperio, Bulgarelli, et al. 2017). Our hypothesis on the role of multisensory integration and executive functions in DSO is indeed speculative, as unfortunately these functions were not directly assessed in our patients. However, this possibility is supported by the finding that frontal and insular disconnections play a crucial role in the emergence of DSO. The role of the insula in the SBO is well known (Baier and Karnath 2008; Craig 2009; Moro et al. 2016), interpreted either as contributing to the integration of interoceptive sensations with other signals about the body (Craig 2009) or as regulating the salience of sensory signals (Menon and Uddin 2010; Allen et al. 2016), including during belief updating about sensory states (Wang et al. 2020) and self-awareness (Kirsch et al. 2021). Indeed, the insula is also part of the salient network, a large-scale brain network identified in resting state fMRI studies and connectivity analyses and thought also to relate to the ventral attention network (Corbetta et al. 2008), involving the temporoparietal junction and the ventral frontal cortex, connected by the ventral branch of the SLF. While damage to this later track was found to be part of the core disconnections of another right hemisphere disruption of the bodily self, namely motor unawareness, or AHP (Pacella et al. 2019), the present study produced inconclusive results regarding the degree of its involvement in DSO. Instead, we found stronger evidence that DSO is predicted by damage to the FIT5, which links the subcentral gyrus to the anterior long gyrus of the insula (Rojkova et al. 2016) and the FIL that connects the PrCG to the ventral part of the middle frontal gyrus and the superior part of the IFG, reaching anteriorly the frontal pole (Catani et al. 2012).

The SMG, in the inferior parietal lobe, and to a lesser degree the precentral and postcentral gyrus, were the only gray matter areas involved in DSO symptoms when the contribution of the anatomical structures was compared with that of the remaining clinical variables (null model). The SMG is implicated in multisensory integration and particularly the spatial coding of incoming sensations (Bolognini et al. 2014; D’Imperio, Tomelleri, et al. 2017b) and actions (Daprati et al. 2010). In addition, the SMG is involved in implicit mentalising (Wiesmann et al. 2020) and the self-other distinction, and particularly the inhibition of emotional egocentricity (Besharati et al. 2016) as also demonstrated by the experimental induction of out-of-body experiences (e.g. Blanke et al. 2002). However, although necessary, SMG damage does not appear sufficient to explain DSO symptoms in isolation, and the involvement of the precentral and postcentral gyrus appears even weaker.

Moreover, our clustering analyses indicated that the gray matter lesions and white matter disconnections associated with DSO symptoms and their severity can cluster together in 2 ways, with some areas contributing equally to each cluster and other not. These differences were not associated with differences in symptom severity in the present study, but more detailed characterization of patients’ behavior in future studies using these advanced neuroanatomical and statistical methods may examine whether these differences are associated with different types of DSO phenomenology.

A limitation of our study, and all structure–function inferences derived from lesion mapping work, is the indirect nature of our functional inferences, which are based on structural brain damage and probabilistic white matter disconnection information. Another limitation regards the manual lesion delineation and registration methods and the sensitivity level of neuroimaging techniques that do not depict the full extent of damage produced by stroke lesions (Hillis et al. 2000). However, these limitations mainly apply to small sample studies, while here, the relatively large number of patients investigated, the strict criteria of inclusion in the 2 groups (that excluded patients that could not be diagnosed with sufficient certainty) and the control for neuropsychological variables reduce these risks. Future studies would benefit from applying functional imaging methods to study DSO patients, although such studies are difficult to perform and, to our knowledge, there is no such group study on the syndrome and thus no corresponding functional connectivity analyses.

We did not control for patients’ age, gender, and education in the analyses, although previous studies reported effects of aging (Giorgio et al. 2010; Zhao et al. 2015), gender (Cosgrove et al. 2007; Kanaan et al. 2012), and education (Umarova et al. 2021) on structural anatomy. These covariates were not added because the over-correction of lesional and disconnection analyses is shown to lead to biases and/or type II errors (Sperber et al. 2020). Thus, we limited the number of control covariates to the lesion volume, which has been found to strongly correlate with general cognitive impairment (Zhang et al. 2014; de Haan and Karnath 2018), and the main clinical variables (Romano and Maravita 2019) as the disentanglement of their neural correlates from DSO is within the main aim of the study. Finally, a more in-depth clinical assessment of neuropsychological variables, interoception, and multisensory integration would be useful in defining the neuropsychological patterns of these patients. However, the neuroanatomical approach and statistical methodologies used in the study require the choice of key but limited, behavioral data as any variables that are entered as covariates have a cost in terms of statistical power (de Haan and Karnath 2018). Thus, we prioritized recruiting a larger sample of patients and presenting only the neuropsychological tests administered to the majority of patients. In conclusion, on the basis of an advanced gray and white matter symptom-lesion and disconnection mapping study, we demonstrate a core fronto-insular-parietal network for the SBO. Combining these results with previous experimental findings, we suggest that the SBO is not limited to bottom-up processes of multisensory integration, but it involves the convergence between such processes, and top-down control and monitoring of sensory salience in different contexts.

Acknowledgments

We thank Sara Bertagnoli and Cristina Bulgarelli for their help in data collection. We are grateful to all the patients and relatives for their willingness to participate in the study.

Funding

This study was supported by a European Research Council Consolidator Award (METABODY: ERC- 2018-COG-818070 to AF); Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale (FRM, DEQ20150331725 to VP); Ministero dell’Istruzione, dell’Università e della Ricerca (MIUR), Italy (PRIN 2017- 2017N7WCLP to VM and MS); and by a Commonwealth Scholarship, an Oppenheimer Memorial Trust Fellowship, and a Neuropsychology International Fellowship Award from the British Psychological Society in conjunction with the British Neuropsychological Society to SB.

Conflict of interest statement. None declared.

Data availability

The datasets generated during and analyzed during the current study are available in the OSF repository, https://osf.io/7yxh5/.

References

Allen
 
M
,
Fardo
 
F
,
Dietz
 
MJ
,
Hillebrandt
 
H
,
Friston
 
KJ
,
Rees
 
G
,
Roepstorff
 
A
.
Anterior insula coordinates hierarchical processing of tactile mismatch responses
.
NeuroImage
.
2016
:
127
:
34
43
.

Apps
 
MAJ
,
Tsakiris
 
M
.
The free-energy self: a predictive coding account of self-recognition
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2014
:
41
:
85
97
.

Azzalini
 
D
,
Buot
 
A
,
Palminteri
 
S
,
Tallon-Baudry
 
C
.
Responses to heartbeats in ventromedial prefrontal cortex contribute to subjective preference-based decisions
.
J Neurosci
.
2021
:
41
(
23
):
5102
5114
.

Baddeley
 
AD
,
Thomson
 
N
,
Buchanan
 
M
.
Word length and the structure of short-term memory
.
J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav
.
1975
:
14
:
575
589
.

Baier
 
B
,
Karnath
 
HO
.
Tight link between our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions
.
2008
:
39
(
2
):
486
488
.

Besharati
 
S
,
Forkel
 
SJ
,
Kopelman
 
M
,
Solms
 
M
,
Jenkinson
 
PM
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
.
Mentalizing the body: spatial and social cognition in anosognosia for hemiplegia
.
Brain
.
2016
:
139
:
971
985
.

Bisiach
 
E
,
Vallar
 
G
,
Perani
 
D
,
Papagno
 
C
,
Berti
 
A
.
Unawareness of disease following lesions of the right hemisphere: anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia
.
Neuropsychologia
.
1986
:
24
:
471
482
.

Blanke
 
O
,
Ortigue
 
S
,
Landis
 
T
,
Seeck
 
M
.
Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions
.
Nature
.
2002
:
419
(
6904
):
269
270
.

Blanke
 
O
,
Slater
 
M
,
Serino
 
A
.
Behavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousness
.
Neuron
.
2015
:
88
:
145
166
.

Bolognini
 
N
,
Ronchi
 
R
,
Casati
 
C
,
Fortis
 
P
,
Vallar
 
G
.
Multisensory remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion: my hand is back!
 
Neurol Clin Pract
.
2014
:
4
:
216
225
.

Bürkner
 
P-C
.
Bdrms: an R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan
.
J Stat Softw
.
2017
:
80
:
1
28
.

Catani
 
M
,
Dell’Acqua
 
F
,
Vergani
 
F
,
Malik
 
F
,
Hodge
 
H
,
Roy
 
P
,
Valabregue
 
R
,
Thiebaut de Schotten
 
M
.
Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain
.
Cortex
.
2012
:
48
:
273
291
.

Cereda
 
C
,
Ghika
 
J
,
Maeder
 
P
,
Bogousslavsky
 
J
.
Strokes restricted to the insular cortex
.
Neurology
.
2002
:
59
(
12
):
1950
1955
.

Committeri
 
G
,
Piervincenzi
 
C
,
Pizzamiglio
 
L
.
Personal neglect: a comprehensive theoretical and anatomo–clinical review
.
Neuropsychology
.
2018
:
32
:
269
279
.

Corbetta
 
M
,
Patel
 
G
,
Shulman
 
GL
.
The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind
.
Neuron
.
2008
:
58
:
306
324
.

Cosgrove
 
KP
,
Mazure
 
CM
,
Staley
 
JK
.
Evolving knowledge of sex differences in brain structure, function, and chemistry
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2007
:
62
(
8
):
847
855
.

Craig
 
AD
.
How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2009
:
10
:
59
70
.

Crucianelli
 
L
,
Serpell
 
L
,
Paloyelis
 
Y
,
Ricciardi
 
L
,
Robinson
 
P
,
Jenkinson
 
P
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
.
The effect of intranasal oxytocin on the perception of affective touch and multisensory integration in anorexia nervosa: protocol for a double-blind placebo-controlled crossover study
.
BMJ Open
.
2019
:
9
(
3
):
e024913
.

D’Imperio
 
D
,
Bulgarelli
 
C
,
Bertagnoli
 
S
,
Avesani
 
R
,
Moro
 
V
.
Modulating anosognosia for hemiplegia: the role of dangerous actions in emergent awareness
.
Cortex
.
2017
:
92
:
187
203
.

D’Imperio
 
D
,
Tomelleri
 
G
,
Moretto
 
G
,
Moro
 
V
.
Modulation of somatoparaphrenia following left-hemisphere damage
.
Neurocase
.
2017
:
23
:
162
170
.

Daprati
 
E
,
Sirigu
 
A
,
Nico
 
D
.
Body and movement: consciousness in the parietal lobes
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2010
:
48
:
756
762
.

de
 
Haan
 
B
,
Karnath
 
HO
.
A hitchhiker’s guide to lesion-behaviour mapping
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2018
:
115
:
5
16
.

de
 
Vignemont
 
F
.
Embodiment, ownership and disownership
.
Conscious Cogn
.
2011
:
20
:
82
93
.

Desikan
 
RS
,
Ségonne
 
F
,
Fischl
 
B
,
Quinn
 
BT
,
Dickerson
 
BC
,
Blacker
 
D
,
Buckner
 
RL
,
Dale
 
AM
,
Maguire
 
RP
,
Hyman
 
BT
, et al.  
An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest
.
NeuroImage
.
2006
:
31
:
968
980
.

Feinberg
 
TE
,
Venneri
 
A
,
Simone
 
AM
,
Fan
 
Y
,
Northoff
 
G
.
The neuroanatomy of asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
.
2010
:
81
:
276
281
.

Feinberg TE, Haber LD, Leeds NE. Verbal asomatognosia.

Neurology
. 1990:40:1391–1394.

Fisher
 
G
,
Quel de Oliveira
 
C
,
Verhagen
 
A
,
Gandevia
 
S
,
Kennedy
 
D
.
Proprioceptive impairment in unilateral neglect after stroke: a systematic review
.
SAGE Open Med
.
2020
:
8
:
205031212095107
.

Fotopoulou
 
A
.
The virtual bodily self: Mentalisation of the body as revealed in anosognosia for hemiplegia
.
Conscious Cogn
.
2015
:
33
:
500
510
.

Fotopoulou
 
A
,
Jenkinson
 
PM
,
Tsakiris
 
M
,
Haggard
 
P
,
Rudd
 
A
,
Kopelman
 
MD
.
Mirror-view reverses somatoparaphrenia: dissociation between first- and third-person perspectives on body ownership
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2011
:
49
:
3946
3955
.

Foulon
 
C
,
Cerliani
 
L
,
Kinkingnéhun
 
S
,
Levy
 
R
,
Rosso
 
C
,
Urbanski
 
M
,
Volle
 
E
,
Thiebaut de Schotten
 
M
.
Advanced lesion symptom mapping analyses and implementation as BCBtoolkit
.
Gigascience
.
2018
:
7
(
3
):
1
17
.

Fox
 
MD
.
Mapping symptoms to brain networks with the human connectome
.
N Engl J Med
.
2018
:
379
:
2237
2245
.

Gallagher
 
S
.
Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2000
:
4
:
14
21
.

Gandola
 
M
,
Invernizzi
 
P
,
Sedda
 
A
,
Ferrè
 
ER
,
Sterzi
 
R
,
Sberna
 
M
,
Paulesu
 
E
,
Bottini
 
G
.
An anatomical account of somatoparaphrenia
.
Cortex
.
2012
:
48
:
1165
1178
.

Gelman
 
A
,
Carlin
 
JB
,
Stern
 
HS
,
Dunson
 
DB
,
Vehtari
 
A
,
Rubin
 
DB
.
Bayesian data analysis
. 3rd ed.
New York (NY)
:
Chapman and Hall/CRC
;
2013
.

Gerstmann
 
J
.
Problem of imperception of disease and of impaired body territories with organic lesions: relation to body scheme and its disorders
.
Arch Neurol Psychiatr
.
1942
:
48
:
890
913
.

Giorgio
 
A
,
Santelli
 
L
,
Tomassini
 
V
,
Bosnell
 
R
,
Smith
 
S
,
De Stefano
 
N
,
Johansen-Berg
 
H
.
Age-related changes in grey and white matter structure throughout adulthood
.
NeuroImage
.
2010
:
51
:
943
951
.

Gronau
 
QF
,
Singmann
 
H
,
Wagenmakers
 
EJ
.
Bridgesampling: an R package for estimating normalizing constants
.
J Stat Softw
.
2020
:
92
(
10
).

Guarantors of Brain. Aids to the examination of the peripheral nervous system. London (UK): Saunders Ltd;

1976

Hartigan
 
JA
,
Wong
 
MA
.
Algorithm AS 136: a K-means clustering algorithm
.
Appl Stat
.
1979
:
28
(
1
):
100
.

Hester
 
RL
,
Kinsella
 
GJ
,
Ong
 
B
.
Effect of age on forward and backward span tasks
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc
.
2004
:
10
:
475
481
.

Hillis
 
AE
,
Barker
 
PB
,
Beauchamp
 
NJ
,
Gordon
 
B
,
Wityk
 
RJ
.
MR perfusion imaging reveals regions of hypoperfusion associated with aphasia and neglect
.
Neurology
.
2000
:
55
:
782
788
.

Invernizzi
 
P
,
Gandola
 
M
,
Romano
 
D
,
Zapparoli
 
L
,
Bottini
 
G
,
Paulesu
 
E
.
What is mine? Behavioral and anatomical dissociations between somatoparaphrenia and anosognosia for hemiplegia
.
Behav Neurol
.
2013
:
26
:
139
150
.

Jefferys
 
WH
,
Berger
 
JO
.
Ockham’s razor and Bayesian analysis
.
Am Sci
.
1992
:
80
:
64
72
.

Jenkinson
 
PM
,
Haggard
 
P
,
Ferreira
 
NC
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
.
Body ownership and attention in the mirror: insights from somatoparaphrenia and the rubber hand illusion
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2013
:
51
(
8
):
1453
1462
.

Jenkinson
 
PM
,
Moro
 
V
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
.
Definition: asomatognosia
.
Cortex
.
2018
:
101
:
300
301
.

Jenkinson
 
PM
,
Papadaki
 
C
,
Besharati
 
S
,
Moro
 
V
,
Gobbetto
 
V
,
Crucianelli
 
L
,
Kirsch
 
LP
,
Avesani
 
R
,
Ward
 
NS
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
.
Welcoming back my arm: affective touch increases body ownership following right-hemisphere stroke
.
Brain Commun
.
2020
:
2 2
(
1
):fcaa034.

Kanaan
 
RA
,
Allin
 
M
,
Picchioni
 
M
,
Barker
 
GJ
,
Daly
 
E
,
Shergill
 
SS
,
Woolley
 
J
,
PK
 
MG
.
Gender differences in white matter microstructure
.
PLoS One
.
2012
:
7
(
6
):e38272. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038272.

Kass
 
RE
,
Raftery
 
AE
.
Bayes factors
.
J Am Stat Assoc
.
1995
:
90
:
773
795
.

Kassambara
 
A
,
Mundt
 
F
. Factoextra: extract and visualize the results of multivariate data analyses. In:
Package version 1.0.7. R Packag version. 1
;
2020

Kaufman
 
L
,
Rousseeuw
 
PJ
.
Finding groups in data: an introduction to cluster analysis (Wiley series in probability and statistics), Eepe
.
Ethz.Ch
;
1990

Kilteni
 
K
,
Maselli
 
A
,
Kording
 
KP
,
Slater
 
M
.
Over my fake body: body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2015
:
24
(
9
):
141
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00141.

Kirsch
 
LP
,
Mathys
 
C
,
Papadaki
 
C
,
Talelli
 
P
,
Friston
 
K
,
Moro
 
V
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
.
Updating beliefs beyond the here-and-now: the counter-factual self in anosognosia for hemiplegia
.
Brain Commun
.
2021
:
3
(
2
):fcab098.

Klingbeil
 
J
,
Wawrzyniak
 
M
,
Stockert
 
A
,
Karnath
 
H-O
,
Saur
 
D
.
Hippocampal diaschisis contributes to anosognosia for hemiplegia: evidence from lesion network-symptom-mapping
.
NeuroImage
.
2020
:
208
:116485.

Longo
 
MR
,
Schüür
 
F
,
Kammers
 
MPM
,
Tsakiris
 
M
,
Haggard
 
P
.
Self awareness and the body image
.
Acta Psychol
.
2009
:
132
:
166
172
.

Martinaud
 
O
,
Besharati
 
S
,
Jenkinson
 
PM
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
.
Ownership illusions in patients with body delusions: different neural profiles of visual capture and disownership
.
Cortex
.
2017
:
87
:
174
185
.

McIntosh
 
RD
,
Brodie
 
EE
,
Beschin
 
N
,
Robertson
 
IH
.
Improving the clinical diagnosis of dpersonal neglect: a reformulated comb and razor test
.
Cortex
.
2000
:
36
:
289
292
.

Menon
 
V
,
Uddin
 
LQ
.
Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
2010
:
214
(
5–6
):
655
667
.

Monai
 
E
,
Bernocchi
 
F
,
Bisio
 
M
,
Bisogno
 
AL
,
Salvalaggio
 
A
,
Corbetta
 
M
.
Multiple network disconnection in anosognosia for hemiplegia
.
Front Syst Neurosci
.
2020
:
14
:
21
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2020.00021.

Monti
 
A
,
Porciello
 
G
,
Tieri
 
G
,
Aglioti
 
SM
.
The “embreathment” illusion highlights the role of breathing in corporeal awareness
.
J Neurophysiol
.
2020
:
123
:
420
427
.

Moro
 
V
,
Pernigo
 
S
,
Tsakiris
 
M
,
Avesani
 
R
,
Edelstyn
 
NMJ
,
Jenkinson
 
PM
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
.
Motor versus body awareness: voxel-based lesion analysis in anosognosia for hemiplegia and somatoparaphrenia following right hemisphere stroke
.
Cortex
.
2016
:
83
:
62
77
.

Moro
 
V
,
Besharati
 
S
,
Scandola
 
M
,
Bertagnoli
 
S
,
Gobbetto
 
V
,
Ponzo
 
S
,
Bulgarelli
 
C
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
,
Jenkinson
 
PM
.
The motor unawareness assessment (MUNA): a new tool for the assessment of anosognosia for hemiplegia
.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
.
2021
:
43
:
91
104
.

Neisser
 
U
,
Jopling
 
DA
.
The conceptual self in context: culture experience self understanding
.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
;
1994

Northoff
 
G
.
Is the self a higher-order or fundamental function of the brain? The "basis model of self-specificity" and its encoding by the brain's spontaneous activity
.
Cogn Neurosci
.
2016
:
7
(
1–4
)
203
222
.

Pacella
 
V
,
Foulon
 
C
,
Jenkinson
 
PM
,
Scandola
 
M
,
Bertagnoli
 
S
,
Avesani
 
R
,
Fotopoulou
 
A
,
Moro
 
V
,
Thiebaut de Schotten
 
M
.
Anosognosia for hemiplegia as a tripartite disconnection syndrome
.
elife
.
2019
:
8
:
1
13
.

Qin
 
P
,
Wang
 
M
,
Northoff
 
G
.
Linking bodily, environmental and mental states in the self-a three-level model based on a meta-analysis
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2020
:
115
:
77
95
.

R Core Team
.
R: a language and environment for statistical computing
.
2020
.

Rojkova
 
K
,
Volle
 
E
,
Urbanski
 
M
,
Humbert
 
F
,
Dell’Acqua
 
F
,
Thiebaut de Schotten
 
M
.
Atlasing the frontal lobe connections and their variability due to age and education: a spherical deconvolution tractography study
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
2016
:
221
:
1751
1766
.

Romano
 
D
,
Maravita
 
A
.
The dynamic nature of the sense of ownership after brain injury clues from asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2019
:
132
:107119.

Romano
 
D
,
Gandola
 
M
,
Bottini
 
G
,
Maravita
 
A
.
Arousal responses to noxious stimuli in somatoparaphrenia and anosognosia: clues to body awareness
.
Brain
.
2014
:
137
:
1213
1223
.

Salvato
 
G
,
Gandola
 
M
,
Veronelli
 
L
,
Agostoni
 
EC
,
Sberna
 
M
,
Corbo
 
M
,
Bottini
 
G
.
The spatial side of somatoparaphrenia: a case study
.
Neurocase
.
2016
:
22
(
2
):
154
160
.

Samad
 
M
,
Chung
 
AJ
,
Shams
 
L
.
Perception of body ownership is driven by Bayesian sensory inference
.
PLoS One
.
2015
:
10
(
2
):e0117178.

Saxe
 
R
,
Jamal
 
N
,
Powell
 
L
.
My body or yours? The effect of visual perspective on cortical body representations
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2006
:
16
:
178
182
.

Sperber
 
C
,
Nolingberg
 
C
,
Karnath
 
HO
.
Post-stroke cognitive deficits rarely come alone: handling co-morbidity in lesion-behaviour mapping
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2020
:
41
:
1387
1399
.

Thiebaut De Schotten
 
M
,
Tomaiuolo
 
F
,
Aiello
 
M
,
Merola
 
S
,
Silvetti
 
M
,
Lecce
 
F
,
Bartolomeo
 
P
,
Doricchi
 
F
.
Damage to white matter pathways in subacute and chronic spatial neglect: a group study and 2 single-case studies with complete virtual “in vivo” tractography dissection
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2014
:
24
:
691
706
.

Thiebaut de Schotten
 
M
,
Dell’Acqua
 
F
,
Ratiu
 
P
,
Leslie
 
A
,
Howells
 
H
,
Cabanis
 
E
,
Iba-Zizen
 
MT
,
Plaisant
 
O
,
Simmons
 
A
,
Dronkers
 
NF
, et al.  
From Phineas gage and monsieur Leborgne to H.M.: revisiting disconnection syndromes
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2015
:
25
:
4812
4827
.

Thiebaut de Schotten
 
M
,
Urbanski
 
M
,
Batrancourt
 
B
,
Levy
 
R
,
Dubois
 
B
,
Cerliani
 
L
,
Volle
 
E
.
Rostro-caudal architecture of the frontal lobes in humans
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2017
:
27
:
4033
4047
.

Tsakiris
 
M
.
My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2010
:
48
:
703
712
.

Tsakiris
 
M
.
The multisensory basis of the self: from body to identity to others
.
Q J Exp Psychol
.
2017
:
70
:
597
609
.

Umarova
 
RM
,
Schumacher
 
LV
,
Schmidt
 
CSM
,
Martin
 
M
,
Egger
 
K
,
Urbach
 
H
,
Hennig
 
J
,
Klöppel
 
S
,
Kaller
 
CP
.
Interaction between cognitive reserve and age moderates effect of lesion load on stroke outcome
.
Sci Rep
.
2021
:
11
:
4478
.

van
 
Buuren
 
S
,
Groothuis-Oudshoorn
 
K
.
Mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R
.
J Stat Softw
.
2011
:
45
:
1
67
.

Vallar
 
G
,
Ronchi
 
R
.
Somatoparaphrenia: a body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature
.
Exp Brain Res
.
2009
:
192
:
533
551
.

Vocat
 
R
,
Staub
 
F
,
Stroppini
 
T
,
Vuilleumier
 
P
.
Anosognosia for hemiplegia: a clinical-anatomical prospective study
.
Brain
.
2010
:
133
:
3578
3597
.

Walla
 
P
,
Duregger
 
C
,
Greiner
 
K
,
Thurner
 
S
,
Ehrenberger
 
K
.
Multiple aspects related to self-awareness and the awareness of others: an electroencephalography study
.
J Neural Transm
.
2008
:
115
:
983
992
.

Wang
 
Y
,
Zou
 
Q
,
Ao
 
Y
,
Liu
 
Y
,
Ouyang
 
Y
,
Wang
 
X
,
Biswal
 
B
,
Cui
 
Q
,
Chen
 
H
.
Frequency-dependent circuits anchored in the dorsal and ventral left anterior insula
.
Sci Rep
.
2020
:
10
(
1
):
16394
.

Wiesmann
 
CG
,
Friederici
 
AD
,
Singer
 
T
,
Steinbeis
 
N
.
Two systems for thinking about others’ thoughts in the developing brain
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2020
:
117
:6928–6935.

Wilson
 
B
,
Cockburn
 
J
,
Halligan
 
P
.
Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect
.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
.
1987
:
68
:
98
102
.

Zeller
 
D
,
Gross
 
C
,
Bartsch
 
A
,
Johansen-Berg
 
H
,
Classen
 
J
.
Ventral premotor cortex may be required for dynamic changes in the feeling of limb ownership: a lesion study
.
J Neurosci
.
2011
:
31
:
4852
4857
.

Zhang
 
Y
,
Kimberg
 
DY
,
Coslett
 
HB
,
Schwartz
 
MF
,
Wang
 
Z
.
Multivariate lesion-symptom mapping using support vector regression
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2014
:
35
:
5861
5876
.

Zhao
 
T
,
Cao
 
M
,
Niu
 
H
,
Zuo
 
X-N
,
Evans
 
A
,
He
 
Y
,
Dong
 
Q
,
Shu
 
N
.
Age-related changes in the topological organization of the white matter structural connectome across the human lifespan
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2015
:
36
:
3777
3792
.

Author notes

Paul M. Jenkinson and Akaterini Fotopoulou authors contributed equally to this work.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)