Abstract

Anthropogenic disturbances are a major concern for the welfare and conservation of wildlife. We recorded heart rate and body temperature of 20 free-living greylag geese in response to a major regularly re-occurring anthropogenic disturbance—New Year’s Eve fireworks. Heart rate and body temperature were significantly higher in the first and second hour of the new year, compared with the same hour on the 31st of December, the average during December and the average during January. Heart rate and body temperature was not significantly affected by sex or age. From 0200 to 0300 onwards, 1st of January heart rates did not significantly differ from the other periods; however, body temperatures were significantly increased until 0300–0400. From 0400 to 0500, heart rate was not affected by any of the investigated factors, whereas body temperature was significantly increased on the 1st of January compared with the 31st of December and the December average but not compared with the January average. To conclude, our results show that New Year’s Eve fireworks cause a substantial physiological response, indicative of a stress response in greylag geese, which is costly in terms of energy expenditure.

1. Introduction

Anthropogenic disturbances, such as noise or light pollution, human presence or motor vehicles, are increasingly becoming a major concern for the welfare and conservation of wildlife (Marion et al., 2020; Corradini et al., 2021; Jerem and Mathews, 2021). Disturbances can result in short-term to long-term changes in the behaviour and physiology of individuals (Bejder et al., 2006; New et al., 2014). Globally, species increase their nocturnality in response to human disturbance (Gaynor et al., 2018). Physiological activation, for example, via the sympathetic branch of the automatic nervous system, causing an increase in heart rate and body temperature (Bartholomew et al., 1964; Cabanac and Guillemette, 2001; Carere and Vanoers, 2004), which helps organisms to cope with environmental challenges [e.g. temperature stress (Bartholomew et al., 1964, Al-Haidary, 2004, Alam et al., 2011), hunger (Mesteig et al., 2000; De Jong et al., 2002; Savory and Kostal, 2006), agonistic encounters (Wascher, 2021), predator exposure (Oulton et al., 2013)] and maintain homeostasis (von Holst, 1998). However, this comes at the cost of increased energy expenditure (Weimerskirch et al., 2002; Wascher et al., 2018; Halsey et al., 2019). This is costly, as animals are limited in their actions by their energy throughput, the amount of energy they consume and use (McNab, 2022). Increases in heart rate and body temperature can also be an indication of emotional arousal and poor welfare (von Borell et al., 2007; Wascher, 2021). Chronic anthropogenic noise has been shown to decrease baseline corticosterone in birds and, conversely, increasing acute stressor-induced corticosterone (Kleist et al., 2018). Bird nestlings in noisy areas had shorter telomere lengths (Grunst et al., 2021). Via such mechanisms, anthropogenic disturbances can negativity affect individuals’ fitness (Daan et al., 1996; Fowler, 1999), for example, decrease hatching success or body condition (Kleist et al., 2018). Combined effects of anthropogenic noise and artificial light affect activity patterns in birds (Dominoni et al., 2020), community structure (Willems et al., 2022) and chick development (Ferraro et al., 2020).

Biologging technologies to measure heart rate can be used to assess effects of anthropogenic disturbance on wildlife. This is especially relevant, as studies have shown that individuals can show pronounced activation of the physiological stress response in the absence of obvious behavioural changes. For example, American black bears, Ursus americanus, and bighorn sheep, Ovis canadensis, showed no behavioural responses but significantly increased heart rate in response to anthropogenic disturbances, such as drone overflights and vehicle traffic (MacArthur et al., 1979; Ditmer et al., 2015).

Anthropogenic noise causes elevated heart rates in farm animals (Talling et al., 1996), fish (Graham and Cooke, 2008) and marine mammals (Hinde et al., 2018). Additionally, in response to a large-scale military manoeuvre moose, Alces alces, showed behavioural changes, i.e. decrease in flush distance, and physiological changes, i.e. higher maximum heart rates (Andersen et al., 1996). Direct human contact can activate the physiological stress response in wild animals; for example, Brown bears, Ursus arctos, significantly increased heart rate in response to dog hunts and human encounters (Le Grand et al., 2019). Human approach towards nesting birds may not always lead to females leaving the nest, but activation of the physiological stress response can increase energy expenditure (Magellanic penguins, Spheniscus magellanicus: Fowler, 1999; Yellow-eyed penguin, Megadyptes antipodes:Ellenberg et al., 2013; wandering albatrosses, Diomedea exulans: Weimerskirch et al., 2002). In contrast, other populations were described to be more resilient; for example, nesting American oystercatchers, Haematopus palliates, did not significantly increase heart rate in response to a variety of human disturbances, including human approach, off-road vehicles and aircraft overflights (Borneman et al., 2014), and greylag geese did not significantly increase heart rate in response to familiar humans approaching (Wascher et al., 2011). Whether or not a heart rate increase can be measured upon such events may be a matter of previous habituation.

A major regularly re-occurring world-wide anthropogenic disturbance are New Year’s Eve firework celebrations, causing noise and light pollution and major anxiety in pet animals. Hence, it is a significant welfare concern (dogs, Canis familiaris: Levine and Mills, 2008, Dale et al., 2010, Gates et al., 2019, Gähwiler et al., 2020, Riemer, 2020; horses, Equus caballus: Gronqvist et al., 2016). A study on several species of captive zoo animals showed no changes in behaviour in response to fireworks in most species (Rodewald et al., 2014). However, birds have been shown to take flight shortly after midnight and move in the air for at least 45 minutes in response to fireworks on New Year’s Eve in the Netherlands (Shamoun-Baranes et al., 2011). Besides these few studies, the effects of fireworks on wildlife are largely unknown.

In the present study, we investigate heart rate and body temperature responses of free-flying greylag geese in response to New Year’s Eve celebrations. We expected a significant increase in both, heart rate and body temperature, in response to the fireworks. Additionally, we investigated any effects of sex and age. Previously, heart rate differences between the sexes have been described only during the reproductive season (Wascher et al., 2018) and specific behavioural contexts (Wascher et al., 2012). Hence, in this study we do not expect different responses between males and females. In respect to age, we were interested whether older individuals, who have experienced multiple New Year’s Eve celebrations, responded less strongly, as they might have habituated to fireworks previously or alternatively sensitized, meaning that response to fireworks would increase over time (Riemer, 2020). Quantifying the impact of fireworks onto the short-term physiological stress response can help to understand the impact of anthropogenic disturbances for wild animals.

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Study site

The present study was conducted in a non-migratory free-living flock of greylag geese in the Almtal, Upper Austria. At the time of data collection, the flock consisted of approximately 150 individuals, marked with coloured leg bands for individual identification. The geese are unrestrained and freely roam the valley from the lake Almsee, on which they usually roost at night (47.747793°, 13.956805°) to the Konrad Lorenz research station (47.814143°, 13.948519°). At the research station, the flock is supplemented with pellets and grain twice daily at 0800 and 1500 hours during the winter months and at 1700 hours during the summer months. Both hand-raised and goose-raised flock members are habituated to the close presence of humans and they neither show avoidance if approached up to a distance of 1 m nor excrete elevated levels of corticosterone metabolites following such situations (Scheiber et al., 2005) or significantly change heart rate when familiar humans approach (Wascher et al., 2011). New Year’s Eve celebrations and fireworks are held in several nearby villages, including Grünau im Almtal and directly at the Almsee, where the geese roost at night. Geese are subjected to visual as well as auditory stimuli from the fireworks. Most of the firework activity starts at 0000 on the 1st of January and lasts for several minutes. We did not collect behavioural data from the focal individuals in this study; however, from anecdotal observations we know that geese take flight during the fireworks and circle over the Almsee but remain on the roosting site.

2.2. Data collection and analysis

A total of 25 individuals (8 females/17 males) were fitted fully implanted transmitter packages (60 × 30 × 11 mm, ~60 g; for further technical details and implantation procedure, see Wascher and Kotrschal, 2013, Wascher et al., 2018). Both heart rate and body core temperature were stored as 2-minute means in the implant over its lifetime and downloaded after electronic packages were surgically removed. Data were recorded during 2005 New Year’s Eve from 20 individuals (6 females/14 males). One male individual was sampled twice during 2005 and 2006. Age of focal individuals ranged from 1 to 12 years (average ± standard deviation = 4.714 ± 2.813). Raw data were filtered with a moving average to remove biologically implausible outlier values. We calculated mean values per hour of the day. We compared daily heart rate pattern on the 31st of December compared with the 1st of January, average values for the month of December and average values for the month of January. Additionally, we calculated mean value over 1 hour (e.g. 00–01, 01–02, 02–03) on the 31st of December and 1st of January and average over the entire month of December and January. Data were analysed using R version 4.0.3 (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria; http://www.r-project.org). We calculated general linear mixed model using the function lme in the package nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2020) with Gaussian error distribution and mean heart rate as well as body temperature as response variable. We calculated separate models for the hours 0000–0100, 0100–0200, 0200–0300, 0300–0400, 0400–0500 and 0500–0600, until no significant difference between different periods could be detected anymore. Period (31st December, 1st January, average December, average January), sex and age were included as explanatory variables. Various model diagnostics were employed to confirm model validity (visual inspection of distribution of residuals, qq plots, residuals plotted against fitted values), none of which suggested violation of model assumptions. To assess multicollinearity between fixed factors, we calculated variance inflation factors (VIFs) using the vif function in the package car (Fox and Weisberg, 2011). VIFs for all factors were below 1.5, indicating that there was no issue with multicollinearity (Zuur et al., 2009). For each model, we fitted individual identity as a random term to control for the potential dependence associated with multiple samples from the same individuals. To describe the variance explained by our models, we provide marginal and conditional R2 values that range from 0 to 1 and describe the proportion of variance explained by the fixed and random effects combined, respectively (Nakagawa and Schielzeth, 2013). We calculated marginal and conditional R2 values using the r.squaredGLMM function in MuMIn (Bartoń, 2019).

Table 1

Results of the general linear model investigating factors affecting heart rate (A) and body temperature (B) between 0000–0100, 0100–0200, 0200–0300, 0300–0400, 0400–0500 and 0500–0600

ParametersEstimate ± SEdft-valueP
0000–0100
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−3477.243 ± 2379.9461−1.4610.149
R2 marginal: 55%, R2 conditional: 9%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−55.424 ± 6.46561−8.571<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−60.569 ± 6.46561−9.367<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−55.721 ± 6.4661−8.617<0.001
Sex−8.933 ± 7.17117−1.2450.229
Age1.803 ± 1.19171.5140.148
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)70.924 ± 161.338610.4390.661
R2 marginal: 24%, R2 conditional: 64%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−1.242 ± 0.1361−9.505<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.284 ± 0.1361−9.822<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−1.18 ± 0.1361−9.026<0.001
Sex−0.427 ± 0.47917−0.8920.384
Age−0.015 ± 0.0817−0.1940.848
0100–0200
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2568.699 ± 1903.81959−1.3490.182
R2 marginal: 21%, R2 conditional: 22%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.973 ± 0.12559−7.728<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.074 ± 0.12559−8.533<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.989 ± 0.12659−7.805<0.001
Sex−0.328 ± 0.46717−0.70.492
Age−0.016 ± 0.07817−0.2120.834
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)72.663 ± 157.464590.4610.646
R2 marginal: 20%, R2 conditional: 66%Period (1 Jan relative to 31. Dec)−0.973 ± 0.12459.144−7.83<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.074 ± 0.12459.144−8.645<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.989 ± 0.12559.036−7.908<0.001
Sex−0.328 ± 0.48817.04−0.6710.51
Age−0.016 ± 0.08216.964−0.2030.841
0200–0300
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2330.824 ± 1529.32959−1.5240.132
R2 marginal: 11%, R2 conditional: 36%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−2.872 ± 2.9159−0.9870.327
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−2.923 ± 2.9159−1.0040.319
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.598 ± 2.934590.8850.379
Sex−3.294 ± 4.57317−0.720.481
Age1.199 ± 0.765171.5670.135
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)52.334 ± 153.373590.3410.734
R2 marginal: 8%, R2 conditional: 80%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.496 ± 0.10859−4.592<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.572 ± 0.10859−5.299<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.48 ± 0.10859−4.42<0.001
Sex−0.255 ± 0.45517−0.5610.582
Age−0.006 ± 0.07617−0.0890.929
0300–0400
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2688.979 ± 1630.46959−1.6490.104
R2 marginal: 8%, R2 conditional: 35%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.545 ± 3.178590.1710.864
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.033 ± 3.178590.010.991
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.323 ± 3.205590.7240.471
Sex−2.869 ± 4.87817−0.5880.564
Age1.377 ± 0.815171.6880.109
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)47.319 ± 150.273590.3140.754
R2 marginal: 4%, R2 conditional: 83%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.399 ± 0.10459−3.811<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.382 ± 0.10459−3.65<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.271 ± 0.10559−2.5760.012
Sex−0.189 ± 0.44617−0.4240.676
Age−0.004 ± 0.07517−0.0590.952
0400–0500
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2235.595 ± 1617.50359−1.3860.171
R2 marginal: 7%, R2 conditional: 26%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.453 ± 3.70359−0.1220.903
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)2.139 ± 3.703590.5770.565
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.132 ± 3.738590.570.57
Sex−5.934 ± 4.85517−1.2220.238
Age1.154 ± 0.809171.4270.171
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)51.501 ± 147.849590.3480.728
R2 marginal: 2%, R2 conditional: 81%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.295 ± 0.11859−2.4910.015
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.285 ± 0.11859−2.4070.019
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.185 ± 0.11959−1.5510.126
Sex−0.097 ± 0.43917−0.220.827
Age−0.006 ± 0.07317−0.090.928
0500–0600
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−1558.818 ± 1747.14859−0.8920.375
R2 marginal: 36%, R2 conditional: 38%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.378 ± 3.336590.1130.91
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.08 ± 0.116590.6870.494
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)1.237 ± 3.336590.370.712
Sex−4.315 ± 5.22517−0.8250.42
Age0.814 ± 0.42170.9320.364
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)74.265 ± 149.291590.4970.62
R2 marginal: 15%, R2 conditional: 83%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.222 ± 0.116591.9060.061
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.08 ± 0.116590.6870.494
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.02 ± 0.11459−0.1760.86
Sex−0.016 ± 0.44317−0.0380.97
Age−0.018 ± 0.07417−0.2440.81
ParametersEstimate ± SEdft-valueP
0000–0100
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−3477.243 ± 2379.9461−1.4610.149
R2 marginal: 55%, R2 conditional: 9%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−55.424 ± 6.46561−8.571<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−60.569 ± 6.46561−9.367<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−55.721 ± 6.4661−8.617<0.001
Sex−8.933 ± 7.17117−1.2450.229
Age1.803 ± 1.19171.5140.148
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)70.924 ± 161.338610.4390.661
R2 marginal: 24%, R2 conditional: 64%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−1.242 ± 0.1361−9.505<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.284 ± 0.1361−9.822<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−1.18 ± 0.1361−9.026<0.001
Sex−0.427 ± 0.47917−0.8920.384
Age−0.015 ± 0.0817−0.1940.848
0100–0200
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2568.699 ± 1903.81959−1.3490.182
R2 marginal: 21%, R2 conditional: 22%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.973 ± 0.12559−7.728<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.074 ± 0.12559−8.533<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.989 ± 0.12659−7.805<0.001
Sex−0.328 ± 0.46717−0.70.492
Age−0.016 ± 0.07817−0.2120.834
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)72.663 ± 157.464590.4610.646
R2 marginal: 20%, R2 conditional: 66%Period (1 Jan relative to 31. Dec)−0.973 ± 0.12459.144−7.83<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.074 ± 0.12459.144−8.645<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.989 ± 0.12559.036−7.908<0.001
Sex−0.328 ± 0.48817.04−0.6710.51
Age−0.016 ± 0.08216.964−0.2030.841
0200–0300
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2330.824 ± 1529.32959−1.5240.132
R2 marginal: 11%, R2 conditional: 36%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−2.872 ± 2.9159−0.9870.327
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−2.923 ± 2.9159−1.0040.319
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.598 ± 2.934590.8850.379
Sex−3.294 ± 4.57317−0.720.481
Age1.199 ± 0.765171.5670.135
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)52.334 ± 153.373590.3410.734
R2 marginal: 8%, R2 conditional: 80%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.496 ± 0.10859−4.592<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.572 ± 0.10859−5.299<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.48 ± 0.10859−4.42<0.001
Sex−0.255 ± 0.45517−0.5610.582
Age−0.006 ± 0.07617−0.0890.929
0300–0400
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2688.979 ± 1630.46959−1.6490.104
R2 marginal: 8%, R2 conditional: 35%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.545 ± 3.178590.1710.864
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.033 ± 3.178590.010.991
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.323 ± 3.205590.7240.471
Sex−2.869 ± 4.87817−0.5880.564
Age1.377 ± 0.815171.6880.109
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)47.319 ± 150.273590.3140.754
R2 marginal: 4%, R2 conditional: 83%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.399 ± 0.10459−3.811<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.382 ± 0.10459−3.65<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.271 ± 0.10559−2.5760.012
Sex−0.189 ± 0.44617−0.4240.676
Age−0.004 ± 0.07517−0.0590.952
0400–0500
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2235.595 ± 1617.50359−1.3860.171
R2 marginal: 7%, R2 conditional: 26%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.453 ± 3.70359−0.1220.903
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)2.139 ± 3.703590.5770.565
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.132 ± 3.738590.570.57
Sex−5.934 ± 4.85517−1.2220.238
Age1.154 ± 0.809171.4270.171
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)51.501 ± 147.849590.3480.728
R2 marginal: 2%, R2 conditional: 81%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.295 ± 0.11859−2.4910.015
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.285 ± 0.11859−2.4070.019
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.185 ± 0.11959−1.5510.126
Sex−0.097 ± 0.43917−0.220.827
Age−0.006 ± 0.07317−0.090.928
0500–0600
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−1558.818 ± 1747.14859−0.8920.375
R2 marginal: 36%, R2 conditional: 38%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.378 ± 3.336590.1130.91
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.08 ± 0.116590.6870.494
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)1.237 ± 3.336590.370.712
Sex−4.315 ± 5.22517−0.8250.42
Age0.814 ± 0.42170.9320.364
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)74.265 ± 149.291590.4970.62
R2 marginal: 15%, R2 conditional: 83%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.222 ± 0.116591.9060.061
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.08 ± 0.116590.6870.494
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.02 ± 0.11459−0.1760.86
Sex−0.016 ± 0.44317−0.0380.97
Age−0.018 ± 0.07417−0.2440.81

Significant factors are highlighted in bold. For each model, R2 marginal value, describing the proportion of variance explained by the fixed and R2 conditional value describing proportion of variance explained by the random effect (individual).

Table 1

Results of the general linear model investigating factors affecting heart rate (A) and body temperature (B) between 0000–0100, 0100–0200, 0200–0300, 0300–0400, 0400–0500 and 0500–0600

ParametersEstimate ± SEdft-valueP
0000–0100
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−3477.243 ± 2379.9461−1.4610.149
R2 marginal: 55%, R2 conditional: 9%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−55.424 ± 6.46561−8.571<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−60.569 ± 6.46561−9.367<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−55.721 ± 6.4661−8.617<0.001
Sex−8.933 ± 7.17117−1.2450.229
Age1.803 ± 1.19171.5140.148
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)70.924 ± 161.338610.4390.661
R2 marginal: 24%, R2 conditional: 64%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−1.242 ± 0.1361−9.505<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.284 ± 0.1361−9.822<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−1.18 ± 0.1361−9.026<0.001
Sex−0.427 ± 0.47917−0.8920.384
Age−0.015 ± 0.0817−0.1940.848
0100–0200
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2568.699 ± 1903.81959−1.3490.182
R2 marginal: 21%, R2 conditional: 22%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.973 ± 0.12559−7.728<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.074 ± 0.12559−8.533<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.989 ± 0.12659−7.805<0.001
Sex−0.328 ± 0.46717−0.70.492
Age−0.016 ± 0.07817−0.2120.834
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)72.663 ± 157.464590.4610.646
R2 marginal: 20%, R2 conditional: 66%Period (1 Jan relative to 31. Dec)−0.973 ± 0.12459.144−7.83<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.074 ± 0.12459.144−8.645<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.989 ± 0.12559.036−7.908<0.001
Sex−0.328 ± 0.48817.04−0.6710.51
Age−0.016 ± 0.08216.964−0.2030.841
0200–0300
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2330.824 ± 1529.32959−1.5240.132
R2 marginal: 11%, R2 conditional: 36%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−2.872 ± 2.9159−0.9870.327
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−2.923 ± 2.9159−1.0040.319
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.598 ± 2.934590.8850.379
Sex−3.294 ± 4.57317−0.720.481
Age1.199 ± 0.765171.5670.135
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)52.334 ± 153.373590.3410.734
R2 marginal: 8%, R2 conditional: 80%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.496 ± 0.10859−4.592<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.572 ± 0.10859−5.299<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.48 ± 0.10859−4.42<0.001
Sex−0.255 ± 0.45517−0.5610.582
Age−0.006 ± 0.07617−0.0890.929
0300–0400
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2688.979 ± 1630.46959−1.6490.104
R2 marginal: 8%, R2 conditional: 35%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.545 ± 3.178590.1710.864
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.033 ± 3.178590.010.991
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.323 ± 3.205590.7240.471
Sex−2.869 ± 4.87817−0.5880.564
Age1.377 ± 0.815171.6880.109
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)47.319 ± 150.273590.3140.754
R2 marginal: 4%, R2 conditional: 83%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.399 ± 0.10459−3.811<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.382 ± 0.10459−3.65<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.271 ± 0.10559−2.5760.012
Sex−0.189 ± 0.44617−0.4240.676
Age−0.004 ± 0.07517−0.0590.952
0400–0500
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2235.595 ± 1617.50359−1.3860.171
R2 marginal: 7%, R2 conditional: 26%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.453 ± 3.70359−0.1220.903
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)2.139 ± 3.703590.5770.565
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.132 ± 3.738590.570.57
Sex−5.934 ± 4.85517−1.2220.238
Age1.154 ± 0.809171.4270.171
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)51.501 ± 147.849590.3480.728
R2 marginal: 2%, R2 conditional: 81%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.295 ± 0.11859−2.4910.015
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.285 ± 0.11859−2.4070.019
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.185 ± 0.11959−1.5510.126
Sex−0.097 ± 0.43917−0.220.827
Age−0.006 ± 0.07317−0.090.928
0500–0600
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−1558.818 ± 1747.14859−0.8920.375
R2 marginal: 36%, R2 conditional: 38%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.378 ± 3.336590.1130.91
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.08 ± 0.116590.6870.494
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)1.237 ± 3.336590.370.712
Sex−4.315 ± 5.22517−0.8250.42
Age0.814 ± 0.42170.9320.364
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)74.265 ± 149.291590.4970.62
R2 marginal: 15%, R2 conditional: 83%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.222 ± 0.116591.9060.061
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.08 ± 0.116590.6870.494
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.02 ± 0.11459−0.1760.86
Sex−0.016 ± 0.44317−0.0380.97
Age−0.018 ± 0.07417−0.2440.81
ParametersEstimate ± SEdft-valueP
0000–0100
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−3477.243 ± 2379.9461−1.4610.149
R2 marginal: 55%, R2 conditional: 9%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−55.424 ± 6.46561−8.571<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−60.569 ± 6.46561−9.367<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−55.721 ± 6.4661−8.617<0.001
Sex−8.933 ± 7.17117−1.2450.229
Age1.803 ± 1.19171.5140.148
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)70.924 ± 161.338610.4390.661
R2 marginal: 24%, R2 conditional: 64%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−1.242 ± 0.1361−9.505<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.284 ± 0.1361−9.822<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−1.18 ± 0.1361−9.026<0.001
Sex−0.427 ± 0.47917−0.8920.384
Age−0.015 ± 0.0817−0.1940.848
0100–0200
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2568.699 ± 1903.81959−1.3490.182
R2 marginal: 21%, R2 conditional: 22%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.973 ± 0.12559−7.728<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.074 ± 0.12559−8.533<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.989 ± 0.12659−7.805<0.001
Sex−0.328 ± 0.46717−0.70.492
Age−0.016 ± 0.07817−0.2120.834
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)72.663 ± 157.464590.4610.646
R2 marginal: 20%, R2 conditional: 66%Period (1 Jan relative to 31. Dec)−0.973 ± 0.12459.144−7.83<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−1.074 ± 0.12459.144−8.645<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.989 ± 0.12559.036−7.908<0.001
Sex−0.328 ± 0.48817.04−0.6710.51
Age−0.016 ± 0.08216.964−0.2030.841
0200–0300
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2330.824 ± 1529.32959−1.5240.132
R2 marginal: 11%, R2 conditional: 36%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−2.872 ± 2.9159−0.9870.327
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−2.923 ± 2.9159−1.0040.319
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.598 ± 2.934590.8850.379
Sex−3.294 ± 4.57317−0.720.481
Age1.199 ± 0.765171.5670.135
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)52.334 ± 153.373590.3410.734
R2 marginal: 8%, R2 conditional: 80%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.496 ± 0.10859−4.592<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.572 ± 0.10859−5.299<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.48 ± 0.10859−4.42<0.001
Sex−0.255 ± 0.45517−0.5610.582
Age−0.006 ± 0.07617−0.0890.929
0300–0400
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2688.979 ± 1630.46959−1.6490.104
R2 marginal: 8%, R2 conditional: 35%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.545 ± 3.178590.1710.864
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.033 ± 3.178590.010.991
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.323 ± 3.205590.7240.471
Sex−2.869 ± 4.87817−0.5880.564
Age1.377 ± 0.815171.6880.109
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)47.319 ± 150.273590.3140.754
R2 marginal: 4%, R2 conditional: 83%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.399 ± 0.10459−3.811<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.382 ± 0.10459−3.65<0.001
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.271 ± 0.10559−2.5760.012
Sex−0.189 ± 0.44617−0.4240.676
Age−0.004 ± 0.07517−0.0590.952
0400–0500
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−2235.595 ± 1617.50359−1.3860.171
R2 marginal: 7%, R2 conditional: 26%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.453 ± 3.70359−0.1220.903
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)2.139 ± 3.703590.5770.565
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)2.132 ± 3.738590.570.57
Sex−5.934 ± 4.85517−1.2220.238
Age1.154 ± 0.809171.4270.171
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)51.501 ± 147.849590.3480.728
R2 marginal: 2%, R2 conditional: 81%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)−0.295 ± 0.11859−2.4910.015
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)−0.285 ± 0.11859−2.4070.019
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.185 ± 0.11959−1.5510.126
Sex−0.097 ± 0.43917−0.220.827
Age−0.006 ± 0.07317−0.090.928
0500–0600
(A) Heart rate(Intercept)−1558.818 ± 1747.14859−0.8920.375
R2 marginal: 36%, R2 conditional: 38%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.378 ± 3.336590.1130.91
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.08 ± 0.116590.6870.494
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)1.237 ± 3.336590.370.712
Sex−4.315 ± 5.22517−0.8250.42
Age0.814 ± 0.42170.9320.364
(B) Body temperature(Intercept)74.265 ± 149.291590.4970.62
R2 marginal: 15%, R2 conditional: 83%Period (1 Jan relative to 31 Dec)0.222 ± 0.116591.9060.061
Period (1 Jan relative to Dec average)0.08 ± 0.116590.6870.494
Period (1 Jan relative to Jan average)−0.02 ± 0.11459−0.1760.86
Sex−0.016 ± 0.44317−0.0380.97
Age−0.018 ± 0.07417−0.2440.81

Significant factors are highlighted in bold. For each model, R2 marginal value, describing the proportion of variance explained by the fixed and R2 conditional value describing proportion of variance explained by the random effect (individual).

Daily course of (A) heart rates and (B) body temperature. Solid lines are means, shaded areas indicate standard error between individuals.
Figure 1

Daily course of (A) heart rates and (B) body temperature. Solid lines are means, shaded areas indicate standard error between individuals.

Mean heart rate (A) and body temperature (B) between 0000 and 0100. Box plots show the median and the interquartile range from the 25th to the 75th percentiles. Whiskers show the bottom 5% to the upper 95% confidence interval. Each data point is an individual’s mean value.
Figure 2

Mean heart rate (A) and body temperature (B) between 0000 and 0100. Box plots show the median and the interquartile range from the 25th to the 75th percentiles. Whiskers show the bottom 5% to the upper 95% confidence interval. Each data point is an individual’s mean value.

2.3. Results

Between 0000 and 0100, heart rate and body temperature were significantly higher in the first hour of the new year, compared with the same hour on the 31st of December, the average during December and the average during January (Table 1; Figs 1 and 2). Compared with average values during the month of December, heart rate increased by 96% and body temperature increased by 3% in the first hour of the new year. Heart rate and body temperature was not significantly associated with sex and age (Table 1). A similar pattern arose between 0100 and 0200, with heart rate and body temperature being significantly higher on the 1st of January compared with the other phases, but sex and age not having an effect (Table 1), reflecting a 31% increase in heart rate and 3% increase in body temperature compared with average December values. Between 0200–0300 and 0300–0400, heart rate was not significantly associated with any factors; however, body temperature was still significantly higher on the 1st of January compared with other phases. Between 0400 and 0500, body temperature was significantly higher on the 1st of January compared with 31st of December and average December values but not average January values. None of the investigated factors significantly affected body temperature between 0500 and 0600 (Table 1).

3. Discussion

In the present study we describe a significant increase in heart rate and body temperature in response to a major regularly re-occurring anthropogenic disturbance, New Year’s Eve firework celebrations. Heart rate was significantly increased from 0000 to 0200 compared with control periods. Similarly, also body temperature increased in response to the disturbance and only returned to baseline between 0500 and 0600 on the 1st of January. Although fireworks are well described as a major stressor in pets and domestic animals (Levine and Mills, 2008; Dale et al., 2010; Gronqvist et al., 2016; Gates et al., 2019; Gähwiler et al., 2020), effects on wildlife remain largely unexplored (Shamoun-Baranes et al., 2011). We cannot conclusively tell whether the implanted geese were responding to the noise pollution or light pollution caused by the fireworks or to a combination of both. It has to be noted that the geese in our study were generally habituated to human presence and, for example, did not significantly increase heart rate while being approached by a familiar human (Wascher et al., 2011). We therefore need to consider the possibility that the observed changes in heart rate might not be representative of what would be observed in unhabituated wild animals, who might display even more pronounced responses or avoid areas in which fireworks occur altogether.

The physiological response we presently describe is likely associated with increased activity and a behavioural response. Anecdotally greylag geese from the studied population have been observed to take flight during the fireworks and circle over the lake Almsee, which is the roosting site (C.A.F. Wascher, personal observation). Hence, we suggest that the increase in heart rate in response to fireworks is likely to be caused by both, increased physical activity and psychological stress (Wascher, 2021). Wild birds have been shown to take flight for 45 minutes during New Year’s Eve fireworks (Shamoun-Baranes et al., 2011); our results suggest an even longer response than this, heart rate only returned to baseline levels between 0200 and 0300, indicating that geese respond to the fireworks for 2 hours, which may also be due to the fact that firework activity does not sharply end a few minutes after midnight, but may occur as single crackers or rockets 1–2 hours into the new year. As we did not conduct behavioural observations, we cannot tell if geese took flight, for how long the geese were in the air and if all focal individuals did so; however, in different contexts, heart rate has been described to return to baseline levels within seconds after a stressor (Wascher et al., 2008b; Wascher et al., 2011; Wascher, 2021).

We have previously, shown that heart rate and body temperature vary—indicative of energy expenditure—profoundly across annual and daily cycles, generally decreasing during winter as compared with summer and significantly increasing during the reproductive period (Wascher et al., 2018). Average daily heart rates in this previous study varied on average 22% between summer and winter, whereas body temperature was about 1° lower in winter compared with summer values. Here, we show heart rate increasing by 96% and body temperature increasing by 3% (about 1°) in the first hour of the new year, suggesting that New Year’s Eve celebration present a major stressor affecting individuals energy expenditure. Further, social contexts have been shown to be strong modulators of heart rate (Wascher et al., 2008b; Wascher et al., 2014) and of course, heart rate increases during locomotion and evidently stressful situations (i.e. a dog on the leash, the geese were not habituated to; Wascher et al., 2011; Wascher and Kotrschal, 2013). Disturbance in response to fireworks is not only very likely stressful, but also presents an energetic cost to the geese (Butler and Woakes, 1980; Wascher et al., 2018; Halsey et al., 2019) and causes a disruption of their night rest period (Raap et al., 2017; Aulsebrook et al., 2020; Grunst et al., 2021); in addition, birds are at risk of becoming disoriented (Van Doren et al., 2017).

Although there were pronounced individual differences in heart rate and body temperature responses, these were not significantly affected by sex and age. Differences in heart rate between the sexes have previously been described as depending on season and only apparent during the reproductive season (Wascher et al., 2018). Outside the reproductive season, differences between the sexes are context dependent: for example, male individuals having a higher heart rate during agonistic encounters compared with females (Wascher et al., 2012). We did not describe an effect of age on the physiological response, showing no indication of the geese to either habituate or sensitize to the fireworks over time (Riemer, 2020). Both predictability and unpredictability as well as personality have been shown to affect behavioural and physiological stress response and depending on context can increase or decrease the response to stimuli (Bassett and Buchanan-Smith, 2007). However, it is questionable whether wild animals perceive New Year’s Eve celebrations as predictable events, as they only occur once a year.

To conclude, our results show that New Year’s Eve fireworks cause a substantial physiological response in greylag geese. A better understanding of the effects of anthropogenic disturbance onto wildlife can be useful for wildlife conservation attempts, and our study is one of few showing negative effects of fireworks onto wildlife. A clear recommendation from our and other studies is to avoid fireworks in nature areas altogether.

Declarations

Ethical Approval

This study was approved under an animal experiment license issued by the Austrian Ministry of Science GZ68.210/41-BrGT/2003. Before and after completion of the study, geese remained in the non-migratory, free-roaming flock of greylag geese in the Almtal.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Funding

This work has been funded by Austrian Science Fund (P15766-B03 and P18744-B03 to K.K.). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Author Contributions

C.A.F.W. conceptualized the study, curated and analysed the data. C.A.F.W., W.A. and K.K. wrote the original draft of the paper and contributed to reviewing and editing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

The data and R code are provided via the platform Zenodo (10.5281/zenodo.6752634). https://zenodo.org/record/6752634#.YrhJvBPMLeo

Acknowledgements

We thank Franz Schober, Gerhard Fluch and Thomas Paumann for technical support; and Wolfgang Zenker and Ulrike Auer for veterinarian support. We acknowledge ongoing support from the ‘Verein der Förderer’ and the Herzog von Cumberland Stiftung.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Conservation Physiology online.

References

Alam
MM
,
Hashem
MA
,
Rahman
MM
,
Hossain
MM
,
Haque
MR
,
Sobhan
Z
,
Islam
MS
(
2013
)
Effect of heat stress on behavior, physiological and blood parameters of goat
.
Progress Agricult
22
:
37
45
.

Al-Haidary
AA
(
2004
)
Physiological responses of naimey sheep to heat stress challenge under semi-arid environments
.
Int J Agricult Biol
6
:
307
309
.

Andersen
R
,
Linnell
JDC
,
Langvatn
R
(
1996
)
Short term behavioural and physiological response of moose Alces alces to military disturbance in Norway
.
Biol Conserv
77
:
169
176
.

Aulsebrook
AE
,
Connelly
F
,
Johnsson
RD
,
Jones
TM
,
Mulder
RA
,
Hall
ML
,
Vyssotski
AL
,
Lesku
JA
(
2020
)
White and amber light at night disrupt sleep physiology in birds
.
Curr Biol
30
:
3657
3663.e5
.

Bartholomew
GA
,
Leitner
P
,
Nelson
JE
(
1964
)
Body temperature, oxygen consumption, and heart rate in three species of Australian flying foxes
.
Physiol Zool
37
:
179
198
.

Bartoń
K
(
2019
) MuMIn: multi-model inference. .

Bassett
L
,
Buchanan-Smith
HM
(
2007
)
Effects of predictability on the welfare of captive animals
.
Appl Anim Behav Sci
102
:
223
245
.

Bejder
L
,
Samuels
A
,
Whitehead
H
,
Gales
N
(
2006
)
Interpreting short-term behavioural responses to disturbance within a longitudinal perspective
.
Anim Behav
72
:
1149
1158
.

von
Borell
E
,
Langbein
J
,
Després
G
,
Hansen
S
,
Leterrier
C
,
Marchant-Forde
J
,
Marchant-Forde
R
,
Minero
M
,
Mohr
E
,
Prunier
A
et al. (
2007
)
Heart rate variability as a measure of autonomic regulation of cardiac activity for assessing stress and welfare in farm animals—a review
.
Physiol Behav
92
:
293
316
.

Borneman
TE
,
Rose
ET
,
Simons
TR
(
2014
)
Minimal changes in heart rate of incubating American oystercatchers (Haematopus palliatus) in response to human activity
.
The Condor
116
:
493
503
.

Butler
PJ
,
Woakes
AJ
(
1980
)
Heart rate, respiratory frequency and wing beat frequency of free flying barnacle geese Branta leucopsis
.
J Exp Biol
85
:
213
226
.

Cabanac
AJ
,
Guillemette
M
(
2001
)
Temperature and heart rate as stress indicators of handled common eider
.
Physiol Behav
74
:
475
479
.

Carere
C
,
Vanoers
K
(
2004
)
Shy and bold great tits (Parus major): body temperature and breath rate in response to handling stress
.
Physiol Behav
82
:
905
912
.

Corradini
A
,
Randles
M
,
Pedrotti
L
,
van
Loon
E
,
Passoni
G
,
Oberosler
V
,
Rovero
F
,
Tattoni
C
,
Ciolli
M
,
Cagnacci
F
(
2021
)
Effects of cumulated outdoor activity on wildlife habitat use
.
Biol Conserv
253
: 108818.

Daan
S
,
Deerenberg
C
,
Dijkstra
C
(
1996
)
Increased daily work precipitates natural death in the kestrel
.
J Anim Ecol
65
:
539
.

Dale
A
,
Walker
J
,
Farnworth
M
,
Morrissey
S
,
Waran
N
(
2010
)
A survey of owners’ perceptions of fear of fireworks in a sample of dogs and cats in New Zealand
.
N Z Vet J
58
:
286
291
.

De Jong
IC
,
Voorst
SV
,
Ehlhardt
DA
,
Blokhuis
HJ
(
2002
)
Effects of restricted feeding on physiological stress parameters in growing broiler breeders
.
Br Poultry Sci
43
:
157
168
.

Ditmer
MA
,
Vincent
JB
,
Werden
LK
,
Tanner
JC
,
Laske
TG
,
Iaizzo
PA
,
Garshelis
DL
,
Fieberg
JR
(
2015
)
Bears show a physiological but limited behavioral response to unmanned aerial vehicles
.
Curr Biol
25
:
2278
2283
.

Dominoni
D
,
Smit
JAH
,
Visser
ME
,
Halfwerk
W
(
2020
)
Multisensory pollution: artificial light at night and anthropogenic noise have interactive effects on activity patterns of great tits (Parus major)
.
Environ Pollut
256
: 113314.

Ellenberg
U
,
Mattern
T
,
Seddon
PJ
(
2013
)
Heart rate responses provide an objective evaluation of human disturbance stimuli in breeding birds
.
Conserv Physiol
1:cot013
.

Erbe
C
,
Dunlop
R
,
Dolman
S
(
2018
)
Effects of noise on marine mammals.
. In
Effects of anthropogenic noise on animals
(pp. 277-309). Springer, New York, NY
.

Ferraro
DM
,
Le
M-LT
,
Francis
CD
(
2020
)
Combined effect of anthropogenic noise and artificial night lighting negatively affect Western bluebird chick development
.
The Condor
122
: duaa037.

Fowler
GS
(
1999
)
Behavioral and hormonal responses of Magellanic penguins (Spheniscus magellanicus) to tourism and nest site visitation
.
Biol Conserv
90
:
143
149
.

Fox
J
,
Weisberg
S
(
2011
)
An {R} Companion to Applied Regression
, Ed2nd.
Sage Publications
,
California

Gähwiler
S
,
Bremhorst
A
,
Tóth
K
,
Riemer
S
(
2020
)
Fear expressions of dogs during New Year fireworks: a video analysis
.
Sci Rep
10
:
16035
.

Gates
M
,
Zito
S
,
Walker
J
,
Dale
A
(
2019
)
Owner perceptions and management of the adverse behavioural effects of fireworks on companion animals: an update
.
N Z Vet J
67
:
323
328
.

Gaynor
KM
,
Hojnowski
CE
,
Carter
NH
,
Brashares
JS
(
2018
)
The influence of human disturbance on wildlife nocturnality
.
Science
360
:
1232
1235
.

Graham
AL
,
Cooke
SJ
(
2008
)
The effects of noise disturbance from various recreational boating activities common to inland waters on the cardiac physiology of a freshwater fish, the largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides)
.
Aquat Conserv
18
:
1315
1324
.

Gronqvist
G
,
Rogers
C
,
Gee
E
(
2016
)
The management of horses during fireworks in New Zealand
.
Animals
6
:
20
.

Grunst
ML
,
Grunst
AS
,
Pinxten
R
,
Eens
M
(
2021
)
Variable and consistent traffic noise negatively affect the sleep behavior of a free-living songbird
.
Sci Total Environ
778
: 146338.

Halsey
LG
,
Green
JA
,
Twiss
SD
,
Arnold
W
,
Burthe
SJ
,
Butler
PJ
,
Cooke
SJ
,
Grémillet
D
,
Ruf
T
,
Hicks
O
et al. (
2019
)
Flexibility, variability and constraint in energy management patterns across vertebrate taxa revealed by long‐term heart rate measurements
.
Funct Ecol
33
:
260
272
.

Jerem
P
,
Mathews
F
(
2021
)
Trends and knowledge gaps in field research investigating effects of anthropogenic noise
.
Conserv Biol
35
:
15
.

Kleist
NJ
,
Guralnick
RP
,
Cruz
A
,
Lowry
CA
,
Francis
CD
(
2018
)
Chronic anthropogenic noise disrupts glucocorticoid signaling and has multiple effects on fitness in an avian community
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci
115
:
E648
E657
.

Le Grand
L
,
Thorsen
NH
,
Fuchs
B
,
Evans
AL
,
Laske
TG
,
Arnemo
JM
,
Sæbø
S
,
Støen
O-G
(
2019
)
Behavioral and physiological responses of Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos) to dog hunts and human encounters
.
Front Ecol Evol
7
:
134
.

Levine
ED
,
Mills
DS
(
2008
)
Long-term follow-up of the efficacy of a behavioural treatment programme for dogs with firework fears
.
Vet Rec
162
:
657
659
.

MacArthur
RA
,
Johnston
RH
,
Geist
V
(
1979
)
Factors influencing heart rate in free-ranging bighorn sheep: a physiological approach to the study of wildlife harassment
.
Can J Zool
57
:
2010
2021
.

Marion
S
,
Davies
A
,
Demšar
U
,
Irvine
RJ
,
Stephens
PA
,
Long
J
(
2020
)
A systematic review of methods for studying the impacts of outdoor recreation on terrestrial wildlife
.
Glob Ecol Conserv
22
: e00917.

McNab
BK
(
2022
)
The physiological ecology of vertebrates: a view from energetics
.
Cornell University Press
,
Ithaca, NY

Mesteig
K
,
Tyler
NJC
,
Blix
AS
(
2000
)
Seasonal changes in heart rate and food intake in reindeer (Rangifer tarandus tarandus)
.
Acta Physiol Scand
170
:
145
151
.

Nakagawa
S
,
Schielzeth
H
(
2013
)
A general and simple method for obtainingR2from generalized linear mixed-effects models
.
Methods Ecol Evol
4
:
133
142
.

New
L
,
Clark
J
,
Costa
D
,
Fleishman
E
,
Hindell
M
,
Klanjšček
T
,
Lusseau
D
,
Kraus
S
,
McMahon
C
,
Robinson
P
et al. (
2014
)
Using short-term measures of behaviour to estimate long-term fitness of southern elephant seals
.
Mar Ecol Prog Ser
496
:
99
108
.

Oulton
LJ
,
Haviland
V
,
Brown
C
(
2013
)
Predator recognition in rainbowfish, Melanotaenia duboulayi, embryos
.
PLoS One
8
: e76061.

Pinheiro
J
,
Bates
D
,
DebRoy
S
,
Sarkar
D
(
2020
)
nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models
. .

Raap
T
,
Sun
J
,
Pinxten
R
,
Eens
M
(
2017
)
Disruptive effects of light pollution on sleep in free-living birds: season and/or light intensity-dependent?
Behav Processes
144
:
13
19
.

Riemer
S
(
2020
)
Effectiveness of treatments for firework fears in dogs
.
J Vet Behav
37
:
61
70
.

Rodewald
A
,
Gansloßer
U
,
Kölpin
T
(
2014
)
Influence of fireworks on zoo animals: studying different species at the zoopark Erfurt during the classic nights
.
Int Zoo News
61
:
8
.

Santicchia
F
,
Wauters
LA
,
Dantzer
B
,
Westrick
SE
,
Ferrari
N
,
Romeo
C
,
Palme
R
,
Preatoni
DG
,
Martinoli
A
(
2020
)
Relationships between personality traits and the physiological stress response in a wild mammal
.
Curr Zool
66
:
197
204
.

Savory
C
,
Kostal
L
(
2006
)
Is expression of some behaviours associated with de-arousal in restricted-fed chickens?
Physiol Behav
88
:
473
478
.

Scheiber
IBR
,
Kralj
S
,
Kotrschal
K
(
2005
)
Sampling effort/frequency necessary to infer individual acute stress responses from fecal analysis in greylag geese (Anser anser)
.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
1046
:
154
167
.

Shamoun-Baranes
J
,
Dokter
AM
,
van
Gasteren
H
,
van
Loon
EE
,
Leijnse
H
,
Bouten
W
(
2011
)
Birds flee en mass from New Year’s eve fireworks
.
Behav Ecol
22
:
1173
1177
.

Talling
JC
,
Waran
NK
,
Wathes
CM
,
Lines
JA
(
1996
)
Behavioural and physiological responses of pigs to sound
.
Appl Anim Behav Sci
48
:
187
201
.

Van Doren
BM
,
Horton
KG
,
Dokter
AM
,
Klinck
H
,
Elbin
SB
,
Farnsworth
A
(
2017
)
High-intensity urban light installation dramatically alters nocturnal bird migration
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci
114
:
11175
11180
.

von
Holst
D
(
1998
) The concept of stress and its relevance for animal behavior. In
Advances in the Study of Behavior
,
Vol. 27
.
Cambridge, Massachusetts
,
Academic Press
, pp.
1
131
.

Wascher
CAF
(
2021
)
Heart rate as a measure of emotional arousal in evolutionary biology
.
Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci
376
:
20200479
.

Wascher
,
C. A. F.
, and
K.
Kotrschal
(
2013
). The costs of sociality measured through heart rate modulation. In
The Social Life of Greylag Geese: Patterns, Mechanisms and Evolutionary Function in an Avian Model System
, pp.
142
155
.
Cambridge University Press
,
Cambridge
.

Wascher
CAF
,
Kotrschal
K
,
Arnold
W
(
2018
)
Free-living greylag geese adjust their heart rates and body core temperatures to season and reproductive context
.
Sci Rep
8
:
2142
.

Wascher
CAF
,
Scheiber
IBR
,
Kotrschal
K
(
2008a
)
Heart rate modulation in bystanding geese watching social and non-social events
.
Proc R Soc B Biol Sci
275
:
1653
1659
.

Wascher
CAF
,
Scheiber
IBR
,
Braun
A
,
Kotrschal
K
(
2011
)
Heart rate responses to induced challenge situations in greylag geese (Anser anser)
.
J Comp Psychol
125
:
116
119
.

Wascher
CAF
,
Valdez
JW
,
Núñez Cebrián
C
,
Baglione
V
,
Canestrari
D
(
2014
)
Social factors modulating attention patterns in carrion crows
.
Behaviour
151
:
555
572
.

Wascher
CAF
,
Weiß
BM
,
Arnold
W
,
Kotrschal
K
(
2012
)
Physiological implications of pair-bond status in greylag geese
.
Biol Lett
8
:
347
350
.

Wascher
CAF
,
Arnold
W
,
Kotrschal
K
(
2008b
)
Heart rate modulation by social contexts in greylag geese (Anser anser)
.
J Comp Psychol
122
:
100
107
.

Weimerskirch
H
,
Shaffer
SA
,
Mabille
G
,
Martin
J
,
Boutard
O
,
Rouanet
JL
(
2002
)
Heart rate and energy expenditure of incubating wandering albatrosses: basal levels, natural variation, and the effects of human disturbance
.
Br J Exp Biol
205
:
475
483
.

Willems
JS
,
Phillips
JN
,
Francis
CD
(
2022
)
Artificial light at night and anthropogenic noise alter the foraging activity and structure of vertebrate communities
.
Sci Total Environ
805
: 150223.

Zuur
AF
,
Ieno
EN
,
Walker
NJ
,
Saveliev
AA
,
Smith
GM
2009
.
Mixed Effects Models and Extension in Ecology With R
.
Springer
,
New York
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Editor: Steven Cooke
Steven Cooke
Editor
Search for other works by this author on: