Abstract

Animal-borne telemetry devices have become a popular and valuable means for studying the cryptic lives of cetaceans. Evaluating the effect of capture, handling and tagging procedures remains largely unassessed across species. Here, we examine the effect of capture, handling and tagging activities on an iconic Arctic cetacean, the narwhal (Monodon monoceros), which has previously been shown to exhibit an extreme response to extended capture and handling. Using accelerometry-derived metrics of behaviour, including activity level, energy expenditure and swimming activity, we quantify the post-release responses and time to recovery of 19 individuals following capture and tagging activities considering the intrinsic covariates of sex and individual size and the extrinsic covariates of handling time and presence of a ‘bolt-on’ satellite telemetry device. From accelerometer-derived behaviour, most narwhals appeared to return to mean baseline behaviour (recovery) within 24 hours after release, which was supported by longer-term measures of diving data. None of the covariates measured, however, had an effect on the time individuals took to recover following release. Using generalized additive models to describe changes in behaviour over time, we found handling time to be a significant predictor of activity levels, energy expenditure and swimming behaviour following release. Individuals held for the longest period (>40 min) were found to display the largest effect in behaviour immediately following release with respect to swimming behaviour and activity levels. We also found some support for relationships between activity levels, energy expenditure and swimming activity and two other covariates: sex and the attachment of a bolt-on configuration satellite tags. Our results indicate that narwhals recover relatively quickly following capture, handling and tagging procedures, but we suggest that researchers should minimize handling time and further investigation is needed on how to mitigate potential effects of bolt-on satellite tags in these sensitive species.

Introduction

Telemetry technology has advanced substantially over the past few decades resulting in sophisticated electronic tags or tag packages that are now smaller than ever (Evans et al., 2013; Lennox et al., 2017). Animal-borne telemetry devices are particularly useful for the study of aquatic animals for which direct observation is often prohibited by a cryptic lifestyle or for those that live in remote and largely inaccessible environments (Walker et al., 2012; Hussey et al., 2015). To date, telemetry devices have provided unprecedented insights into migration routes and timing (e.g. Hauser et al. 2014), diving behaviour (e.g. Owen et al., 2019), three-dimensional movement (e.g. Fahlman et al., 2008), swimming speed (e.g. Williams et al., 2004) and physiology (e.g. Andrews and Enstipp, 2016). While these data are proving powerful in management and conservation contexts (Brooks et al., 2017; Hays et al., 2019), there is increasing awareness of animal welfare related to invasive tagging procedures that has naturally led to a call from regulatory agencies for the development of taxon-specific guidelines for their application (Gales et al., 2009). Many refinement frameworks also highlight the need to assess considerations on a species-specific level (Hawkins, 2004) as well as across all aspects of experimental design for biologging studies (Casper, 2009). As a result, several expert groups pooled collective knowledge from field activities, as well as previous work assessing the use and placement of telemetry devices, to create best practice recommendations for pinnipeds (Horning et al., 2017b, 2019) and cetaceans (Andrews et al., 2019). These best-practice recommendations provide an important framework moving forward and emphasize the need for data to examine how animal handling and the placement of telemetry devices potentially impact health, behaviour and survival of marine mammals.

Cetaceans, especially, have seen a rapid increase in the number of individuals fitted with telemetry devices over recent years (McIntyre, 2014; Andrews et al., 2019). These telemetry devices are generally attached using either a non-invasive method, such as suction cup mounting (Goldbogen et al., 2013), or through more invasive anchored mounts along the dorsal fin or ridge, such as the ‘limpet’-style tag (Andrews et al., 2015), and can often be attached without directly restraining individuals by using a pole or cross-bow to achieve short-term (days) and medium-term (weeks) deployment durations. Attachment techniques for long-term deployments (months) using ‘bolt-on’ configurations, however, often require capture and handling (Balmer et al., 2011; Heide-Jørgensen et al., 2017). While researchers follow best practices for capture, handling and tag attachment, approved through Animal Care Committees in respective organizations, assessments of the impact of these procedures on cetaceans are limited (Andrews et al. 2019).

Narwhals (Monodon monoceros) are an iconic medium-sized odontocete and a culturally important species ranging across much of the Eastern Arctic. Narwhals exhibit sexual dimorphism, both in terms of body size and most notably, by the presence of a tusk that primarily occurs in males (Reeves and Tracey, 1980; Petersen et al., 2012; Heide-Jørgensen, 2018). In the Canadian Arctic, narwhals typically spend the largely ice-free summer months in and around fjords, inlets and sounds (Heide-Jørgensen et al., 2013). Using animal-borne telemetry devices, previous work has documented their yearly life cycle and movement patterns (Heide-Jørgensen et al., 2002, 2006; Laidre and Heide-Jørgensen, 2005), deep-diving capabilities (Laidre and Heide-Jørgensen, 2005; Heide-Jørgensen et al., 2015; Watt et al., 2015), responses to predators (Laidre et al., 2006; Breed et al., 2017), novel behaviours such as upside-down swimming at depth (Dietz et al., 2007) and potential for extreme sensitivity to future climate scenarios (Williams et al., 2011). However, studies have also revealed disturbance associated with extended animal handling during the capture and tag attachment process (Williams et al., 2011, 2017a). The response of individuals to handling periods greater than 60 minutes was characterized by extreme bradycardia coupled with higher swimming effort and deep diving (Williams et al., 2017a). Such a strong response by narwhals post-tagging is concerning and highlights the need for a greater understanding of the effects of capture, handling and tagging beyond the first 90 minutes post-release that were the focus of Williams et al. (2017a).

Animal-borne accelerometers provide a tool to quantify behavioural effects following capture and tagging events through recording high-resolution, continuous three-dimensional movement data over time. Several metrics of behaviour have been extracted from accelerometry data across a wide variety of species, including changes in activity levels (norm of jerk, defined as the square-root of the sum of squares for differential of acceleration in all axes; Ydesen et al. 2014; Barkley et al. 2020), approximate energy expenditure (Vectorial Dynamic Body Acceleration, VeDBA; Broell et al., 2016; Stothart et al., 2016; Udyawer et al., 2017) and, most often for aquatic animals including narwhal, swimming behaviour (tail-beat frequency or stroke rate; Williams et al. 2004, 2017a, b; Sato et al., 2007; Martin Lopez et al., 2015; Ladds et al., 2017). Diving response has also been shown to be a key indicator of how individuals and populations respond to disturbance (DeRuiter et al., 2013; Miller et al., 2015; Williams et al., 2015; van Beest et al., 2018; Warren et al., 2020). Quantifying the degree of departure of animal behaviour from the normal or baseline values following capture and tagging events and the length over which detrimental effects modify behaviour remains an important part of holistically evaluating research activities (Wilson and McMahon 2006). Through the use of data from individuals equipped with biologging devices, it is possible to assess across- and within-individual changes in behaviour following capture and tagging using the above metrics. Specifically, a return to a long-term mean in a given behavioural index (i.e. baseline behaviour), provides an indicator of recovery for that metric following invasive research activities.

We used a combination of behavioural metrics derived from accelerometers coupled with dive data to characterize both the post-release behaviour and the time to return to baseline behaviour of narwhals following capture and tagging events across three independent field programs. Specifically, we aimed to characterize activity levels, energy expenditure and swimming behaviour across- and within-individuals following release from capture and tagging events to assess the effect of handling time on these behavioural metrics for the first 72 hours post-release (defined by the accelerometer sampling duration). Furthermore, we examined time to recovery to baseline behaviour (measured here as the return of individual deviance of baseline behaviour to zero, baseline measured beyond 36 hours after release) and assessed if key covariates (sex, handling time and the presence of a bolt-on tag) may have influenced behaviour and recovery times. By using longer-term measures of dive behaviour (baseline measured 7–14 days post-release), we further aimed to determine if the relatively short behavioural window provided by acceleration data accurately captured the period of recovery in narwhals.

Methods

Study site and capture, handling and tagging protocols

Over three summer seasons in 2012, 2017 and 2018, a total of 29 narwhals were captured and equipped with satellite transmitting tags and/or recoverable biologging devices in Tremblay Sound (72°21.389 N, −81°05.855 W) on northern Baffin Island, Nunavut, Canada (see Table 1). Tagging efforts were part of a larger program to monitor ecosystem-wide health in the high Arctic led by Fisheries and Oceans Canada’s long-term marine mammal monitoring program.

Summary of study animals, including animal sex, animal length (cm), age class (adults classified as body length, >300 cm; Hay 1984), minutes held follow capture and prior to release (tcap) and the presence of a satellite tag attached via ‘bolt-on’ configuration. The type of accelerometer (ACC-type) is also included. The number of hours of acceleration data recorded while attached to each narwhal is also included (hours); however, several tags were programmed with delays in recording for a period of up to 4 hours (indicated by the *)

Table 1
Summary of study animals, including animal sex, animal length (cm), age class (adults classified as body length, >300 cm; Hay 1984), minutes held follow capture and prior to release (tcap) and the presence of a satellite tag attached via ‘bolt-on’ configuration. The type of accelerometer (ACC-type) is also included. The number of hours of acceleration data recorded while attached to each narwhal is also included (hours); however, several tags were programmed with delays in recording for a period of up to 4 hours (indicated by the *)
AnimalSexLength (cm)Age classtcap (min)Bolt-on tag?HoursACC type
12–03F390Adult30Y5TDR-DD
12–05F262Juvenile30Y15TDR-DD
17–03F400Adult36Y182*MBL
17–04M432Adult47Y38*MBL
17–05M488Adult67Y48*MBL
17–08F375Adult37Y83Acousonde
17–09F385Adult36Y70*Acousonde
17–10M400Adult30N189*MBL
17–11F390Adult39Y12*Acousonde
17–12F425Adult25Y15*Acousonde
17–13M298Juvenile36Y26*Acousonde
17–14M230Juvenile18N3*Acousonde
17–18F370Adult31Y11Acousonde
17–19F380Adult35Y15Acousonde
17–20F408Adult35Y22Acousonde
18–02F357Adult34Y112*Acousonde
18–03M303Adult35N19*Acousonde
18–04F382Adult23N107*Acousonde
AnimalSexLength (cm)Age classtcap (min)Bolt-on tag?HoursACC type
12–03F390Adult30Y5TDR-DD
12–05F262Juvenile30Y15TDR-DD
17–03F400Adult36Y182*MBL
17–04M432Adult47Y38*MBL
17–05M488Adult67Y48*MBL
17–08F375Adult37Y83Acousonde
17–09F385Adult36Y70*Acousonde
17–10M400Adult30N189*MBL
17–11F390Adult39Y12*Acousonde
17–12F425Adult25Y15*Acousonde
17–13M298Juvenile36Y26*Acousonde
17–14M230Juvenile18N3*Acousonde
17–18F370Adult31Y11Acousonde
17–19F380Adult35Y15Acousonde
17–20F408Adult35Y22Acousonde
18–02F357Adult34Y112*Acousonde
18–03M303Adult35N19*Acousonde
18–04F382Adult23N107*Acousonde
Table 1
Summary of study animals, including animal sex, animal length (cm), age class (adults classified as body length, >300 cm; Hay 1984), minutes held follow capture and prior to release (tcap) and the presence of a satellite tag attached via ‘bolt-on’ configuration. The type of accelerometer (ACC-type) is also included. The number of hours of acceleration data recorded while attached to each narwhal is also included (hours); however, several tags were programmed with delays in recording for a period of up to 4 hours (indicated by the *)
AnimalSexLength (cm)Age classtcap (min)Bolt-on tag?HoursACC type
12–03F390Adult30Y5TDR-DD
12–05F262Juvenile30Y15TDR-DD
17–03F400Adult36Y182*MBL
17–04M432Adult47Y38*MBL
17–05M488Adult67Y48*MBL
17–08F375Adult37Y83Acousonde
17–09F385Adult36Y70*Acousonde
17–10M400Adult30N189*MBL
17–11F390Adult39Y12*Acousonde
17–12F425Adult25Y15*Acousonde
17–13M298Juvenile36Y26*Acousonde
17–14M230Juvenile18N3*Acousonde
17–18F370Adult31Y11Acousonde
17–19F380Adult35Y15Acousonde
17–20F408Adult35Y22Acousonde
18–02F357Adult34Y112*Acousonde
18–03M303Adult35N19*Acousonde
18–04F382Adult23N107*Acousonde
AnimalSexLength (cm)Age classtcap (min)Bolt-on tag?HoursACC type
12–03F390Adult30Y5TDR-DD
12–05F262Juvenile30Y15TDR-DD
17–03F400Adult36Y182*MBL
17–04M432Adult47Y38*MBL
17–05M488Adult67Y48*MBL
17–08F375Adult37Y83Acousonde
17–09F385Adult36Y70*Acousonde
17–10M400Adult30N189*MBL
17–11F390Adult39Y12*Acousonde
17–12F425Adult25Y15*Acousonde
17–13M298Juvenile36Y26*Acousonde
17–14M230Juvenile18N3*Acousonde
17–18F370Adult31Y11Acousonde
17–19F380Adult35Y15Acousonde
17–20F408Adult35Y22Acousonde
18–02F357Adult34Y112*Acousonde
18–03M303Adult35N19*Acousonde
18–04F382Adult23N107*Acousonde

Narwhal were captured following standard protocols and monitored by a veterinarian (Orr et al., 2001; Heide-Jørgensen et al., 2015). In brief, a 50 m x 10 m gill net was set perpendicular to shore and monitored by a minimum of two people for the entire period set. Immediately following a narwhal entering the net, two boats were deployed: the first to locate the animals and pull the net to the surface to allow the individual/s to breathe and the second to release the net from the anchor buoy. Following release, a shore based team pulled the net to the beach, securing individual narwhal in approximately 50 cm of water with animals facing out to deeper water. A padded rope loop was applied around the tail stalk and the animal was disentangled from the net. Captured narwhal were then held by a minimum of four experienced handlers and equipped with satellite transmitting tags (various models including TDR10, Wildlife Computers, Inc. and CTD Oceanography SRDL with GPS, SMRU Instrumentation used to derive dive behaviour, see below) via spider wires crimped to three sub-dermal 10 mm pins made of Tecaform™ sterilized in Ethylene Oxide gas. An 11-mm stainless steel tube sharpened at one end was used to cut a path for insertion of each pin into the dorsal ridge. Custom-made lock washers were then placed on the pin to the point of skin contact with no pressure. Washers were locked in place and pins trimmed flush to the outer-side of the washer. Stainless steel cables attached to the tag were inserted through the washers, adjusted to the desired length and crimped to secure the tag to the animal (Orr et al., 2001). All capture and tagging protocols were approved by the Fisheries and Oceans Animal Care Committee and a License for Scientific Purposes was granted.

Of the 29 narwhal captured over the three field seasons, 20 were outfitted with a telemetry device that included an accelerometer unit; either an Acousonde™ (n = 13; Model B003B, Greeneridge Sciences, Inc.), a daily diary (n = 3; TDR-DD, Wildlife Computers, Inc.) or a Maritime BioLoggers (MBL) accelerometer (n = 4). Accelerometers (Acousounde, TDR-DD and MBL) were fixed within recoverable biologging packages that consisted of a float for recovery (i.e. to bring the device to the surface) and a SPOT and VHF tag to locate the unit once at the surface. Accelerometer packages were attached to the narwhal posterior to the dorsal ridge via a suction cup and/or tethered line attached to one pin of the main satellite tag via release timers (Little Leonardo; see above, Fig. 1, Table 1). Accelerometers were programmed to remain attached on animals for 3 to 7 days (mean deployment duration 53.9 hours). The Acousonde units primarily sampled high-resolution acoustic data, but were also programmed to sample tri-axial magnetometry and acceleration as well as other auxiliary data channels including light level, pressure and temperature (Burgess, 2009). Sampling rate was set at 800 Hz, but for the purposes of this study was subsampled at 50 Hz (± 4 g). One individual Acousonde unit, however, failed to collect acceleration data and was not considered further in the analysis. MBL units were programmed to sample acceleration at 50 Hz with a 4 g range. The TDR10-Daily Diary tag was housed in a float pack with a SPOT5 tag from Wildlife Computers. Each float pack unit was suction cupped onto the side of the narwhal dorsal ridge and were not tethered to the satellite tag. The SPOT5 tag provided location information useful for relocating the tag after release from the animal, while the Daily Diary tags sampled high-resolution acceleration, orientation and speed (via paddle wheel), through the use of an accelerometer and magnetometer, and also include measurements of depth, temperature and light (Wilson et al., 2008). Daily Diary tags sampled acceleration at 16 Hz ± 2 g. For one individual equipped with a TDR-DD, the tag fall off within one hour of deployment and consequently it was not included in analysis.

Images showing net deployment anchored perpendicular to shore (A) in Tremblay Sound (72°21.389 N, −81°05.855 W), an example of attachment of the bolt-on configuration (3 pins) for a TDR-10 satellite tag, and accompanying (B) Maritime Biologgers (MBL) deployment package with timed release mechanism attached to pins or (C) Acousonde unit attached via both a suction cup mounting and little Leonardo releases held across the spider wires holding the TDR-10 satellite tag to the dorsal ridge and (D) a Daily Diary (TDR-DD) tag attached with a suction cup under a bolt-on satellite tag (2 pins).
Figure 1

Images showing net deployment anchored perpendicular to shore (A) in Tremblay Sound (72°21.389 N, −81°05.855 W), an example of attachment of the bolt-on configuration (3 pins) for a TDR-10 satellite tag, and accompanying (B) Maritime Biologgers (MBL) deployment package with timed release mechanism attached to pins or (C) Acousonde unit attached via both a suction cup mounting and little Leonardo releases held across the spider wires holding the TDR-10 satellite tag to the dorsal ridge and (D) a Daily Diary (TDR-DD) tag attached with a suction cup under a bolt-on satellite tag (2 pins).

Post-release behaviour assessed using accelerometer-derived metrics

We used three behavioural metrics of energy expenditure, swimming behaviour and activity levels from accelerometer data to examine post-release behaviour. First, we used a smoothed vector of Vectorial Dynamic Body Acceleration (sVeDBA) as a proxy for coarse energy expenditure (Miwa et al., 2015; Broell et al., 2016; Udyawer et al., 2017; Grémillet et al., 2018; Wilson et al., 2019). To derive static acceleration (relative animal body position and postural dynamics with respect to gravity), raw acceleration data for each deployment were first low-pass filtered in each axis at 0.1 Hz (Shepard et al., 2008). Dynamic acceleration was then derived by subtracting static acceleration from raw acceleration data in each axis (Shepard et al., 2008) and VeDBA estimated by taking the vectorial sum of dynamic acceleration across all three orthogonal axes and smoothing using a 3 s moving window (Fig. 2; Stothart et al. 2016). Second, to quantify activity level, the derivative of acceleration, jerk, was calculated by taking the differential of raw acceleration in each axis and then computing the norm of jerk (Fig. 2; Ydesen et al. 2014). Third, we estimated tail-beat frequency per second as a measure of swimming behaviour. Tail-beat frequency (TBFreq) was derived through fast Fourier transform of dynamic acceleration in the Z-axis, approximating dorso-ventral movement, within a 9-second moving window around each second to minimize spectral leakage. The primary frequency was extracted at each second (Fehlmann et al., 2017). Derived TBFreq was then smoothed using a running mean of 60 seconds to remove flow noise associated with swimming or wiggle from the tag attachment (Cade et al., 2017); any spikes in frequency above 1 Hz remaining are likely due to artefacts from a quick movement experienced by the tag or where suction cups may have been dislodged (Fig. 2).

Example of behaviour across the full recording period (~ 22 hours) for a 3.2 m male narwhal 17–20, including the period immediately after release. Behavioural metrics include (a) activity levels (norm of jerk), (b) energy expenditure (sVeDBA) and (c) swimming activity (TBFreq), before the tag fell off just after 12:00 on the second day as indicated by the green arrows.
Figure 2

Example of behaviour across the full recording period (~ 22 hours) for a 3.2 m male narwhal 17–20, including the period immediately after release. Behavioural metrics include (a) activity levels (norm of jerk), (b) energy expenditure (sVeDBA) and (c) swimming activity (TBFreq), before the tag fell off just after 12:00 on the second day as indicated by the green arrows.

We were interested in how these three behavioural metrics estimated for each narwhal deviated from long-term mean values following release from capture, handling and tagging activities. For modelling purposes, each accelerometer-derived behavioural metric was summarized as an hourly mean value. To calculate within-individual deviance for each metric, we calculated a pooled mean value using data beyond the first 36 hours to act as a baseline. Pooled mean values were separated by tag types to account for differences in attachment set up (Acousonde and MBL/TDR-DD; see Table 1). These pooled means were then subtracted from hourly mean values for each individual/tag type in order to calculate within-individual deviance for each behavioural metric. While any pooled mean value for ‘baseline behaviour’ would ideally be derived from data several days post-release for each individual separately (e.g. Dechen Quinn et al. 2012), we were limited in sampling duration for many of the accelerometers because most units detached from the narwhals earlier than expected (e.g. only 6 individuals had usable acceleration data > 48 hours post-release; Table 1). As a result of this limitation, we explored variation in the three behavioural metrics for the first 72 hours post-release. We are confident that this represents an acceptable time frame for assessment given that several studies have noted that post-escape or post-disturbance behaviour returns to normal within the first few hours up to one day across a variety of species (Gales et al., 2012; Hastie et al., 2015; Russell et al., 2016; Whitney et al., 2016; Williams et al., 2017a; Blackwell et al., 2018; van Beest et al., 2018), and our longer-term diving data indicated that narwhals recovered within 36 hours (see below).

Modelling recovery

Time to recovery

We determined the time point at which both the population of narwhal (across-individuals) and individual animals (within-individual) had recovered, or returned to baseline behaviour. Across-individuals, patterns in mean hourly deviance for each of the three behavioural metrics were plotted for the first 72 hours post-release. The time at which mean hourly deviance was no longer significantly different from zero was used as a measure of when the population of individuals had recovered, i.e. behavioural metrics returned to expected values of ‘normal’ behaviour (Dechen Quinn et al., 2012; Thiemann et al., 2013; Rode et al., 2014). Within-individuals, the mean time (hour) in which centred-values were no longer significantly different from zero across all three behavioural metrics (within 95% confidence interval for within-individual deviance) was used to define the point at which individuals recovered to baseline values or assumed normal behaviour (Dechen Quinn et al., 2012; Thiemann et al., 2013; Rode et al., 2014). Individual recovery points to baseline values were compared among individuals with additional covariates using generalized linear models with a Poisson distribution and log-link and were ranked based on evidence (Zuur et al., 2009). Additional individual covariates comprised (i) with or without ‘bolt-on’ configuration of satellite telemetry device, (ii) sex, (iii) age class (adult vs. juvenile delineated based on standard length of individuals >300 cm; Hay, 1984) and (iv) handling time (categorical; short: tcap < 30 min; medium: 30–40 min; long: > 40 min).

Behavioural response following capture, handling and tagging

To understand individual narwhal post-release behaviour relative to tagging procedures and intrinsic factors (sex and size), we examined the temporal changes in individual deviance for each of the accelerometer-derived metrics with a set of generalized additive models. Given we were interested in how behaviours are modified post-release, our base models described changes in all three metrics as a function of time since release (trel). Relationships were not linear, consequently single thin-plate smoothing splines were applied, with an additional shrinkage penalty in order to obtain the simplest spline and allow overly complex relationships to shrink to zero (Wood 2006). Models were designed a priori for relationships of interest as well as to deal with collinearity across model covariates (including satellite tag presence, sex, age class and handling time described above) and relatively small sample size. Each of the categorical covariates was modelled as either parametric covariates or as varying-coefficient smoothing interactions separately. All generalized additive modelling was performed within the R package ‘mgcv’ (Wood, 2006). All candidate models were ranked based on Akaike Information Criterion, corrected for small sample size (AICc; Burnham and Anderson, 2002). All analyses were conducted using R v.3.6.3 (R Core Team 2020).

Post-release dive behaviour to validate accelerometry recovery estimates

To complement the accelerometer-based time to recovery analysis, we investigated diving data collected on a subset of the narwhals (n = 10). These time series depth data were collected over a considerably longer period than the accelerometer data and thus provides a means to determine if the 72 hour time frame used for the three accelerometer-derived behavioural metrics was adequate. While changes in diving behaviour are a useful indicator to investigate post-disturbance events (DeRuiter et al., 2013; Miller et al., 2015; Williams et al., 2015; van Beest et al., 2018; Warren et al., 2020), the small sample size of useable time series dive data limited our capacity to investigate individual differences in dive behaviour through additive modelling. Diving behaviour were derived from satellite telemetry devices (TDR10 and CTD-SRDL; recorded approximately every 75 s) and quantified by taking hourly estimates of mean and maximum dive depths (meters). To determine the approximate normal dive behaviour of an individual, we calculated a long-term average of mean and max depths by averaging all data between 7 and 14 days following release. To determine deviance in dive behaviour across the first 72 hours, the long-term mean value was then subtracted from both hourly mean and maximum dive depths. Identical to our approach for analysing the three accelerometer metrics, we then used the time when the across-individual deviance for mean and maximum dive depth were not significantly different from 0 as the population-level time of recovery (Dechen Quinn et al., 2012).

Results

Acceleration data were recorded for an average of 53.9 hours across all narwhals sampled (n = 18; range, 3–189 hours; see Table 1); data, however, were not available for most individuals for the first four hours post-tag attachment/release due to programmed delays for auxiliary sampling (Table 1).

While mean hourly deviance for pooled data was not significantly different from zero for any of the three accelerometer-derived behavioural metrics over the first 72 hour period (Fig. 4A–C), narwhal overall were observed to be slightly more active and expend more energy for the first hour (Fig. 4A) and tail-beat frequency was slightly faster than normal for the first 24 hours (Fig. 4C). Wide confidence intervals further highlight high variability in accelerometry-derived behaviour following release. Individual recovery time points were generally reached within hours following release. At the individual level, mean estimated recovery time was 9 hours pooled across the three accelerometer-derived behaviours (range, 1–28 hours; excluding two individuals that had < 5 hours of accelerometer data). There was no significant difference in mean recovery time between male and female narwhal (βsex = 0.21 ± 0.18, z = 1.15, P = 0.24), nor with respect to presence of a bolt-on satellite tag (βtag = −0.10 ± 0.21, z = −0.51, P = 0.61). Binned handling time (tcap) as a predictor mean recovery time across individuals indicated that those held for medium durations (30–40 min) occasionally took longer to recover than those held for shorter durations (<30 min; βmedium = 0.32 ± 0.19, z = 1.66, P = 0.09), though these responses were highly variable and not significant. Individuals held for longer periods (>40 min) had a higher mean recover time value, however, these were not significantly different from those held for a short duration (βlong = 0.10 ± 0.30, z = 0.35, P = 0.72; see Fig. 5). However, all covariates were equal in explanatory power to that of a null model (< 3 ΔAICc; see Table S2).

Dive data indicated that narwhals spent significantly more time at shallower than average mean and maximum dive depths for the first 24 hours post-release (Fig. 4D, E). After this point, across-individual deviance in both dive metrics and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals were not significantly different from the long-term mean (0, based on data from 7–14 days post-release). This lends confidence that our accelerometer-based analysis was able to capture the post-release recovery period within the 72-hour time frame used.

Deviance in narwhal activity level (jerk) was best explained by a thin-plate smooth with shrinkage of trel that varied by sex and included the parametric effect of binned tcap, explaining 19.4% of model deviance (Table 2). Individuals who were held for longer periods (>40 min) tended to be more active in the first 72 hours following release (βLong = 0.26 ± 0.11, t = 2.30, P = 0.02) than those held for short (<30 min) and medium durations (30–40 min; Fig. 3A), though the overall effect size was small (see Table S1). Energy expenditure (deviance - VeDBAs) was best explained by spline function of trel that varied by sex as well as with the additional parametric effect of tcap (5.86 and 7.02% model deviance explained, respectively; Table 2), but were roughly equivalent in model evidence (ΔAICc < 4). Females displayed the greatest change in energy expenditure following release, while males were modelled to maintain constant energy expenditure as a function of trel after shrinkage penalization, indicating that males may expend less energy overall as compared to the population mean (Fig. 3B). The additional effects of tcap on energy expenditure, while significant, were extremely small (effect size ranging −0.01 to −0.004 for each tcap bin; Fig. 3B and Table S1). Swimming behaviour (deviance—TBFreq), however, was best explained by a spline function of trel that varied by tcap, explaining 18.1% of model deviance (Table 2). Individuals held for the longest period of time (>40 min) displayed a higher than normal swimming activity over time (Fig. 3C). There was also evidence to suggest that sex or the presences of a ‘bolt-on’ satellite tag may have influenced swimming behaviour in the first 72 hours after release (both explaining 19.1% deviance, Table 2). Females and those with ‘bolt-on’ satellite tags were found to have an overall lower TBFreq, though the overall effect size was extremely small (βsex = −0.04 ± 0.01, t = −3.19, P = 0.001; βtag = −0.03 ± 0.01, t = −2.55, P = 0.01; Table S1). The relatively low percentage of across-individual deviance explained by all models suggests a high degree of variability in hourly behaviour data from these accelerometers. Age class was ultimately removed from analyses as a covariate as all but one juvenile individual (of n = 3) had data spanning more than 15 hours post-release. Full model output, including significance of the smoothing terms, can be found in Table S1.

Table 2

Model results for time-varying patterns in hourly post-release behaviour of narwhal following capture and handling. Time since release (trel) was modelled as a thin-plate smoothing spline with shrinkage. Covariates, including animal sex and the presence of a bolt-on satellite tag (Sat.tag), and time held in captivity (binned tcap) were modelled as both parametric terms and as varying coefficient models (‘by =’). Models including age class were removed from model selection due to insufficient sample size. Best models are highlighted in bold, ranked based upon Akaike Information Criterion (AICc), corrected for small sample size and deviance explained

ActivityEnergy expenditureSwimming
Model parametersAICcΔ AICcDeviance explainedAICcΔ AICcDeviance explainedAICcΔ AICcDeviance explained
null s(trel)1632.1881.995.9%−1894.7915.213.9%−647.5495.91.7%
Sex + s(trel)1609.7859.599.6%−1894.5715.434.3%−689.5453.98.7%
Sat.Tag + s(trel)1629.279.016.7%−1889.5720.433.3%−645.8197.631.8%
tcap + s(trel)1604.354.1110.7%−1898.7111.295.2%−706.6636.7811.6%
s(trel, by = Sex)1571.9521.7615.9%−1906.443.565.8%−649.3794.072.3%
s(trel, by = Sat.Tag)1651.58101.391.1%−1891.3218.683.2%−652.8890.563.5%
tcap + s(trel, by = Sex)1550.19019.4%−1910.0007.0%−706.1437.311.7%
tcap + s(trel, by = Sat.Tag)1620.6170.428.4%−1892.2217.784.0%−714.9528.4913.5%
s(trel, by = tcap)1569.3519.1617.9%−1894.6515.354.2%−737.226.2218.1%
Sex + s(trel, by = tcap)1565.6315.4418.7%−1893.5816.424.4%−743.44019.1%
Sat.Tag + s(trel, by = tcap)1563.6613.4719.0%−1889.5720.4313.4%−742.291.1519.1%
ActivityEnergy expenditureSwimming
Model parametersAICcΔ AICcDeviance explainedAICcΔ AICcDeviance explainedAICcΔ AICcDeviance explained
null s(trel)1632.1881.995.9%−1894.7915.213.9%−647.5495.91.7%
Sex + s(trel)1609.7859.599.6%−1894.5715.434.3%−689.5453.98.7%
Sat.Tag + s(trel)1629.279.016.7%−1889.5720.433.3%−645.8197.631.8%
tcap + s(trel)1604.354.1110.7%−1898.7111.295.2%−706.6636.7811.6%
s(trel, by = Sex)1571.9521.7615.9%−1906.443.565.8%−649.3794.072.3%
s(trel, by = Sat.Tag)1651.58101.391.1%−1891.3218.683.2%−652.8890.563.5%
tcap + s(trel, by = Sex)1550.19019.4%−1910.0007.0%−706.1437.311.7%
tcap + s(trel, by = Sat.Tag)1620.6170.428.4%−1892.2217.784.0%−714.9528.4913.5%
s(trel, by = tcap)1569.3519.1617.9%−1894.6515.354.2%−737.226.2218.1%
Sex + s(trel, by = tcap)1565.6315.4418.7%−1893.5816.424.4%−743.44019.1%
Sat.Tag + s(trel, by = tcap)1563.6613.4719.0%−1889.5720.4313.4%−742.291.1519.1%
Table 2

Model results for time-varying patterns in hourly post-release behaviour of narwhal following capture and handling. Time since release (trel) was modelled as a thin-plate smoothing spline with shrinkage. Covariates, including animal sex and the presence of a bolt-on satellite tag (Sat.tag), and time held in captivity (binned tcap) were modelled as both parametric terms and as varying coefficient models (‘by =’). Models including age class were removed from model selection due to insufficient sample size. Best models are highlighted in bold, ranked based upon Akaike Information Criterion (AICc), corrected for small sample size and deviance explained

ActivityEnergy expenditureSwimming
Model parametersAICcΔ AICcDeviance explainedAICcΔ AICcDeviance explainedAICcΔ AICcDeviance explained
null s(trel)1632.1881.995.9%−1894.7915.213.9%−647.5495.91.7%
Sex + s(trel)1609.7859.599.6%−1894.5715.434.3%−689.5453.98.7%
Sat.Tag + s(trel)1629.279.016.7%−1889.5720.433.3%−645.8197.631.8%
tcap + s(trel)1604.354.1110.7%−1898.7111.295.2%−706.6636.7811.6%
s(trel, by = Sex)1571.9521.7615.9%−1906.443.565.8%−649.3794.072.3%
s(trel, by = Sat.Tag)1651.58101.391.1%−1891.3218.683.2%−652.8890.563.5%
tcap + s(trel, by = Sex)1550.19019.4%−1910.0007.0%−706.1437.311.7%
tcap + s(trel, by = Sat.Tag)1620.6170.428.4%−1892.2217.784.0%−714.9528.4913.5%
s(trel, by = tcap)1569.3519.1617.9%−1894.6515.354.2%−737.226.2218.1%
Sex + s(trel, by = tcap)1565.6315.4418.7%−1893.5816.424.4%−743.44019.1%
Sat.Tag + s(trel, by = tcap)1563.6613.4719.0%−1889.5720.4313.4%−742.291.1519.1%
ActivityEnergy expenditureSwimming
Model parametersAICcΔ AICcDeviance explainedAICcΔ AICcDeviance explainedAICcΔ AICcDeviance explained
null s(trel)1632.1881.995.9%−1894.7915.213.9%−647.5495.91.7%
Sex + s(trel)1609.7859.599.6%−1894.5715.434.3%−689.5453.98.7%
Sat.Tag + s(trel)1629.279.016.7%−1889.5720.433.3%−645.8197.631.8%
tcap + s(trel)1604.354.1110.7%−1898.7111.295.2%−706.6636.7811.6%
s(trel, by = Sex)1571.9521.7615.9%−1906.443.565.8%−649.3794.072.3%
s(trel, by = Sat.Tag)1651.58101.391.1%−1891.3218.683.2%−652.8890.563.5%
tcap + s(trel, by = Sex)1550.19019.4%−1910.0007.0%−706.1437.311.7%
tcap + s(trel, by = Sat.Tag)1620.6170.428.4%−1892.2217.784.0%−714.9528.4913.5%
s(trel, by = tcap)1569.3519.1617.9%−1894.6515.354.2%−737.226.2218.1%
Sex + s(trel, by = tcap)1565.6315.4418.7%−1893.5816.424.4%−743.44019.1%
Sat.Tag + s(trel, by = tcap)1563.6613.4719.0%−1889.5720.4313.4%−742.291.1519.1%
Plots of the interaction between time after release and sex as well as the additive effects relative to handling time, tcap, for deviance associated with (A) activity levels and (B) energy expenditure. Changes in swimming behaviour (C) were best explained by the interaction of time after release and handling time. Modelled behavioural responses relative to the population mean (deviance = 0) is highlighted by the horizontal dashed line. Data for individuals that were held for the longest time (tcap > 40 min) were only modelled to 50 hours as a result of limited accelerometry data. The spline calculated for male narwhal (B) was zeroed out as a result of the shrinkage penalty implemented to prevent overly complex relationships.
Figure 3

Plots of the interaction between time after release and sex as well as the additive effects relative to handling time, tcap, for deviance associated with (A) activity levels and (B) energy expenditure. Changes in swimming behaviour (C) were best explained by the interaction of time after release and handling time. Modelled behavioural responses relative to the population mean (deviance = 0) is highlighted by the horizontal dashed line. Data for individuals that were held for the longest time (tcap > 40 min) were only modelled to 50 hours as a result of limited accelerometry data. The spline calculated for male narwhal (B) was zeroed out as a result of the shrinkage penalty implemented to prevent overly complex relationships.

Discussion

Evaluating the behavioural responses of individuals following tagging and capture events remains an important goal for maintaining and evaluating best practices for telemetry studies in cetaceans. Generally, studies of tagging effects have focused on wound healing assessed by photographic resighting of tagged individuals (Robbins et al., 2013; McIntyre, 2014; Andrews et al., 2015; Best et al., 2015; Gendron et al., 2015; Heide-Jørgensen et al., 2017; Norman et al., 2018) or improving issues of drag and cost of transport associated with carrying telemetry devices (van der Hoop et al., 2018; Kyte et al., 2019). Here, we used high-resolution accelerometry data to examine the behaviour of individual narwhal following routine capture and telemetry device application by way of evidence-based model ranking with covariates for individual characteristics, handling time and the application of ‘bolt-on’ telemetry devices.

Mean population or across-individual hourly deviance with 95% confidence intervals (solid black and dashed lines, respectively) for accelerometer-derived behavioural metrics (A) activity levels, (B) energy expenditure, (C) swimming activity and (D, E) dive behaviour for the first 72 hours following release from capture/tagging events. Deviance was calculated by pooling hourly mean values for accelerometry data (36+ hours post-release, limited by measurement durations) and dive data (7–14 day hourly mean). Coloured lines represent loess-smoothed trend lines to further highlight trends in behaviour.
Figure 4

Mean population or across-individual hourly deviance with 95% confidence intervals (solid black and dashed lines, respectively) for accelerometer-derived behavioural metrics (A) activity levels, (B) energy expenditure, (C) swimming activity and (D, E) dive behaviour for the first 72 hours following release from capture/tagging events. Deviance was calculated by pooling hourly mean values for accelerometry data (36+ hours post-release, limited by measurement durations) and dive data (7–14 day hourly mean). Coloured lines represent loess-smoothed trend lines to further highlight trends in behaviour.

Summary of mean recovery time points (hours) after release as a function of handling time (tcap: short, <30 min; medium, 30–40 min; long, >40 min). Thick black bars in the middle of the boxes indicate the median value (bounded by the interquartile range) for time to recovery for each of the handling durations.
Figure 5

Summary of mean recovery time points (hours) after release as a function of handling time (tcap: short, <30 min; medium, 30–40 min; long, >40 min). Thick black bars in the middle of the boxes indicate the median value (bounded by the interquartile range) for time to recovery for each of the handling durations.

Generally, we found that individual narwhal recovered quickly from capture and tagging activities. While there was significant inter-individual variability in the magnitude of the response, most individuals appeared to recover in the first 8 hours following release with respect to accelerometry-derived behavioural metrics of activity levels, energy expenditure, and swimming behaviour. We also found no evidence to suggest that handling time had a major impact on recovery time. Other studies linking changes in behaviour have found individuals recover quickly following pile-driving activities (Russell et al., 2016), mass-stranding events (Gales et al., 2012) and with respect to tagging events (Whitney et al., 2016; Warren et al., 2020). Longer-term measures of diving behaviour in a subset of these narwhal further supported that most individuals returned to normal diving patterns within the first 24 hours after release. A study on harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) noted that individuals exhibited shallower diving following both an initial handling event as well as a measured noise dose-response experiment, but also showed that the same individuals appear to have recovered to baseline behaviour within 24 hours or less (van Beest et al., 2018). Similar shallow diving has been noted in narwhal in response to predation events and the presence of predators (Laidre et al., 2006; Breed et al., 2017), though these events resulted in longer disruptions of behaviour than measured here. Acoustically-tagged narwhal exhibited a period of silence (no vocalizations) after tagging in East Greenland, ranging from 9 to 37 hours after release (Blackwell et al., 2018). The authors acknowledged, however, that this may be linked to narwhal being outside the feeding grounds as there was no relation to handling time with respect to the time to recovery (Blackwell et al., 2018). Sperm whale (Physeter macrocephalus) tagged with suction cup telemetry devices via pole deployments also displayed lower clicking and buzzing rates, coupled with shallower dives, in the hours following tag deployment (Warren et al., 2020).

Handling time was, however, found to be a significant covariate for the observed deviance in post-release behaviour for narwhals in the current study. Most individuals were held for an average of 35 minutes, which is at the lower end of handling times reported in previous work (Williams et al., 2017a). Our models suggest that individuals held for longer periods of time tended to have higher than normal swimming activity immediately following release, as well as higher activity levels, but with little change in energy expenditure. This suggests that the few individuals held for greater than 40 minutes may have exhibited a greater escape response to handling events than those held for less time (Fig. 3). Previous work has noted that narwhals can exhibit extreme responses to handling events, sometimes expressing extreme bradycardia events and escape responses in the first 60 minutes following release (Williams et al., 2017a). While Williams et al. (2017a) did not explicitly define a recovery period following tagging events, they considered a ‘post-escape period’ as occurring 45 to 90 minutes after release and found that individuals who were held for more than 60 minutes tended to display heart rate and stroke rate relationships more consistent with an escape response and cardiac freeze; those that were held for less than 60 minutes appeared to more quickly return to normal flight behaviours and cardiac output (Williams et al., 2017a). Our accelerometer-derived behaviours appear to support this. We found that individuals held for the longest period displayed the largest change in activity and swimming behaviour following release, indicating that holding individuals for greater than 40 minutes resulted in a significant difference in post-release behaviours. Our diving behaviour proxies also appear to support the work of Williams et al. (2017a); narwhals here, all held for less than 60 minutes, generally appeared to dive to and spend the majority of their time at shallower than normal depths for the first 24 hours following release (Fig. 4), suggesting that individuals may have exhibited a temporary flight response. Similar dispersal to shallow water was noted in narwhal in the days following an attack by killer whales (Orcinus orca; Laidre et al. 2006). Even when individuals are not handled, cumulative exposure to research activities may still drive a similar dive response (Warren et al., 2020).

In addition to handling time, this study also presents some evidence to suggest that individuals with ‘bolt-on’ satellite tags had a lower tail-beat frequency following release than those without. Several studies have been attempted to determine the effects of equipping animals with invasive telemetry devices in a wide variety of species (Walker et al., 2012; Andrews et al., 2015, 2019; Shuert et al., 2015; Chivers et al., 2016; Horning et al., 2017a; Lear et al., 2018). Even when not found to impact survival, physiological costs associated with telemetry devices likely exist with respect to increased drag associated with the disruption of laminar flow across the body (Andrews et al., 2015; Andrews and Enstipp, 2016; Kyte et al., 2019). Effect sizes associated with the ‘bolt-on’ tags here were very small with respect to their impact on swimming behaviour, and we failed to find a significant relationship between ‘bolt-on’ tags and recovery time. While our overall sample size of individuals is large compared to other studies, it is unlikely that our models had sufficient power to detect any major behavioural differences as a result of narwhal carrying a ‘bolt-on’ satellite tag over longer time scales.

Alongside the attachment of a ‘bolt-on’ satellite tag, there was also evidence to suggest that individual sex had an influence on all accelerometry-derived behaviours in the first 72 hours after release. Models including individual sex and the presence of a ‘bolt-on’ satellite tag were apparently equal in their explanatory power for swimming behaviour (ΔAICc < 2; Table 2). Females were found to have both lower activity levels and tail-beat frequencies, but this did not appear to have a significant effect on recovery time. While age class was ultimately removed, individuals with satellite tags were larger, older individuals who may simply exhibit slower tailbeat frequencies. Larger whales can be assumed to have larger peduncle muscles in which to create a greater output of momentum for each stroke (Goldbogen et al., 2006). This has been demonstrated in experimental studies of fish species where increased body length scales as a power equation for both propulsive force, as well as a decrease in the frequency of strokes (Bainbridge, 1958; Ware, 1978; Webb et al., 1984; Ohlberger et al., 2007). Increased body size has been shown to result in a logarithmic decrease in the cost of transport in aquatic animals, defined as the energy required to move a unit of body mass a unit distance (Schmidt-Nielsen, 1972; Fish, 1994). Mammalian studies have used captive surrogates to scale up stroke-by-stroke energy costs to other species and have shown similar results (Williams et al., 2004, 2017b). While it was not possible to age these individuals beyond coarse size classes, it is likely that this change in both activity levels and swimming behaviour is a result of differences in body size and development (Noren et al., 2006), but requires further investigation.

Long-term attachments of telemetry devices, through invasive procedures such as ‘bolt-on’ configurations remain an important tool for understanding aspects of marine animal migrations, habitat use, site fidelity, and year-round behaviour and can greatly contribute to the conservation efforts of many species (Hussey et al., 2015; Hazen et al., 2017; Andrews et al., 2019). Long-term deployments often require active capture and handling in order to attach the devices. Consideration should therefore be given to determine if such invasive techniques are necessary in order to achieve the goals of the proposed work (Andrews et al., 2019); planning requires thorough and transparent discussion of questions and tagging approaches as well as potential outcomes of the tagging mode used with all involved stakeholders. Under certain scenarios, short-term deployments via suction cups or remote tagging can address study questions effectively (e.g. Goldbogen et al., 2013), whereby the impact of tagging on behaviour is minimal and therefore data loss related to non-normal behaviour is minimal. Addressing questions such as migration timing and extent, however, will require more invasive ‘bolt-on’ tagging procedures unless alternative long-term attachment methods are developed (Heide-Jørgensen et al., 2017). Opportunities to advance long-term attachment methods will require broader discussion among the scientific community and tag manufacturers that will likely lead to smaller design and more battery efficient tags (as discussed in Andrews et al., 2019; Horning et al., 2019). This will also require that tag manufacturers are willing to invest in research and development of new tags and attachment methods. For the Arctic whales, including traditional Inuit Qaujimajatuqangit could lead to the development and design of novel dart heads for long-term deployments via remote tag attachment given their wealth of experience with harpoon use (Noongwook et al., 2007; Idrobo and Berkes, 2012; Johnson et al., 2015; Pearce et al., 2015; Pedersen et al. 2020).

Future climate scenarios (Williams et al., 2011) and an increase in the incidence of disease in the Arctic (VanWormer et al., 2019) further highlights the importance of considering the implications of capture and tagging practices, the sample sizes required to answer the question at hand (Sequeira et al., 2019), and whether alternative tagging methods without the need for capture may be more appropriate (Best et al., 2015; Seyboth et al., 2017). If, for example, ‘bolt-on’ tag configurations are required to investigate stock structure in narwhal in the Arctic, researchers should consider equipping a small sample size of animals with accelerometer packages in conjunction with satellite tags and undertaking initial field investigations of data following the above approaches to determine impact (Wilson and McMahon, 2006). Evidence-based effects of capture, handling and tagging generated while in the field would then provide confidence in the approach, while considering the welfare of the animals related to uncertainties such as impacts of climate change and remains a key recommendation of best practice guidelines (Wilson and McMahon, 2006; Horning et al., 2017b, 2019; Andrews et al., 2019). Supporting the work of previous studies, we recommend that every effort should be taken to minimize handling time if active capture and ‘bolt-on’ tag placement is required. We also advocate further investigation on how to mitigate the potential effects of ‘bolt-on’ tags through modified designs, approaches and rigorous experimental design in the field in order to strive for continued refinement in biologging applications (Hawkins, 2004; Casper 2009), especially for sensitive species such as narwhal (Laidre et al., 2008).

Funding

This work was supported by the NSERC Northern supplement and NSERC Discovery funds to N.E.H. C.R.S was supported by Mitacs through the Mitacs Accelerate program (www.mitacs.ca) in partnership with the World Wildlife Fund. M.A.M. thanks the Canadian Research Chairs program. Funding for fieldwork was provided by Fisheries and Oceans Canada, the Nunavut Wildlife Research Trust, and the World Wildlife Fund.

Acknowledgements

Narwhal tagging efforts were led by the Department of Fisheries and Oceans long-term marine mammal monitoring program in conjunction with the University of Windsor and with support from the World Wildlife Fund and the Nunavut Wildlife Management Board. All field team members of the Ecosystem Approach to Tremblay Sound are thanked for their hard work and commitment in the field, especially Bob Hodgson who organized and led the expeditions. We would especially like to thank the community of Mittimatalik (Pond Inlet) and the Mittimatalik Hunter and Trapper Organization for extensive support in the field and tagging efforts. We also owe huge gratitude to Jack Orr for his wealth of experience in dealing with narwhal and for leading the expedition equipping narwhal with satellite transmitters during the first research period. Thanks also go to Polar Continental Shelf Program and Golder Associates for logistic support in the field. The authors also wish to thank an anonymous reviewer for constructive comments to improve an earlier version of this manuscript.

Conflicts of interest

The authors claim no conflict of interest.

References

Andrews
 
RD
 et al. (
2019
)
Best practice guidelines for cetacean tagging
.
J Cetacean Res Manag
 
20
:
27
66
.

Andrews
 
RD
,
Baird
 
RW
,
Schorr
 
GS
,
Mittal
 
R
,
Howle
 
LE
,
Hanson
 
MB
(
2015
)
Improving attachments of remotely-deployed dorsal fin-mounted tags: tissue structure, hydrodynamics, in situ performance, and tagged-animal follow-up. Final report to the Office of Naval Research, grant number N000141010686
,
37 pp
.

Andrews
 
RD
,
Enstipp
 
MR
(
2016
)
Diving physiology of seabirds and marine mammals: relevance, challenges and some solutions for field studies
.
Comp Biochem Physiol Part A Mol Integr Physiol
 
202
:
38
52
.

Bainbridge
 
BYR
(
1958
)
The speed of swimming of fish as related to size and to the frequency and amplitude of the tail beat
.
J Exp Biol
 
35
:
109
133
.

Balmer
 
BC
 et al. (
2011
)
Evaluation of a single-pin, satellite-linked transmitter deployed on bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) along the coast of Georgia, USA
.
Aquat Mamm
 
37
:
187
192
.

Barkley
 
AN
,
Broell
 
F
,
Pettitt-Wade
 
H
,
Watanabe
 
YY
,
Marcoux
 
M
,
Hussey
 
NE
(
2020
)
A framework to estimate the likelihood of species interactions and behavioural responses using animal-borne acoustic telemetry transceivers and accelerometers
.
J Anim Ecol
 
89
:
146
160
.

Best
 
PB
,
Mate
 
B
,
Lagerquist
 
B
(
2015
)
Tag retention, wound healing, and subsequent reproductive history of southern right whales following satellite-tagging
.
Mar Mammal Sci
 
31
:
520
539
.

Blackwell
 
SB
,
Tervo
 
OM
,
Conrad
 
AS
,
Sinding
 
MHS
,
Hansen
 
RG
,
Ditlevsen
 
S
,
Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
(
2018
)
Spatial and temporal patterns of sound production in East Greenland narwhals
.
PLoS One
 
13
:
1
23
.

Breed
 
GA
,
Matthews
 
CJD
,
Marcoux
 
M
,
Higdon
 
JW
,
Le Blanc
 
B
,
Petersen
 
SD
,
Orr
 
J
,
Reinhart
 
NR
,
Ferguson
 
SH
(
2017
)
Sustained disruption of narwhal habitat use and behavior in the presence of Arctic killer whales
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
 
114
:
2628
2633
.

Broell
 
F
,
Burnell
 
C
,
Taggart
 
CT
(
2016
)
Measuring abnormal movements in free-swimming fish with accelerometers: implications for quantifying tag and parasite load
.
J Exp Biol
 
219
:
695
705
.

Brooks
 
JL
 et al. (
2017
)
Use of Fish telemetry in rehabilitation planning, management, and monitoring in areas of concern in the Laurentian Great Lakes
.
Environ Manag
 
60
:
1139
1154
.

Burgess
 
WC
(
2009
)
The Acousonde: a miniature autonomous wideband recorder
.
J Acoust Soc Am
 
125
:
2588
2588
.

Burnham
 
KP
,
Anderson
 
DR
(
2002
)
Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach
, Ed 2.
Springer
,
New York
.

Cade
 
DE
,
Barr
 
KR
,
Calambokidis
 
J
,
Friedlaender
 
AS
,
Goldbogen
 
JA
(
2017
)
Determining forward speed from accelerometer jiggle in aquatic environments
.
J Exp Biol
 
jeb.170449
.

Casper
 
RM
(
2009
)
Guidelines for the instrumentation of wild birds and mammals
.
Anim Behav
 
78
:
1477
1483
.

Chivers
 
LS
,
Hatch
 
SA
,
Elliott
 
KH
(
2016
)
Accelerometry reveals an impact of short-term tagging on seabird activity budgets
.
Condor
 
118
:
159
168
.

Dechen Quinn
 
AC
,
Williams
 
DM
,
Porter
 
WF
(
2012
)
Postcapture movement rates can inform data-censoring protocols for GPS-collared animals
.
J Mammal
 
93
:
456
463
.

DeRuiter
 
SL
 et al. (
2013
)
First direct measurements of behavioural responses by Cuvier’s beaked whales to mid-frequency active sonar
.
Biol Lett
 
9
:
20130223
20130223
.

Dietz
 
R
,
Shapiro
 
AD
,
Bakhtiari
 
M
,
Orr
 
J
,
Tyack
 
PL
,
Richard
 
P
,
Eskesen
 
IG
,
Marshall
 
G
(
2007
)
Upside-down swimming behaviour of free-ranging narwhals
.
BMC Ecol
 
7
:
1
10
.

Evans
 
K
,
Lea
 
MA
,
Patterson
 
TA
(
2013
)
Recent advances in bio-logging science: technologies and methods for understanding animal behaviour and physiology and their environments
.
Deep Res Part II Top Stud Oceanogr
 
88–89
:
1
6
.

Fahlman
 
A
,
Wilson
 
R
,
Svärd
 
C
,
Rosen
 
DAS
,
Trites
 
AW
(
2008
)
Activity and diving metabolism correlate in Steller sea lion Eumetopias jubatus
.
Aquat Biol
 
2
:
75
84
.

Fehlmann
 
G
,
O’Riain
 
MJ
,
Hopkins
 
PW
,
O’Sullivan
 
J
,
Holton
 
MD
,
Shepard
 
ELC
,
King
 
AJ
(
2017
)
Identification of behaviours from accelerometer data in a wild social primate
.
Anim Biotelemetry
 
5
:
6
.

Fish
 
FE
(
1994
)
Influence of hydrodynamic design and propulsive mode on mammalian swimming energetics
.
Aust J Zool
 
42
:
1
16
.

Gales
 
NJ
,
Bowen
 
WD
,
Johnston
 
DW
,
Kovacs
 
KM
,
Littnan
 
CL
,
Perrin
 
WF
,
Reynolds
 
JE
,
Thompson
 
PM
(
2009
)
Guidelines for the treatment of marine mammals in field research
.
Mar Mammal Sci
 
25
:
725
736
.

Gales
 
R
,
Alderman
 
R
,
Thalmann
 
S
,
Carlyon
 
K
(
2012
)
Satellite tracking of long-finned pilot whales (Globicephala melas) following stranding and release in Tasmania, Australia
.
Wildl Res
 
39
:
520
531
.

Gendron
 
D
,
Serrano
 
IM
,
de la Cruz
 
AU
,
Calambokidis
 
J
,
Mate
 
B
(
2015
)
Long-term individual sighting history database: an effective tool to monitor satellite tag effects on cetaceans
.
Endanger Species Res
 
26
:
235
241
.

Goldbogen
 
JA
,
Calambokidis
 
J
,
Friedlaender
 
AS
,
Francis
 
J
,
DeRuiter
 
SL
,
Stimpert
 
AK
,
Falcone
 
E
,
Southall
 
BL
(
2013
)
Underwater acrobatics by the world’s largest predator: 360° rolling manoeuvres by lunge-feeding blue whales
.
Biol Lett
 
9
:
20120986
.

Goldbogen
 
JA
,
Calambokidis
 
J
,
Shadwick
 
RE
,
Oleson
 
EM
,
McDonald
 
MA
,
Hildebrand
 
JA
(
2006
)
Kinematics of foraging dives and lunge-feeding in fin whales
.
J Exp Biol
 
209
:
1231
1244
.

Grémillet
 
D
,
Lescroël
 
A
,
Ballard
 
G
,
Dugger
 
KM
,
Massaro
 
M
,
Porzig
 
EL
,
Ainley
 
DG
(
2018
)
Energetic fitness: field metabolic rates assessed via 3D accelerometry complement conventional fitness metrics
.
Funct Ecol
 
32
:
1203
1213
.

Hastie
 
GD
,
Russell
 
DJF
,
Mcconnell
 
B
,
Moss
 
S
,
Thompson
 
D
,
Janik
 
VM
(
2015
)
Sound exposure in harbour seals during the installation of an offshore wind farm: predictions of auditory damage
.
J Appl Ecol
 
52
:
631
640
.

Hauser
 
DDW
,
Laidre
 
KL
,
Suydam
 
RS
,
Richard
 
PR
(
2014
)
Population-specific home ranges and migration timing of Pacific Arctic beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas)
.
Polar Biol
 
37
:
1171
1183
.

Hawkins
 
P
(
2004
)
Bio-logging and animal welfare: practical refinements
.
Mem Natl Inst Polar Res Spec Issue
 
58
:
58
68
.

Hay
 
KA
(
1984
)
The life history of the narwhal (Monodon Monoceros L.) in the Eastern Canadian Arctic. McGill University
.

Hays
 
GC
 et al. (
2019
)
Translating marine animal tracking data into conservation policy and management
.
Trends Ecol Evol
 
34
:
15
.

Hazen
 
EL
 et al. (
2017
)
WhaleWatch: a dynamic management tool for predicting blue whale density in the California current
.
J Appl Ecol
 
54
:
1415
1428
.

Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
(
2018
) Narwhal: Monodon monoceros.
In
:
Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals
. Academic Press, pp.
627
631
.

Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
,
Dietz
 
R
,
Laidre
 
KL
,
Richard
 
P
(
2002
)
Autumn movements, home ranges, and winter density of narwhals (Monodon monoceros) tagged in Tremblay Sound, Baffin Island
.
Polar Biol
 
25
:
331
341
.

Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
,
Laidre
 
KL
,
Jensen
 
MV
,
Dueck
 
L
,
Postma
 
LD
(
2006
)
Dissolving stock discreteness with satellite tracking: bowhead whales in Baffin Bay
.
Mar Mammal Sci
 
22
:
34
45
.

Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
,
Nielsen
 
NH
,
Hansen
 
RG
,
Schmidt
 
HC
,
Blackwell
 
SB
,
Jørgensen
 
OA
(
2015
)
The predictable narwhal: satellite tracking shows behavioural similarities between isolated subpopulations
.
J Zool
 
297
:
54
65
.

Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
,
Nielsen
 
NH
,
Teilmann
 
J
,
Leifsson
 
PS
(
2017
)
Long-term tag retention on two species of small cetaceans
.
Mar Mammal Sci
 
33
:
713
725
.

Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
,
Richard
 
PR
,
Dietz
 
R
,
Laidre
 
KL
(
2013
)
A metapopulation model for Canadian and West Greenland narwhals
.
Anim Conserv
 
16
:
331
343
.

Horning
 
M
 et al. (
2019
)
Best practice recommendations for the use of external telemetry devices on pinnipeds
.
Anim Biotelemetry
 
7
:
1
17
.

Horning
 
M
,
Haulena
 
M
,
Rosenberg
 
JF
,
Nordstrom
 
C
(
2017a
)
Intraperitoneal implantation of life-long telemetry transmitters in three rehabilitated harbor seal pups
.
BMC Vet Res
 
13
:
139
.

Horning
 
M
 et al. (
2017b
)
Best practice recommendations for the use of fully implanted telemetry devices in pinnipeds
.
Anim Biotelemetry
 
5
:
1
15
.

Hussey
 
NE
,
Kessel
 
ST
,
Aarestrup
 
K
,
Cooke
 
SJ
,
Cowley
 
PD
,
Fisk
 
AT
,
Harcourt
 
RG
,
Holland
 
KN
,
Iverson
 
SJ
,
Kocik
 
JF
, et al. (
2015
)
Aquatic animal telemetry: A panoramic window into the underwater world. Science (80- ) 348: 1255642
.

Idrobo
 
CJ
,
Berkes
 
F
(
2012
)
Pangnirtung Inuit and the Greenland shark: co-producing knowledge of a little discussed species
.
Hum Ecol
 
40
:
405
414
.

Johnson
 
N
 et al. (
2015
)
The contributions of community-based monitoring and traditional knowledge to Arctic observing networks: reflections on the state of the field
.
Arctic
 
68
:
1
13
.

Kyte
 
A
,
Pass
 
C
,
Pemberton
 
R
,
Sharman
 
M
,
McKnight
 
JC
(
2019
)
A computational fluid dynamics (CFD) based method for assessing the hydrodynamic impact of animal borne data loggers on host marine mammals
.
Mar Mammal Sci
 
35
:
364
394
.

Ladds
 
MA
,
Rosen
 
DAS
,
Slip
 
DJ
,
Harcourt
 
RG
(
2017
)
Proxies of energy expenditure for marine mammals: an experimental test of “the time trap.”
.
Sci Rep
 
7
:
1
10
.

Laidre
 
KL
,
Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
(
2005
)
Winter feeding intensity of narwhals (Monodon monoceros)
.
Mar Mammal Sci
 
21
:
45
57
.

Laidre
 
KL
,
Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
,
Orr
 
JR
(
2006
)
Reactions of narwhals, Monodon monoceros, to killer whale, Orcinus orca, attacks in the eastern Canadian Arctic
.
Can Field-Naturalist
 
120
:
457
465
.

Laidre
 
KL
,
Stirling
 
I
,
Lowry
 
LF
,
Wiig
 
Ø
,
Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
,
Ferguson
 
SH
(
2008
)
Quanifying the sensitivity of Arctic marine mammals to climate-induced habitat change
.
Ecol Appl
 
18
:
S97
S125
.

Lear
 
KO
,
Gleiss
 
AC
,
Whitney
 
NM
(
2018
)
Metabolic rates and the energetic cost of external tag attachment in juvenile blacktip sharks Carcharhinus limbatus
.
J Fish Biol
 
93
:
391
395
.

Lennox
 
RJ
 et al. (
2017
)
Envisioning the future of aquatic animal tracking: technology, science, and application
.
Bioscience
 
67
:
884
896
.

Martin Lopez
 
LM
,
Miller
 
PJO
,
Aguilar de Soto
 
N
,
Johnson
 
MP
(
2015
)
Gait switches in deep-diving beaked whales: biomechanical strategies for long-duration dives
.
J Exp Biol
 
218
:
1325
1338
.

McIntyre
 
T
(
2014
)
Trends in tagging of marine mammals: a review of marine mammal biologging studies
.
African J Mar Sci
 
36
:
409
422
.

Miller
 
PJO
 et al. (
2015
)
First indications that northern bottlenose whales are sensitive to behavioural disturbance from anthropogenic noise
.
R Soc Open Sci
 
2
. doi: .

Miwa
 
M
,
Oishi
 
K
,
Nakagawa
 
Y
,
Maeno
 
H
,
Anzai
 
H
,
Kumagai
 
H
,
Okano
 
K
,
Tobioka
 
H
,
Hirooka
 
H
(
2015
)
Application of overall dynamic body acceleration as a proxy for estimating the energy expenditure of grazing farm animals: relationship with heart rate
.
PLoS One
 
10
:
1
19
.

Noongwook
 
G
,
Huntington
 
HP
,
George
 
JC
(
2007
)
Traditional knowledge of the bowhead whale (Balaena mysticetus) around St. Lawrence Island, Alaska
.
Arctic
 
60
:
47
54
.

Noren
 
SR
,
Biedenbach
 
G
,
Edwards
 
EF
(
2006
)
Ontogeny of swim performance and mechanics in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
.
J Exp Biol
 
209
:
4724
4731
.

Norman
 
SA
 et al. (
2018
)
Assessment of wound healing of tagged gray (Eschrichtius robustus) and blue (Balaenoptera musculus) whales in the eastern North Pacific using long-term series of photographs
.
Mar Mammal Sci
 
34
:
27
53
.

Ohlberger
 
J
,
Staaks
 
G
,
Hölker
 
F
(
2007
)
Effects of temperature, swimming speed and body mass on standard and active metabolic rate in vendace (Coregonus albula)
.
J Comp Physiol B Biochem Syst Environ Physiol
 
177
:
905
916
.

Orr
 
JR
,
Joe
 
R
,
Evic
 
D
(
2001
)
Capturing and handling of White whales (Delphinapterus lencas) in the Canadian Arctic for instrumentation and release
.
Arctic
 
54
:
299
304
.

Owen
 
K
,
Andrews
 
RD
,
Baird
 
RW
,
Schorr
 
GS
,
Webster
 
DL
(
2019
)
Lunar cycles influence the diving behavior and habitat use of short-finned pilot whales around the main Hawaiian islands
.
Mar Ecol Prog Ser
 
629
:
193
206
.

Pearce
 
T
,
Ford
 
J
,
Willox
 
AC
,
Smit
 
B
(
2015
)
Inuit traditional ecological knowledge (TEK), subsistence hunting and adaptation to climate change in the Canadian arctic
.
Arctic
 
68
:
233
245
.

Pedersen
 
C
 et al. (
2020
)
ScIQ: an invitation and recommendations to combine science and Inuit Qaujimajatuqangit for meaningful engagement of inuit communities in research
.
Arct Sci
 
6
:
326
339
.

Petersen
 
SD
,
Tenkula
 
D
,
Ferguson
 
SH
,
Kelley
 
T
,
Yurkowski
 
DJ
(
2012
)
Sex determination of belugas and narwhals: understanding implications of harvest sex ratio
.
Can Sci Advis Sec Res Doc
 
19
.

Reeves
 
RR
,
Tracey
 
S
(
1980
)
Monodon monoceros
.
Mamm Species
 
1
7
.

Robbins
 
J
,
Zerbini
 
AN
,
Gales
 
N
,
Gulland
 
F
,
Double
 
MC
,
Andrews-Goff
 
V
,
Kennedy
 
AS
,
Landry
 
S
,
Mattila
 
DK
,
Tackaberry
 
J
(
2013
)
Satellite Tag Effectiveness and Impacts on Large Whales: Preliminary Results of a Case Study with Gulf of Maine Humpback Whales
.
Report to the Scientific Committee of the International Whaling Commission
.

Rode
 
KD
,
Pagano
 
AM
,
Bromaghin
 
JF
,
Atwood
 
TC
,
Durner
 
GM
,
Simac
 
KS
,
Amstrup
 
SC
(
2014
)
Effects of capturing and collaring on polar bears: findings from long-term research on the southern Beaufort Sea population
.
Wildl Res
 
41
:
311
322
.

Russell
 
DJF
,
Hastie
 
GD
,
Thompson
 
D
,
Janik
 
VM
,
Hammond
 
PS
,
Scott-Hayward
 
LAS
,
Matthiopoulos
 
J
,
Jones
 
EL
,
McConnell
 
BJ
(
2016
)
Avoidance of wind farms by harbour seals is limited to pile driving activities
.
J Appl Ecol.
. doi: .

Sato
 
K
 et al. (
2007
)
Stroke frequency, but not swimming speed, is related to body size in free-ranging seabirds, pinnipeds and cetaceans
.
Proc R Soc B Biol Sci
 
274
:
471
477
.

Schmidt-Nielsen
 
K
(
1972
)
Energy cost of swimming, flying, and running. Science (80- ) 177: 222–228
.

Sequeira
 
AMM
 et al. (
2019
)
The importance of sample size in marine megafauna tagging studies
.
Ecol Appl
 
29
:
1
17
.

Seyboth
 
E
,
Botta
 
S
,
Mendes
 
CRB
,
Negrete
 
J
,
Rosa
 
LD
,
Secchi
 
ER
(
2017
)
Isotopic evidence of the effect of warming on the northern Antarctic peninsula ecosystem
.
Deep Sea Res Part II Top Stud Oceanogr
 
1
11
.

Shepard
 
ELC
,
Wilson
 
RP
,
Quintana
 
F
,
Laich
 
AG
,
Liebsch
 
N
,
Albareda D a., Halsey LG, Gleiss A, Morgan DT, Myers AE, et al.
(
2008
)
Identification of animal movement patterns using tri-axial accelerometry
.
Endanger Species Res
 
10
:
47
60
.

Shuert
 
CR
,
Horning
 
M
,
Mellish
 
J-AE
(
2015
)
The effect of novel research activities on long-term survival of temporarily captive Steller Sea lions (Eumetopias jubatus)
.
PLoS One
 
10
: e0141948.

Stothart
 
MR
,
Elliott
 
KH
,
Wood
 
T
,
Hatch
 
SA
,
Speakman
 
JR
(
2016
)
Counting calories in cormorants: dynamic body acceleration predicts daily energy expenditure measured in pelagic cormorants
.
J Exp Biol
 
219
:
2192
2200
.

R Core Team
(
2020
) R: A language and environment for statistical computing.
R Foundation for Statistical Computing
,
Vienna, Austria
. URL: https://www.R-project.org/.

Thiemann
 
GW
,
Derocher
 
AE
,
Cherry
 
SG
,
Lunn
 
NJ
,
Peacock
 
E
,
Sahanatien
 
V
(
2013
)
Effects of chemical immobilization on the movement rates of free-ranging polar bears
.
J Mammal
 
94
:
386
397
.

Udyawer
 
V
,
Simpfendorfer
 
CA
,
Heupel
 
MR
,
Clark
 
TD
(
2017
)
Temporal and spatial activity-associated energy partitioning in free-swimming sea snakes
.
Funct Ecol
 
31
:
1739
1749
.

van
 
Beest
 
FM
,
Teilmann
 
J
,
Hermannsen
 
L
,
Galatius
 
A
,
Mikkelsen
 
L
,
Sveegaard
 
S
,
Balle
 
JD
,
Dietz
 
R
,
Nabe-Nielsen
 
J
(
2018
)
Fine-scale movement responses of free-ranging harbour porpoises to capture, tagging and short-term noise pulses from a single airgun
.
R Soc Open Sci
 
5
:
170110
.

van der
 
Hoop
 
JM
,
Fahlman
 
A
,
Shorter
 
KA
,
Gabaldon
 
J
,
Rocho-Levine
 
J
,
Petrov
 
V
,
Moore
 
MJ
(
2018
)
Swimming energy economy in bottlenose dolphins under variable drag loading
.
Front Mar Sci
 
5
:
1
13
.

VanWormer
 
E
 et al. (
2019
)
Viral emergence in marine mammals in the North Pacific may be linked to Arctic Sea ice reduction
.
Sci Rep
 
9
:
15569
.

Walker
 
KA
,
Trites
 
AW
,
Haulena
 
M
,
Weary
 
DM
(
2012
)
A review of the effects of different marking and tagging techniques on marine mammals
.
Wildl Res
 
39
:
15
30
.

Ware
 
DM
(
1978
)
Bioenergetics of pelagic Fish: theoretical change in swimming speed and ration with body size
.
J Fish Res Board Canada
 
35
:
220
228
.

Warren
 
VE
,
Miller
 
PJO
,
Tyack
 
PL
(
2020
)
Short-term responses of sperm whales Physeter macrocephalus to the attachment of suction cup tags
.
Mar Ecol Prog Ser
 
645
:
219
234
.

Watt
 
CA
,
Orr
 
JR
,
Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
,
Nielsen
 
NH
,
Ferguson
 
SH
(
2015
)
Differences in dive behaviour among the world’s three narwhal Monodon monoceros populations correspond with dietary differences
.
Mar Ecol Prog Ser
 
525
:
273
285
.

Webb
 
PM
,
Kostecki
 
PT
,
Stevens
 
ED
(
1984
)
The effect of size and swimming speed on locomotor kinematics of rainbow trout
.
J Exp Biol
 
109
:
77
95
.

Whitney
 
NM
,
White
 
CF
,
Gleiss
 
AC
,
Schwieterman
 
GD
,
Anderson
 
P
,
Hueter
 
RE
,
Skomal
 
GB
(
2016
)
A novel method for determining post-release mortality, behavior, and recovery period using acceleration data loggers
.
Fish Res
 
183
:
210
221
.

Williams
 
TM
,
Blackwell
 
SB
,
Richter
 
B
,
Sinding
 
M-HS
,
Heide-Jørgensen
 
MP
(
2017a
)
Paradoxical escape responses by narwhals (Monodon monoceros)
.
Science
 
(80- ) 358: 1328–1331
.

Williams
 
TM
,
Fuiman
 
LA
,
Horning
 
M
,
Davis
 
RW
(
2004
)
The cost of foraging by a marine predator, the Weddell seal Leptonychotes weddellii: pricing by the stroke
.
J Exp Biol
 
207
:
973
982
.

Williams
 
TM
 et al. (
2015
)
Exercise at depth alters bradycardia and incidence of cardiac anomalies in deep-diving marine mammals
.
Nat Commun
 
6
:
1
9
.

Williams
 
TM
,
Kendall
 
TL
,
Richter
 
BP
,
Ribeiro-French
 
CR
,
John
 
JS
,
Odell
 
KL
,
Losch
 
BA
,
Feuerbach
 
DA
,
Stamper
 
MA
(
2017b
)
Swimming and diving energetics in dolphins: A stroke-by-stroke analysis for predicting the cost of flight responses in wild odontocetes
.
J Exp Biol
 
220
:
1135
1145
.

Williams
 
TM
,
Noren
 
SR
,
Glenn
 
M
(
2011
)
Extreme physiological adaptations as predictors of climate-change sensitivity in the narwhal, Monodon monoceros
.
Mar Mammal Sci
 
27
:
334
349
.

Wilson
 
RP
 et al. (
2019
)
Estimates for energy expenditure in free-living animals using acceleration proxies; a reappraisal
.
J Anim Ecol.
doi: .

Wilson
 
RP
,
McMahon
 
CR
(
2006
)
Measuring devices on wild animals: what constitutes acceptable practice ?
 
Front Ecol Environ
 
4
:
147
154
.

Wilson
 
RP
,
Shepard
 
ELC
,
Liebsch
 
N
(
2008
)
Prying into the intimate details of animal lives: use of a daily diary on animals
.
Endanger Species Res
 
4
:
123
137
.

Wood
 
SN
(
2006
)
Generalized additive models: an introduction with R
,
1st Edition. CRC Press, Taylor & Francis Group, Boca Raton
.

Ydesen
 
KS
,
Wisniewska
 
DM
,
Hansen
 
JD
,
Beedholm
 
K
,
Johnson
 
M
,
Madsen
 
PT
(
2014
)
What a jerk: prey engulfment revealed by high-rate, super-cranial accelerometry on a harbour seal (Phoca vitulina)
.
J Exp Biol
 
217
:
2239
2243
.

Zuur
 
AF
,
Ieno
 
EN
,
Walker
 
NJ
,
Saveliev
 
AA
,
Smith
 
GM
(
2009
)
Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R
.
Springer Science & Business Media
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Editor: Steven Cooke
Steven Cooke
Editor
Search for other works by this author on: