Abstract

Down syndrome (DS), caused by trisomy 21, is the most common chromosomal disorder associated with developmental cognitive deficits. Despite intensive efforts, the genetic mechanisms underlying developmental cognitive deficits remain poorly understood, and no treatment has been proven effective. The previous mouse-based experiments suggest that the so-called Down syndrome critical region of human chromosome 21 is an important region for this phenotype, which is demarcated by Setd4/Cbr1 and Fam3b/Mx2. We first confirmed the importance of the Cbr1-Fam3b region using compound mutant mice, which carry a duplication spanning the entire human chromosome 21 orthologous region on mouse chromosome 16 [Dp(16)1Yey] and Ms1Rhr. By dividing the Setd4-Mx2 region into complementary Setd4-Kcnj6 and Kcnj15-Mx2 intervals, we started an unbiased dissection through generating and analyzing Dp(16)1Yey/Df(16Setd4-Kcnj6)Yey and Dp(16)1Yey/Df(16Kcnj15-Mx2)Yey mice. Surprisingly, the Dp(16)1Yey-associated cognitive phenotypes were not rescued by either deletion in the compound mutants, suggesting the possible presence of at least one causative gene in each of the two regions. The partial rescue by a Dyrk1a mutation in a compound mutant carrying Dp(16)1Yey and the Dyrk1a mutation confirmed the causative role of Dyrk1a, whereas the absence of a similar rescue by Df(16Dyrk1a-Kcnj6)Yey in Dp(16)1Yey/Df(16Dyrk1a-Kcnj6)Yey mice demonstrated the importance of Kcnj6. Our results revealed the high levels of complexities of gene actions and interactions associated with the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region as well as their relationship with developmental cognitive deficits in DS.

Introduction

Human trisomy 21 [Down syndrome (DS)] is the most common chromosomal abnormality associated with developmental cognitive deficits in the human population (1,2), occurring in one in approximately 691 and 1000 newborns in the USA (3) and Europe (4), respectively. In addition, the pregnancy termination rate after pre-natal diagnosis of human trisomy 21 has not increased, and the incidence rate of DS has not decreased in the last decade in countries such as the USA, due partly to improved medical care and social support for individuals with DS (5). However, despite improved medical care, there is still no effective treatment available to improve cognitive function for individuals with DS. Therefore, there is a particularly urgent need to explore in depth the genetic mechanisms underlying developmental cognitive deficits in DS to facilitate the development of new interventions.

To establish genotype–phenotype relationships, several groups have examined human segmental trisomies. In these studies, data generated from patients with segmental trisomy of human chromosome 21 (Hsa21) were used to map genomic regions associated with DS phenotypes. During these investigative efforts, the Down syndrome critical region (DSCR) was defined and proposed to be responsible for major DS phenotypes, including developmental cognitive deficits (6,7). Unfortunately, interpretation of the genotype–phenotype relationships inferred from these studies is complicated because some patients with segmental trisomy 21 carried additional chromosomal anomalies. For instance, through translocation, some individuals were segmentally trisomic for other chromosomes (6,7). Another problem is that the triplication endpoints almost always differ in each case of segmental trisomy, which makes it difficult to distinguish the contributions of trisomy from individual difference (8).

To pursue an alternative strategy for establishing the genotype–phenotype relationship, mouse models have been generated on the basis of evolutionary conservation between Hsa21 and three regions of the mouse genome located on mouse chromosome 10 (Mmu10), Mmu16 and Mmu17. The Hsa21 orthologous region on Mmu16 is the largest one and contains ∼65.7% of all the Hsa21 gene orthologs (Fig. 1). Triplication of this region caused developmental cognitive deficits in Dp(16)1Yey/+, abbreviated as Dp(16)1/+, mice (9). The mouse DSCR, demarcated by Setd4/Cbr1 and Fam3b/Mx2, is located within this region (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Material, Table S1). Mouse-based experimental results support the proposal that the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region plays a critical role in causing developmental cognitive deficits (10–12). Of 29–31 Hsa21 gene orthologs in the region, Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 are considered important candidates for contributing to this phenotype (10). It is possible that these and other genes in the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region contribute to the phenotypes by interacting directly and/or indirectly among themselves and/or with other triplicated genes. In this study, we set out to examine these possibilities by generating and analyzing mouse mutants carrying specific genetic alterations.

Mouse mutants and their cognitive phenotypes. Hsa21 and the orthologous region on Mmu16 are shown. Dashed lines indicate the locations of specific genes. PR, partial rescue observed; NR, no rescue observed.
Figure 1.

Mouse mutants and their cognitive phenotypes. Hsa21 and the orthologous region on Mmu16 are shown. Dashed lines indicate the locations of specific genes. PR, partial rescue observed; NR, no rescue observed.

Results

Confirmation of the impact of the Cbr1-Fam3b region using Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice

The mouse DSCR is located in the Hsa21 orthologous region on Mmu16, which is demarcated by Setd4/Cbr1 and Fam3b/Mx2 (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Material, Table S1). Dp(16)1, a duplication spanning the entire Hsa21 orthologous region on Mmu16, serves as a reference for genetic dissection of the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region (Fig. 1) (13). The presence of Dp(16)1/+ caused impairment of cognitively relevant phenotypes in mice, including contextual fear conditioning and hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) (9) which is viewed as a major physiological phenomenon associated with learning and memory (14,15). We have previously shown that reducing the copy number of the Cbr1-Fam3b region from three to two rescued impaired hippocampal LTP caused by the presence of Dp(16)1 (12). In this study, we extended the analysis of the Cbr1-Fam3b region by including the T-maze spontaneous alternation test and the contextual fear-conditioning test. The T-maze test has been used to reveal functional abnormalities of the mouse hippocampal system. The contextual fear-conditioning test has been used to examine the capacity for hippocampal-mediated contextual memory in mice. Both tests have been used extensively to analyze mouse models of DS (9,10,16). In the T-maze test, Dp(16)1/+ mice performed significantly worse than wild-type controls (P < 0.01). But converting the Cbr1-Fam3b to two copies rendered the difference of the performances between Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice and wild-type controls insignificant (P = 0.247), even though the difference between Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr and Dp(16)1/+ mice is not significant either (P = 0.085; Fig. 2A). In the contextual fear-conditioning test, mice of all genotypes had a similar low baseline freezing level before presentation of the foot shock (P > 0.05; Fig. 2B). Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr, Dp(16)1/+ and +/+ mice showed elevated freezing behavior when they were returned to the test chamber 24 h after the initial training and foot shock. Similar to our previous observations, Dp(16)1/+ mice froze significantly less than wild-type controls (P < 0.01) (Fig. 2B) (9). Additional comparative analysis showed that Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice froze significantly more than Dp(16)1/+ mice (P < 0.05), but did not reach the level achieved by wild-type controls (P < 0.01). To facilitate interpretation of the contextual fear condition data, we performed a foot-shock sensitivity test and detected no difference in the mean threshold of the current to elicit flinching or vocalizing between mice with the aforementioned genotypes as well as all other genotypes used in the subsequent contextual fear-conditioning tests in this study (data not shown). These results indicate that the normalization of the Cbr1-Fam3b region to two copies partially rescued the cognitive deficits caused by the presence of Dp(16)1.

Analysis of Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice. (A) Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr (n = 15), Dp(16)/+ (n = 13) and +/+ (n = 13) mice were examined in the T-maze. (B) The three groups of mice in (A) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.
Figure 2.

Analysis of Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice. (A) Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr (n = 15), Dp(16)/+ (n = 13) and +/+ (n = 13) mice were examined in the T-maze. (B) The three groups of mice in (A) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.

Examination of the impacts of the Setd4-Kcnj6 and Kcnj15-Mx2 regions by generating and analyzing compound mouse mutants

After confirming the impact of the Cbr1-Fam3b region on cognitive behaviors using Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice, we started the unbiased dissection of the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region by dividing the region into two complementary intervals. To do this, we generated Df(16Setd4-Kcnj6)Yey [abbreviated as Df(16)5Yey or Df(16)5] and Df(16Kcnj15-Mx2)Yey [abbreviated as Df(16)6Yey or Df(16)6], which have deletions of 15 and 16 Hsa21 gene orthologs, respectively (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Material, Table S1).

To generate Df(16)5 in mouse embryonic stem (ES) cells, MICER clones MHPN267e05 and MHPP54c08 (17) were used to target loxP into the regions proximal to Setd4 and distal to Kcnj6, respectively (Fig. 3A). To generate Df(16)6 in ES cells, MICER clones MHPP54c08 and MHPN178b08 (17) were used to target loxP into the regions proximal to Kcnj15 and distal to Mx2, respectively (Fig. 4A). The ES cell clones carrying Df(16)5 and Df(16)6 were generated by transfection of a Cre-expression vector into double-targeted ES cells and identified by Southern blot analysis (Figs 3A and 4A). The deletions were confirmed by fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH) analysis (18,19) (Figs 3B and C and 4B and C). Df(16)5/+ and Df(16)6/+ mice were generated using the aforementioned ES cells and were identified by Southern blot analysis (Figs 3D and 4D). Df(16)5/+ and Df(16)6/+ mice appear overtly normal. Both strains showed transmission ratio distortion, with either deletion transmitted at the ratio lower than predicted.

Generation and analysis of Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 mice. (A) Strategy to generate Df(16)5. 5′, 5′ half of a HPRT minigene; 3′, 3′ half of a HPRT minigene; N, neomycin resistance gene; P, puromycin resistance gene; Ty, tyrosinase transgene; Ag, K14-Agouti transgene; ►, loxP; A, AflII; K, KpnI. (B) Genomic locations of BAC probes for FISH analysis. (C) FISH analysis of metaphase chromosomes prepared from Df(16)5/+ ES cells. (D) Southern blot analysis, using Probe 5C, of AflII-digested tail DNA from a Df(16)5/+ mouse. (E) Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 (n = 13), Dp(16)/+ (n = 13) and +/+ (n = 16) mice were examined in the T-maze. (F) The three groups of mice in (E) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. (G) Analysis of hippocampal LTP. Brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 (n = 9), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 9) and +/+ (n = 9) mice were analyzed using electrophysiological recordings in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Recordings of fEPSPs were carried out before and after TBS inductions. Evoked potentials were normalized to the fEPSPs recorded prior to TBS induction. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.
Figure 3.

Generation and analysis of Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 mice. (A) Strategy to generate Df(16)5. 5′, 5′ half of a HPRT minigene; 3′, 3′ half of a HPRT minigene; N, neomycin resistance gene; P, puromycin resistance gene; Ty, tyrosinase transgene; Ag, K14-Agouti transgene; ►, loxP; A, AflII; K, KpnI. (B) Genomic locations of BAC probes for FISH analysis. (C) FISH analysis of metaphase chromosomes prepared from Df(16)5/+ ES cells. (D) Southern blot analysis, using Probe 5C, of AflII-digested tail DNA from a Df(16)5/+ mouse. (E) Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 (n = 13), Dp(16)/+ (n = 13) and +/+ (n = 16) mice were examined in the T-maze. (F) The three groups of mice in (E) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. (G) Analysis of hippocampal LTP. Brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 (n = 9), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 9) and +/+ (n = 9) mice were analyzed using electrophysiological recordings in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Recordings of fEPSPs were carried out before and after TBS inductions. Evoked potentials were normalized to the fEPSPs recorded prior to TBS induction. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.

Generation and analysis of Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice. (A) Strategy to generate Df(16)6. For the definitions of the abbreviations, see the legend in Figure 3A. A, AflII; K, KpnI; X, XbaI. (B) Genomic locations of BAC probes for FISH analysis. (C) FISH analysis of metaphase chromosomes prepared from Df(16)6/+ ES cells. (D) Southern blot analysis, using Probe 6C, of AflII-digested tail DNA from a Df(16)6/+ mouse. (E) Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 (n = 10), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 12) and +/+ (n = 11) mice were examined in the T-maze. (F) The three groups of mice in (E) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. (G) Analysis of hippocampal LTP. Brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 (n = 8), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 9) and +/+ (n = 9) mice were analyzed using electrophysiological recordings in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Recordings of fEPSPs were carried out before and after TBS inductions. Evoked potentials were normalized to the fEPSPs recorded prior to TBS induction. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.
Figure 4.

Generation and analysis of Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice. (A) Strategy to generate Df(16)6. For the definitions of the abbreviations, see the legend in Figure 3A. A, AflII; K, KpnI; X, XbaI. (B) Genomic locations of BAC probes for FISH analysis. (C) FISH analysis of metaphase chromosomes prepared from Df(16)6/+ ES cells. (D) Southern blot analysis, using Probe 6C, of AflII-digested tail DNA from a Df(16)6/+ mouse. (E) Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 (n = 10), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 12) and +/+ (n = 11) mice were examined in the T-maze. (F) The three groups of mice in (E) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. (G) Analysis of hippocampal LTP. Brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 (n = 8), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 9) and +/+ (n = 9) mice were analyzed using electrophysiological recordings in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Recordings of fEPSPs were carried out before and after TBS inductions. Evoked potentials were normalized to the fEPSPs recorded prior to TBS induction. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.

To assess the impact on the level of mRNAs by the copy number variation of their coding genes, we analyzed expression of App, Pigp, Dyrk1a, Kcnj6 and Hmgn1 in Dp(16)1/+, Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 brains using TaqMan® real-time quantitative reverse transcriptase–polymerase chain reaction (RT–PCR) (Fig. 1). All five genes are present at three copies in Dp(16)1/+ brains, but Pigp, Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 are converted to two copies in Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 brains and Hmgn1 is converted to two copies in Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 brains. The quantitative RT–PCR results showed that the detected mRNA levels reflected the copy numbers of their coding genes in the mutants (Fig. 1 and Table 1).

Table 1.

Normalized relative values of expression in the brains (RQ ± SEM)*

Gene nameDp(16)1/+ over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 over +/+Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 over +/+
App1.46 ± 0.101.56 ± 0.111.57 ± 0.151.55 ± 0.051.54 ± 0.03
Pigp1.50 ± 0.101.05 ± 0.191.84 ± 0.221.67 ± 0.091.48 ± 0.07
Dyrk1a1.52 ± 0.090.84 ± 0.071.22 ± 0.080.95 ± 0.031.03 ± 0.00
Kcnj61.37 ± 0.060.87 ± 0.091.23 ± 0.080.96 ± 0.061.28 ± 0.05
Hmgn11.55 ± 0.131.62 ± 0.261.10 ± 0.011.70 ± 0.081.67 ± 0.05
Gene nameDp(16)1/+ over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 over +/+Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 over +/+
App1.46 ± 0.101.56 ± 0.111.57 ± 0.151.55 ± 0.051.54 ± 0.03
Pigp1.50 ± 0.101.05 ± 0.191.84 ± 0.221.67 ± 0.091.48 ± 0.07
Dyrk1a1.52 ± 0.090.84 ± 0.071.22 ± 0.080.95 ± 0.031.03 ± 0.00
Kcnj61.37 ± 0.060.87 ± 0.091.23 ± 0.080.96 ± 0.061.28 ± 0.05
Hmgn11.55 ± 0.131.62 ± 0.261.10 ± 0.011.70 ± 0.081.67 ± 0.05

*The values represent the means of triplicated samples. Gapdh was used as an internal control and is disomic in all strains. In the Hsa21 orthologous region on Mmu16, App is located proximal to the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region, Pigp, Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 are located within the deleted region in Df(16)5 and Hmgn1 is located within the deleted region of Df(16)6.

Table 1.

Normalized relative values of expression in the brains (RQ ± SEM)*

Gene nameDp(16)1/+ over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 over +/+Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 over +/+
App1.46 ± 0.101.56 ± 0.111.57 ± 0.151.55 ± 0.051.54 ± 0.03
Pigp1.50 ± 0.101.05 ± 0.191.84 ± 0.221.67 ± 0.091.48 ± 0.07
Dyrk1a1.52 ± 0.090.84 ± 0.071.22 ± 0.080.95 ± 0.031.03 ± 0.00
Kcnj61.37 ± 0.060.87 ± 0.091.23 ± 0.080.96 ± 0.061.28 ± 0.05
Hmgn11.55 ± 0.131.62 ± 0.261.10 ± 0.011.70 ± 0.081.67 ± 0.05
Gene nameDp(16)1/+ over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 over +/+Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 over +/+Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 over +/+
App1.46 ± 0.101.56 ± 0.111.57 ± 0.151.55 ± 0.051.54 ± 0.03
Pigp1.50 ± 0.101.05 ± 0.191.84 ± 0.221.67 ± 0.091.48 ± 0.07
Dyrk1a1.52 ± 0.090.84 ± 0.071.22 ± 0.080.95 ± 0.031.03 ± 0.00
Kcnj61.37 ± 0.060.87 ± 0.091.23 ± 0.080.96 ± 0.061.28 ± 0.05
Hmgn11.55 ± 0.131.62 ± 0.261.10 ± 0.011.70 ± 0.081.67 ± 0.05

*The values represent the means of triplicated samples. Gapdh was used as an internal control and is disomic in all strains. In the Hsa21 orthologous region on Mmu16, App is located proximal to the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region, Pigp, Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 are located within the deleted region in Df(16)5 and Hmgn1 is located within the deleted region of Df(16)6.

To analyze the impact of the Setd4-Kcnj6 and Kcnj15-Mx2 regions on DS-associated cognitively relevant phenotypes, we compared the phenotypes of Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice with those of Dp(16)1/+ mice and wild-type controls. Our results showed that both Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice performed significantly worse than wild-type controls in the T-maze and contextual fear-conditioning tests (P < 0.05; Figs 3E and F and 4E and F). In addition, neither Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 nor Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice showed improvement over Dp(16)1/+ mice in these tests (P > 0.05; Figs 3E and F and 4E and F). To assess the impact of the Setd4-Kcnj6 and Kcnj15-Mx2 regions on hippocampal synaptic plasticity, we performed in vitro electrophysiological analysis in the CA1 region of hippocampus in brain slices with the focus on hippocampal LTP. LTP in the brain slices was induced by theta burst stimulation (TBS). Mean slopes of extracellular field excitatory post-synaptic potentials (fEPSPs) were measured for 60 min after the TBS. A comparative analysis showed that the magnitude of LTP of the synaptic response after TBS of the brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice are significantly lower than that from wild-type controls (P < 0.05, Figs 3G and 4G). Further comparison showed that the magnitude of LTP after TBS of the brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice appeared slightly higher than that from Dp(16)1/+ mice, but the differences are not statistically significant (P > 0.05; Figs 3G and 4G). The behavioral and electrophysiological data indicate that converting either the Setd4-Kcnj6 region or the Kcnj15-Mx2 region to two copies does not rescue the cognitively relevant phenotypes caused by Dp(16)1. This suggests that both Setd4-Kcnj6 and Kcnj15-Mx2 regions contain dosage-sensitive genes and that the causative gene(s) in either region can contribute to the cognitively relevant phenotypes.

Examination of the impact of Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 by generating and analyzing compound mouse mutants

We showed earlier that reducing the copy number of the Setd4-Kcnj6 region from three to two in Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 mice did not improve learning and memory compared with Dp(16)1/+ mice. This result is unexpected because Dyrk1a and Kcnj6, the major candidate genes for DS-associated cognitive deficits (10), are located within this interval. Dyrk1a encodes a dual-specificity tyrosine-phosphorylation-regulated kinase, which plays a critical role in signaling (20–22). Kcnj6 encodes for a G protein-activated potassium channel, which regulates neuronal excitability by mediating the inhibitory effects of G-protein-coupled receptors for neuromodulators and neurotransmitters (23,24). If copy number increases of these genes contribute to the cognitively relevant phenotypes, then normalizations of the copy number in Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 mice should have led to improvement of cognitive function when compared with Dp(16)1/+ mice. Therefore, we decided to examine the impact of Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 using the subtractive strategy with Dp(16)1 as the reference.

To examine the impact of Dyrk1a, we generated a Dyrk1a mutant allele in mice using the SIGTR ES cell line XQ0369, which carries Dyrk1aGt(XQ0369)Wtsi (abbreviated as Dyrk1am1). By sequencing the RT–PCR product, we confirmed the presence of the specific fusion mRNA in the XQ0369 ES cells generated by insertion of the beta-Geo cassette into intron 4 of Dyrk1a (Fig. 5B and Supplementary Material, Fig. S1).This insertion causes a disruption in the 321-amino acid kinase domain of DYRK1A, which occurs after the first 14 amino acids encoded by exon 4. To examine the impact of Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 simultaneously, we generated Df(16Dyrk1a-Kcnj6)Yey [abbreviated as Df(16)7Yey or Df(16)7] in mouse ES cells. MICER clones MHPN85f19 and MHPP54c08 (17) were used to target loxP into the regions proximal to Dyrk1a and distal to Kcnj6, respectively (Fig. 6A). The ES cell clones carrying Df(16)7 were generated by transfection of a Cre-expression vector into double-targeted ES cells and identified by Southern blot analysis (Fig. 6A). The deletion was confirmed by FISH analysis (Fig. 6B and C). Dyrk1am1/+ and Df(16)7Yey/+ mice were generated using the aforementioned ES cells. Dyrk1am1/+ mice were maintained using beta-Geo-specific PCR-based genotyping (Fig. 5A). Dyrk1am1/+ mice appear overtly normal. After backcrossing to C57BL/6J or 129Sv mice, Dyrk1am1/+ mice showed transmission ratio distortion with the mutation transmitted at the ratio lower than predicted. Therefore, Dyrk1am1/+ mice were maintained by crossing Dyrk1am1/+ mice either with wild-type (129Sv × C57BL/6J)F1 mice or with Dp(16)1/+ mice. Sibling mating of Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 mice did not result in any viable pups carrying either the Dp(16)1 allele or the Dyrk1am1 allele alone, indicating that the homozygosity of either mutation led to embryonic lethality. The embryonic lethal phenotype caused by Dyrk1am1/Dyrk1am1 and the aforementioned mutation-associated haploinsufficiency are very similar to the phenotypes of another Dyrk1a knockout mouse strain (25). Df(16)7/+ mice were identified by Southern blot analysis (Fig. 6D). Df(16)7Yey/+ mice appear overtly normal. The strain showed transmission ratio distortion with the deletion transmitted at the ratio lower than predicted.

Generation and analysis of Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1mice. (A) The gene trap insertion site in Dyrk1a in the ES cell clone QX0369. Arrow heads indicate the primer locations for RT–PCR. (B) Confirmation of the gene trap event by RT–PCR. RNAs were purified from the QX0369 ES cells and the brains of a Dyrk1am1/+ mouse and a wild-type littermate. RT–PCR was performed using the forward primer located in exon 4 of Dyrk1a and the reverse primer in the beta-Geo cassette at the annealing temperature of 70°C. The resulting product was 296 bp. Lane 1, Dyrk1am1/+ mouse; lane 2, wild-type littermate; lane 3, Dyrk1a gene trap cell clone QX0369; MW, 100 bp DNA ladder. Sequencing of the PCR products confirms the presence of a fusion mRNA (Supplementary Material, Fig. S1). (C) Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 (n = 16), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 16) and +/+ (n = 16) mice were examined in the T-maze. (D) The three groups of mice in (C) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. (E) Analysis of LTP. Brain slices from Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 (n = 13), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 13) and +/+ (n = 13) mice were analyzed using electrophysiological recordings in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Recordings of fEPSPs were carried out before and after TBS inductions. Evoked potentials were normalized to the fEPSPs recorded prior to TBS induction. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.
Figure 5.

Generation and analysis of Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1mice. (A) The gene trap insertion site in Dyrk1a in the ES cell clone QX0369. Arrow heads indicate the primer locations for RT–PCR. (B) Confirmation of the gene trap event by RT–PCR. RNAs were purified from the QX0369 ES cells and the brains of a Dyrk1am1/+ mouse and a wild-type littermate. RT–PCR was performed using the forward primer located in exon 4 of Dyrk1a and the reverse primer in the beta-Geo cassette at the annealing temperature of 70°C. The resulting product was 296 bp. Lane 1, Dyrk1am1/+ mouse; lane 2, wild-type littermate; lane 3, Dyrk1a gene trap cell clone QX0369; MW, 100 bp DNA ladder. Sequencing of the PCR products confirms the presence of a fusion mRNA (Supplementary Material, Fig. S1). (C) Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 (n = 16), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 16) and +/+ (n = 16) mice were examined in the T-maze. (D) The three groups of mice in (C) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. (E) Analysis of LTP. Brain slices from Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 (n = 13), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 13) and +/+ (n = 13) mice were analyzed using electrophysiological recordings in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Recordings of fEPSPs were carried out before and after TBS inductions. Evoked potentials were normalized to the fEPSPs recorded prior to TBS induction. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.

Generation and analysis of Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice. (A) Strategy to generate Df(16)7. For the definitions of the abbreviations, see the legend in Figure 3A. H, HpaI; K, KpnI; X, XbaI. (B) Genomic locations of BAC probes for FISH analysis. (C) FISH analysis of metaphase chromosomes prepared from Df(16)7/+ ES cells. (D) Southern blot analysis, using Probe 7C, of HpaI-digested tail DNA from a Df(16)7/+ mouse. (E) Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 (n = 14), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 14) and +/+ (n = 14) mice were examined in the T-maze. (F) The three groups of mice in (E) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. (G) Analysis of hippocampal LTP. Brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 (n = 8), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 9) and +/+ (n = 9) mice were analyzed using electrophysiological recordings in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Recordings of fEPSPs were carried out before and after TBS inductions. Evoked potentials were normalized to the fEPSPs recorded prior to TBS induction. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.
Figure 6.

Generation and analysis of Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice. (A) Strategy to generate Df(16)7. For the definitions of the abbreviations, see the legend in Figure 3A. H, HpaI; K, KpnI; X, XbaI. (B) Genomic locations of BAC probes for FISH analysis. (C) FISH analysis of metaphase chromosomes prepared from Df(16)7/+ ES cells. (D) Southern blot analysis, using Probe 7C, of HpaI-digested tail DNA from a Df(16)7/+ mouse. (E) Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 (n = 14), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 14) and +/+ (n = 14) mice were examined in the T-maze. (F) The three groups of mice in (E) were examined in a contextual fear-conditioning test. The percentages of time frozen before the foot shock and during the 24 h contextual tests are shown. (G) Analysis of hippocampal LTP. Brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 (n = 8), Dp(16)1/+ (n = 9) and +/+ (n = 9) mice were analyzed using electrophysiological recordings in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Recordings of fEPSPs were carried out before and after TBS inductions. Evoked potentials were normalized to the fEPSPs recorded prior to TBS induction. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01.

To assess the impact on the level of mRNAs by the copy number variation of their coding genes, we analyzed expression of App, Pigp, Dyrk1a, Kcnj6 and Hmgn1 in Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 brains using TaqMan real-time quantitative PCR. All five genes are present at three copies in Dp(16)1Yey/+ brains, but Dyrk1a is converted to two copies in Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 mice and Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 are converted to two copies in Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice (Fig. 1). The quantitative PCR results showed that the detected mRNA levels reflected the copy numbers of their coding genes in the mutants (Fig. 1 and Table 1).

To analyze the impact of Dyrk1a on DS-associated cognitively relevant phenotypes, we compared Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 mice with Dp(16)1/+ mice and wild-type controls in the T-maze and contextual fear-conditioning tests. Our results showed that Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 mice performed better than Dp(16)1/+ mice in the T-maze and contextual fear-conditioning tests (P < 0.01), even though their performance did not reach the level achieved by wild-type controls (P < 0.05) (Fig. 5C and D). When we compared hippocampal LTPs, we found that the magnitude of LTP after TBS of the brain slices from Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 mice was higher than that from Dp(16)1/+ mice (P < 0.01), but it was not as high as that from the wild-type controls (P < 0.01; Fig. 5E). These data indicate that Dyrk1a is indeed a causative gene for the cognitive deficits observed in Dp(16)1/+ mice. To examine the simultaneous impact of Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 on cognitively relevant phenotypes, we compared Dp(16)1/Df(16)7, Dp(16)1/+ and wild-type control mice in the T-maze and contextual fear-conditioning tests. Our results showed that Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice performed significantly worse than wild-type controls in the T-maze and contextual fear-conditioning tests (P < 0.05). In addition, Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice did not show improvement over Dp(16)1/+ mice in these tests (P > 0.05; Fig. 6E and F). We compared hippocampal LTPs of these mice and found that the magnitude of LTP after TBS of the brain slices from Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice was significantly lower than that from wild-type control mice (P < 0.01, Fig. 6G). Further comparison showed that the magnitude of LTP from Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice appeared slightly higher than that from Dp(16)1/+ mice, but the difference is not statistically significant (P > 0.05; Fig. 6G).

The aforementioned genetic analysis based on Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr, Dp(16)1/Df(16)5, Dp(16)1/Df(16)6, Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice indicates that both Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 are key genes in DS-associated cognitively relevant phenotypes, with Dyrk1a playing a causative role and Kcnj6 playing a critical mediating role in these phenotypes. Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice, but not Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 mice, showed improved cognitive function when compared with Dp(16)1/+ mice, suggesting that the Kcnj15-Mx2 interval contains a causative gene(s) for DS-associated developmental cognitive deficits (Fig. 1).

Discussion

Many surprises have occurred in the history of genetic analysis of DS. This is because DS is caused by the triplication of the entire human chromosome 21. Actions and interactions of approximately 175 gene orthologs associated with Hsa21 often led to unexpected results in relationship to DS phenotypes.

In this study, we focussed on genetic analysis of the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region, which is built upon the previous results from other laboratories as well as from our own laboratories. Using the subtractive strategy, we confirmed the importance of the Cbr1-Fam3b region in causing developmental cognitive deficits in DS using Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice. In the attempt to identify the location(s) of the causative gene(s), we generated and analyzed two compound mutants, Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)6, each of which carries a deletion spanning approximately half of the Setd4-Mx2 region (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Material, Table S1). The first major surprise is that neither compound mutant showed improvement over Dp(16)1/+ mice in T-maze and contextual fear-conditioning tests. Based on this result, we speculate the possible presence of at least two dosage-sensitive causative genes, with at least one in each deletion region. Our speculation is that when the causative gene(s) in the Setd4-Kcnj6 region is normalized in Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 mice, the causative gene(s) in the Kcnj15-Mx2 region could still exert its effect on causing cognitive deficits. Similarly, when the causative gene(s) in the Kcnj15-Mx2 region is normalized in Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice, the causative gene(s) in the Setd4-Kcnj6 region could still be able to have an impact on cognitive function.

Because Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 have been proposed as the major causative genes for cognitive deficits in DS, we generated and analyzed Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 and Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice. The improved performance of Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 mice in T-maze and contextual fear-conditioning tests over Dp(16)1/+ mice confirms the causative role of Dyrk1a (Figs 1 and 5). This result is similar to the data generated using Ts65Dn mice-based subtractive approach (26), even though there are significant genomic differences between Ts65Dn and Dp(16)1/+ mice, such as the extra 15 Hsa21 gene orthologs triplicated in Dp(16)1/+ mice over Ts65Dn mice, and the triplication of the Mmu17 centromeric region which is orthologous to a region of Hsa6 in Ts65Dn mice but not in Dp(16)1/+ mice. The analysis of the next compound mutant led to the second major surprise of our study that Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice showed no significant improvement of cognitively relevant phenotypes over Dp(16)1/+ mice when both Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 were converted to two copies in the Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice.

Through this study, we established two important facts. First, Dyrk1a is a causative gene because partial rescue occurs when Dyrk1a was converted to two copies in Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 mice. Secondly, Kcnj6 plays an important role because no rescue occurs when both Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 were converted to two copies in Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice. To interpret all our data generated in this study, we hypothesize the presence of a causative gene ‘X’ for DS-associated cognitive deficits in the Kcnj15-Mx2 region and further hypothesize that the effect of the triplication of this gene is suppressed by the triplication of Kcnj6. In Dp(16)1/+ mice, the presence of three copies of Dyrk1a contributes to cognitive deficits, whereas the effect of the triplication of X is suppressed by the triplication of Kcnj6 (Fig. 7A). In Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice, converting Dyrk1a to two copies causes partial rescue of cognitive deficits. Because there is another causative gene(s) outside of the Cbr1-Famb3 region in the Hsa21 orthologous region on Mmu16, the rescue is only partial. X is present only in two copies in Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice, so it does not contribute to cognitive deficits (Fig. 7B). In Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 mice, Dyrk1a is present in two copies and does not contribute to cognitive deficits, whereas X is present in three copies and Kcnj6 is present in two copies, so the effect of the triplication of X is not suppressed by Kcnj6. Therefore, X contributes to cognitive deficits in Dp(16)1/Df(16)5 mice (Fig. 7C). In Dp(16)1/Df(16)6 mice, Dyrk1a is present in three copies and contributes to cognitive deficits, whereas X is present only in two copies and does not contribute to cognitive deficits (Fig. 7D). In Dp(16)1/Dyrk1am1 mice, converting Dyrk1a to two copies causes partial rescue of cognitive deficits, while the effect of the triplication of X is suppressed by the triplication of Kcnj6 (Fig. 7E). In Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice, Dyrk1a is present in two copies and does not contribute to cognitive deficits, whereas X is present in three copies and Kcnj6 is present in two copies, so the effect of the triplication of X is not suppressed by Kcnj6. Therefore, X contributes to cognitive deficits in Dp(16)1/Df(16)7 mice (Fig. 7F).

Actions and interactions of the genes in the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region in relationship to DS-associated cognitive deficits. D, Dyrk1a; K, Kcnj6; X, a gene located in the deletion interval of Df(16)6. PR, partial rescue observed; NR, no rescue observed.
Figure 7.

Actions and interactions of the genes in the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region in relationship to DS-associated cognitive deficits. D, Dyrk1a; K, Kcnj6; X, a gene located in the deletion interval of Df(16)6. PR, partial rescue observed; NR, no rescue observed.

Our results set up a framework by which Kcnj6 may contribute to cognitive deficits associated with the triplication of the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region (Fig. 7). This information may also explain why ethosuximide, an inhibitor of the Kcnj6-encoded protein, cannot rescue cognitive deficits of Ts65Dn mice (27), because Dyrk1a is present in three copies in Ts65Dn mice and also reducing the effectiveness of Kcnj6 could enhance the effect of the triplication of another causative gene, i.e. X in the Kcnj15-Mx2 region (Fig. 7).

One of the next challenges is to establish the identity of the X gene, which is necessary to understand the mechanism of Kcnj6-associated suppression. Among 16 Hsa21 gene orthologs in the Kcnj15-Mx2 region, Ets2, Psmg1, Brwd1, Hmgn1, Wrb, Pcp4 and Dscam are the potential candidates because they are well expressed in the brain (28–38). One of the approaches to identify the X gene is to compound a null allele of a candidate gene with Dp(16)1 and Df(16)7 simultaneously. This can be achieved by transferring Df(16)7 or the null allele of the gene to the chromosome homolog carrying Dp(16)1 by crossover. The identity of the X gene will be revealed by partial rescue of cognitive deficits in such a triple compound mutant. Alternative to individual null alleles, smaller engineered deletions within the Kcnj15-Mx2 region could be used in the same compounding strategy to systemically dissect the region with the goal to identify the X gene.

Our aforementioned reasoning and gene-hunting strategy are built upon the hypothetic presence of a single causative gene in the Kcnj15-Mx2 region, but an additional unknown causative gene(s) may exist in the Kcnj15-Mx2 region and/or the Setd4-Kcnj6 region. Thus, the gene-hunting strategy may need to be expanded to identify the potential additional causative gene(s).

By generating and analyzing multiple compound mutants, we showed here a highly complex picture of gene actions and interactions associated with the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region as well as the impact of these events on developmental cognitive deficits in DS. Such complexities present a challenge to fully elucidate the precise mechanisms underlying DS-associated developmental cognitive deficits.

Materials and Methods

Generation of mouse mutants

We generated new chromosomal deletions using Cre/loxP-mediated chromosome engineering (39). Specific MICER clones (17) were selected for use as the targeting vectors to deliver loxP to the two endpoints of each deletion in the genome of mouse ES cells, i.e. AB2.2 ES cells (40). Prior to gene targeting, MICER clones were linearized in the mouse genomic DNA inserts with specific restriction enzymes (Supplementary Material, Table S2). The linearized targeting vectors were electroporated into ES cells, which were then selected with G418 or puromycin. Double-targeted ES cell clones were identified by Southern blot analysis using PCR products as probes. ES cell culture and electroporation were carried out as described (41). To induce recombination between targeted loxP sites, a Cre-expression vector, pOG231 (42), was electroporated into double-targeted cells. ES cell clones carrying individual deletions were identified by Southern blot analysis with ES cell DNA digested with specific restriction enzymes and hybridized with specific probes (Supplementary Material, Table S2) and confirmed by FISH analysis.

To generate a Dyrk1a mutant allele in mice, the SIGTR ES cell line XQ0369, which carries Dyrk1aGt(XQ0369)Wtsi (i.e. Dyrk1am1), was obtained from the Mutant Mouse Regional Resource Centers, (http://www.informatics.jax.org/allele/MGI:4338238). This mutant ES cell line was generated by the Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute via a gene trap event in intron 4 of Dyrk1a using the gene trap vector pGT01xf (http://www.sanger.ac.uk/resources/mouse/sigtr/). We examined this mutant ES cell line by carrying out RT–PCR analysis of ES cell RNA with PCR primers based on exon 4 of Dyrk1a (forward primer: 5′-GGGCCAGGGGGACGATTCCAGT-3′) and the beta-Geo cassette in the gene trap vector (reverse primer: 5′-CGCCAGGGTTTTCCCAGTCACGA-3′) (Fig. 5A). The RT–PCR product was sequenced to confirm the presence of the specific fusion mRNA as the consequence of the gene trap event (Fig. 5B and Supplementary Material, Fig. S1). Our sequence is identical to the junction sequence generated by the Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute for this cell line (http://www.genetrap.org/cgi-bin/annotation.py?cellline=XQ0369).

The ES cell lines carrying the aforementioned individual mutations were used to generate germline chimeras by injecting them into blastocysts isolated from C57BL/6J mice, as described previously (19,41,43). Deletion mutant mice were identified by Southern blot analysis of mouse tail DNA (Supplementary Material, Table S2). Dyrk1am1/+ mice were identified using beta-Geo-specific PCR-based analysis of mouse tail DNA with the following primer pair: 5′-ACGAGTTCTTCTGAGCGGGACTCT-3′ and 5′-GATAACCGTATTACCGCCTTTG-3′ (Fig. 5A) and confirmed by sequencing the junction between exon 4 of Dyrk1a and the gene trap vector pGT01xf using cDNA generated from brain mRNA of Dyrk1am1/+ mice (Fig. 5B).

Fluorescent in situ hybridization

FISH analysis was performed as described previously (18,19). The metaphase chromosome spreads of ES cells were prepared, as described previously (18,19). To detect the chromosomal deletions between Setd4 and Kcnj6, between Kcnj15 and Mx2 or between Dyrk1a and Kcnj6 on Mmu16, individual BAC clones carrying mouse genomic DNA mapped within these regions were selected and labeled with digoxigenin and detected with anti-digoxigenin-rhodamine antibody (Supplementary Material, Table S2). A BAC clone carrying mouse genomic DNA mapped to a Hsa21 orthologous region on Mmu16 but proximal to the Setd4/Cbr1-Fam3b/Mx2 region was used to identify Mmu16 and labeled with biotin and detected with fluorescein isothiocyanate-avidin (Supplementary Material, Table S2). Chromosomes were counterstained with 4′,6′-diamidino-2-2phenylindole.

Animals

Dp(16)1/+ mice were identified by Southern blot analysis (13) as well as by a PCR-based genotyping strategy. The latter was designed on the basis of the unique 355 bp junction sequence between the 5′-HPRT vector backbone and the mouse genomic DNA insert in the targeting vector pTVZfp295 (13) using the forward primer 5′-CTGCCAGCCACTCTAGCTCT-3′ and the reverse primer 5′-AATTTCTGTGGGGCAAAATG-3′. Mutant mice carrying Dp(16)1, Df(16)5, Df(16)6 and Df(16)7 were first established in a (129Sv × C57BL/6J)F1 background and backcrossed to C57BL/6J mice for five generations. Dyrk1am1/+ mice were first established by crossing chimeras with C57BL/6J mice, and the mutant progeny were maintained by crossing to (129Sv × C57BL/6J)F1 mice. Ms1Rhr mice were purchased from the Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME, USA) and maintained by crossing to (C57BL/6JEiJ × C3Sn.BLiA-Pde6b+)F1 mice (44). Dp(16)1;Ms1Rhr mice were generated by crossing Dp(16)1/+ mice to Ms1Rhr mice.

All mice were maintained at a temperature- and humidity-controlled animal facility with a 12 h light and dark schedule and had ad libitum access to food and water. All mice used in the experiments were 2–4 months old. Before behavioral experiments, each mouse was pre-handled for 2 min every day for 1 week. All the experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Roswell Park Cancer Institute.

RT–PCR and real-time quantitative RT–PCR

RT–PCR and real-time quantitative RT–PCR were used to analyze expression of the selected genes in ES cells and mice carrying different genotypes. Gapdh, located on Mmu6, was used as the internal disomic control for all of the mice examined. Total RNAs, isolated from ES cells or mouse brains using TRIzol Reagent (Life Technologies, Grand Island, NY, USA), were used to generate cDNA by Superscript version III reverse transcriptase (Life Technologies), which were then used as the templates for PCR experiments. For real-time quantitative RT–PCR analysis, the cDNA samples from three mice with each genotype were analyzed using the gene-specific primers and probes from TaqMan Gene Expression Assays System (Life Technologies). The PCR reactions were carried out in the StepOnePlusTM Real-Time PCR systems (Life Technologies) with the following amplification conditions: an initial activation and denaturation at 95°C for 10 min, followed by 40 cycles of denaturation at 95°C for 15 s, primer annealing and extension at 60°C for 1 min.

T-maze spontaneous alternation test

A modified protocol of Deacon and Rawlins (45) was used for the continuous spontaneous alternation task to examine hippocampal function. The maze was made of opaque acrylic (Plexiglas) with a sliding door separating the start arm (90 cm × 10 cm × 20 cm) from the rest of the apparatus, which comprised a choice point at the intersection of the start arm with the left and right arms of the maze (37 cm × 10 cm × 20 cm for each arm). A divider panel (20 cm × 20 cm) was centered at the intersection of the ‘T’ so that it extended 10 cm into the start arm. At the start of the test, a mouse was confined to the start arm for 5 min and then permitted free access to the rest of the maze for 10 min. The T-maze was cleaned with 10% ethanol between mice. A choice was recorded when the whole body of the mouse including its tail left the start arm and entered into one of the lateral arms. Entry into a lateral arm opposite to the one previously chosen was defined as an alternation. Alternation performance was defined as the percentage of times the mouse alternated between the lateral arms over the total number of possible alternations.

Contextual fear-conditioning test

The contextual fear-conditioning test was performed as described previously (9). In brief, each mouse was given 2 min to explore the test chamber (baseline activity). The floor was a grid of stainless steel rods connected to an electric shock generator. A video camera was mounted in the front of the chamber and a ceiling light illuminated the interior through the transparent ceiling. Delivery of a foot shock (1 mA scrambled) for 2 s was controlled by Video Freeze Software (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA). The mouse was allowed to stay in the chamber for an additional 30 s and then removed. Approximately 24 h later, each mouse was returned to the chamber and monitored for freezing behavior for 3 min, without any foot shock being delivered. Freezing behavior was recorded automatically by Video Freeze Software. Mean freezing activity during the contextual exposure was calculated as a measure of contextual learning.

Foot-shock sensitivity test

To facilitate accurate interpretation of the data from the fear-conditioning tests, we performed a foot-shock sensitivity test using the fear-conditioning test chamber. A foot shock was delivered every 10 s starting at 0.05 mA, with a 0.05 mA increment between each shock. The minimal level of current needed to elicit flinching or vocalizing was recorded.

Hippocampal slice preparation

The procedure for hippocampal slice preparation used in electrophysiological recordings was as described previously (12). Briefly, after being removed from the skulls, brains were placed in an ice-cold solution (pH 7.4) consisting of the following (in mm): sucrose 110, NaCl 60, KCl 3, NaH2PO4 1.25, NaHCO3 28, MgSO4 7, (+)-sodium-l-ascorbate 0.6, CaCl2 0.5 and glucose 5. The solution was continuously bubbled with oxygen gas (95% O2–5% CO2). The hippocampal formation was resected and sectioned into 350 µm transverse slices using a vibratome. After the slices were transferred to an interface-recording chamber (Automate Scientific, Inc., Berkeley, CA, USA), they were submerged in artificial cerebrospinal fluid, which comprised the following (in mm): NaCl 119, KCl 3, NaHCO3 26.2, MgSO4 1.3, NaH2PO4 1, CaCl2 2.5 and glucose 11. Incubation was carried out for at least 1 h at a constant temperature of 28 ± 1°C. The top surfaces of the slices were exposed to humidified oxygen gas (95% O2–5% CO2).

Electrophysiological recordings and induction of synaptic plasticity

Standard extracellular recording techniques were employed as described previously (12). In short, Schaffer collateral-commissural fibers were stimulated with a bipolar Teflon-coated tungsten electrode (Microprobes, Gaithersburg, MD, USA). fEPSPs were recorded using a glass microelectrode filled with 2 m NaCl positioned in the stratum radiatum of the CA1 hippocampal area. LTP was induced by TBS which includes 15 bursts of four pulses at 100 Hz and delivered at an interburst interval of 200 ms through the stimulating electrode. We performed electrophysiological recordings for 80 min on each brain slice, including 20 min of baseline recording and 60 min after the TBS induction. Signals from recording electrodes were amplified with Model 1800 microelectrode AC amplifier (A-M Systems, Carlsborg, WA, USA) and digitized at a 10 kHz sampling rate by an Axon Digidata 1400 A Data Acquisition System (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA, USA). Traces were obtained by pClamp 10.2 and analyzed using Clampfit 10.2 software (Molecular Devices).

Statistical analysis

Data from the T-maze spontaneous alternation test, contextual fear-conditioning test, foot-shock sensitivity test and hippocampal LTP from electrophysiological experiments were subjected to one-way analysis of variance between genotypes. In the behavioral phenotyping experiments, n indicates the number of mice. In the electrophysiological analysis, n indicates the number of slices analyzed. All values reported in the figures and tables are expressed as means ± SEM.

Funding

This study was supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01NS66072, R01HL91519, R21GM114645 and P30CA16056), Roswell Park Alliance Foundation, Louis Sklarow Memorial Fund, Jerome Lejeune Foundation, the Children's Guild Foundation and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (81100844).

Acknowledgement

The authors would like to thank Zhongyou Li for his assistance.

Conflict of Interest statement. None of the authors has any conflict of interest.

References

1

Antonarakis
S.E.
,
Lyle
R.
,
Dermitzakis
E.T.
,
Reymond
A.
,
Deutsch
S.
(
2004
)
Chromosome 21 and down syndrome: from genomics to pathophysiology
.
Nat. Rev. Genet.
,
5
,
725
738
.

2

Roizen
N.J.
,
Patterson
D.
(
2003
)
Down's syndrome
.
Lancet
,
361
,
1281
1289
.

3

Parker
S.E.
,
Mai
C.T.
,
Canfield
M.A.
,
Rickard
R.
,
Wang
Y.
,
Meyer
R.E.
,
Anderson
P.
,
Mason
C.A.
,
Collins
J.S.
,
Kirby
R.S.
et al. . (
2010
)
Updated National Birth Prevalence estimates for selected birth defects in the United States, 2004–2006
.
Birth Defects Res. A Clin. Mol. Teratol.
,
88
,
1008
1016
.

4

Khoshnood
B.
,
Greenlees
R.
,
Loane
M.
,
Dolk
H.
,
Committee
E.P.M.
,
Group
E.W.
(
2011
)
Paper 2: EUROCAT public health indicators for congenital anomalies in Europe
.
Birth Defects Res. A Clin. Mol. Teratol.
,
91
(Suppl. 1)
,
S16
S22
.

5

Natoli
J.L.
,
Ackerman
D.L.
,
McDermott
S.
,
Edwards
J.G.
(
2012
)
Prenatal diagnosis of Down syndrome: a systematic review of termination rates (1995–2011)
.
Prenat. Diagn.
,
32
,
142
153
.

6

Korenberg
J.R.
,
Chen
X.N.
,
Schipper
R.
,
Sun
Z.
,
Gonsky
R.
,
Gerwehr
S.
,
Carpenter
N.
,
Daumer
C.
,
Dignan
P.
,
Disteche
C.
(
1994
)
Down syndrome phenotypes: the consequences of chromosomal imbalance
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
,
91
,
4997
5001
.

7

Sinet
P.M.
,
Theophile
D.
,
Rahmani
Z.
,
Chettouh
Z.
,
Blouin
J.L.
,
Prieur
M.
,
Noel
B.
,
Delabar
J.M.
(
1994
)
Mapping of the Down syndrome phenotype on chromosome 21 at the molecular level
.
Biomed. Pharmacother.
,
48
,
247
252
.

8

Noble
J.
(
1998
)
Natural history of Down’s syndrome: a brief review for those involved in antenatal screening
.
J. Med. Screen.
,
5
,
172
177
.

9

Yu
T.
,
Liu
C.
,
Belichenko
P.
,
Clapcote
S.J.
,
Li
S.
,
Pao
A.
,
Kleschevnikov
A.
,
Bechard
A.R.
,
Asrar
S.
,
Chen
R.
et al. . (
2010
)
Effects of individual segmental trisomies of human chromosome 21 syntenic regions on hippocampal long-term potentiation and cognitive behaviors in mice
.
Brain Res.
,
1366
,
162
171
.

10

Belichenko
N.P.
,
Belichenko
P.V.
,
Kleschevnikov
A.M.
,
Salehi
A.
,
Reeves
R.H.
,
Mobley
W.C.
(
2009
)
The ‘Down syndrome critical region’ is sufficient in the mouse model to confer behavioral, neurophysiological, and synaptic phenotypes characteristic of Down syndrome
.
J. Neurosci.
,
29
,
5938
5948
.

11

Olson
L.E.
,
Roper
R.J.
,
Sengstaken
C.L.
,
Peterson
E.A.
,
Aquino
V.
,
Galdzicki
Z.
,
Siarey
R.
,
Pletnikov
M.
,
Moran
T.H.
,
Reeves
R.H.
(
2007
)
Trisomy for the Down syndrome ‘critical region’ is necessary but not sufficient for brain phenotypes of trisomic mice
.
Hum. Mol. Genet.
,
16
,
774
782
.

12

Zhang
L.
,
Meng
K.
,
Jiang
X.
,
Liu
C.
,
Pao
A.
,
Belichenko
P.V.
,
Kleschevnikov
A.M.
,
Josselyn
S.
,
Liang
P.
,
Ye
P.
et al. . (
2014
)
Human chromosome 21 orthologous region on mouse chromosome 17 is a major determinant of Down syndrome-related developmental cognitive deficits
.
Hum. Mol. Genet.
,
23
,
578
589
.

13

Li
Z.
,
Yu
T.
,
Morishima
M.
,
Pao
A.
,
LaDuca
J.
,
Conroy
J.
,
Nowak
N.
,
Matsui
S.
,
Shiraishi
I.
,
Yu
Y.
(
2007
)
Duplication of the entire 22.9-Mb human chromosome 21 syntenic region on mouse chromosome 16 causes cardiovascular and gastrointestinal abnormalities
.
Hum. Mol. Genet.
,
16
,
1359
1366
.

14

Bliss
T.V.
,
Collingridge
G.L.
(
1993
)
A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus
.
Nature
,
361
,
31
39
.

15

Malenka
R.C.
,
Nicoll
R.A.
(
1999
)
Long-term potentiation—a decade of progress?
Science
,
285
,
1870
1874
.

16

Costa
A.C.
,
Stasko
M.R.
,
Schmidt
C.
,
Davisson
M.T.
(
2010
)
Behavioral validation of the Ts65Dn mouse model for Down syndrome of a genetic background free of the retinal degeneration mutation Pde6b(rd1)
.
Behav. Brain Res.
,
206
,
52
62
.

17

Adams
D.J.
,
Biggs
P.J.
,
Cox
T.
,
Davies
R.
,
van der Weyden
L.
,
Jonkers
J.
,
Smith
J.
,
Plumb
B.
,
Taylor
R.
,
Nishijima
I.
et al. . (
2004
)
Mutagenic insertion and chromosome engineering resource (MICER)
.
Nat. Genet.
,
36
,
867
871
.

18

Robertson
E.
(
1987
) In
Robertson
E.
(ed.),
Teratocarcinomas and Embryonic Stem Cells
A Practical Approach
.
IRL Press
,
Oxford
, pp.
71
112
.

19

Yu
Y.E.
,
Morishima
M.
,
Pao
A.
,
Wang
D.Y.
,
Wen
X.Y.
,
Baldini
A.
,
Bradley
A.
(
2006
)
A deficiency in the region homologous to human 17q21.33–q23.2 causes heart defects in mice
.
Genetics
,
173
,
297
307
.

20

Fernandez-Martinez
J.
,
Vela
E.M.
,
Tora-Ponsioen
M.
,
Ocana
O.H.
,
Nieto
M.A.
,
Galceran
J.
(
2009
)
Attenuation of Notch signalling by the Down-syndrome-associated kinase DYRK1A
.
J. Cell Sci.
,
122
,
1574
1583
.

21

Hammerle
B.
,
Ulin
E.
,
Guimera
J.
,
Becker
W.
,
Guillemot
F.
,
Tejedor
F.J.
(
2011
)
Transient expression of Mnb/Dyrk1a couples cell cycle exit and differentiation of neuronal precursors by inducing p27KIP1 expression and suppressing NOTCH signaling
.
Development
,
138
,
2543
2554
.

22

Kelly
P.A.
,
Rahmani
Z.
(
2005
)
DYRK1A enhances the mitogen-activated protein kinase cascade in PC12 cells by forming a complex with Ras, B-Raf, and MEK1
.
Mol. Biol. Cell
,
16
,
3562
3573
.

23

Mark
M.D.
,
Herlitze
S.
(
2000
)
G-protein mediated gating of inward-rectifier K+ channels
.
Eur. J. Biochem.
,
267
,
5830
5836
.

24

Yamada
M.
,
Inanobe
A.
,
Kurachi
Y.
(
1998
)
G protein regulation of potassium ion channels
.
Pharmacol. Rev.
,
50
,
723
760
.

25

Fotaki
V.
,
Dierssen
M.
,
Alcantara
S.
,
Martinez
S.
,
Marti
E.
,
Casas
C.
,
Visa
J.
,
Soriano
E.
,
Estivill
X.
,
Arbones
M.L.
(
2002
)
Dyrk1A haploinsufficiency affects viability and causes developmental delay and abnormal brain morphology in mice
.
Mol. Cell. Biol.
,
22
,
6636
6647
.

26

Garcia-Cerro
S.
,
Martinez
P.
,
Vidal
V.
,
Corrales
A.
,
Florez
J.
,
Vidal
R.
,
Rueda
N.
,
Arbones
M.L.
,
Martinez-Cue
C.
(
2014
)
Overexpression of Dyrk1A is implicated in several cognitive, electrophysiological and neuromorphological alterations found in a mouse model of Down syndrome
.
PLoS ONE
,
9
,
e106572
.

27

Vidal
V.
,
Garcia
S.
,
Martinez
P.
,
Corrales
A.
,
Florez
J.
,
Rueda
N.
,
Sharma
A.
,
Martinez-Cue
C.
(
2012
)
Lack of behavioral and cognitive effects of chronic ethosuximide and gabapentin treatment in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome
.
Neuroscience
,
220
,
158
168
.

28

Kahlem
P.
,
Sultan
M.
,
Herwig
R.
,
Steinfath
M.
,
Balzereit
D.
,
Eppens
B.
,
Saran
N.G.
,
Pletcher
M.T.
,
South
S.T.
,
Stetten
G.
et al. . (
2004
)
Transcript level alterations reflect gene dosage effects across multiple tissues in a mouse model of Down syndrome
.
Genome Res.
,
14
,
1258
1267
.

29

Lyle
R.
,
Gehrig
C.
,
Neergaard-Henrichsen
C.
,
Deutsch
S.
,
Antonarakis
S.E.
(
2004
)
Gene expression from the aneuploid chromosome in a trisomy mouse model of Down syndrome
.
Genome Res.
,
14
,
1268
1274
.

30

Barlow
G.M.
,
Micales
B.
,
Lyons
G.E.
,
Korenberg
J.R.
(
2001
)
Down syndrome cell adhesion molecule is conserved in mouse and highly expressed in the adult mouse brain
.
Cytogenet. Cell Genet.
,
94
,
155
162
.

31

Yamakawa
K.
,
Huot
Y.K.
,
Haendelt
M.A.
,
Hubert
R.
,
Chen
X.N.
,
Lyons
G.E.
,
Korenberg
J.R.
(
1998
)
DSCAM: a novel member of the immunoglobulin superfamily maps in a Down syndrome region and is involved in the development of the nervous system
.
Hum. Mol. Genet.
,
7
,
227
237
.

32

Wolvetang
E.J.
,
Wilson
T.J.
,
Sanij
E.
,
Busciglio
J.
,
Hatzistavrou
T.
,
Seth
A.
,
Hertzog
P.J.
,
Kola
I.
(
2003
)
ETS2 overexpression in transgenic models and in Down syndrome predisposes to apoptosis via the p53 pathway
.
Hum. Mol. Genet.
,
12
,
247
255
.

33

Abuhatzira
L.
,
Shamir
A.
,
Schones
D.E.
,
Schaffer
A.A.
,
Bustin
M.
(
2011
)
The chromatin-binding protein HMGN1 regulates the expression of methyl CpG-binding protein 2 (MECP2) and affects the behavior of mice
.
J. Biol. Chem.
,
286
,
42051
42062
.

34

Egeo
A.
,
Mazzocco
M.
,
Sotgia
F.
,
Arrigo
P.
,
Oliva
R.
,
Bergonon
S.
,
Nizetic
D.
,
Rasore-Quartino
A.
,
Scartezzini
P.
(
1998
)
Identification and characterization of a new human cDNA from chromosome 21q22.3 encoding a basic nuclear protein
.
Hum. Genet.
,
102
,
289
293
.

35

Ling
K.H.
,
Hewitt
C.A.
,
Tan
K.L.
,
Cheah
P.S.
,
Vidyadaran
S.
,
Lai
M.I.
,
Lee
H.C.
,
Simpson
K.
,
Hyde
L.
,
Pritchard
M.A.
et al. . (
2014
)
Functional transcriptome analysis of the postnatal brain of the Ts1Cje mouse model for Down syndrome reveals global disruption of interferon-related molecular networks
.
BMC Genomics
,
15
,
624
.

36

Renelt
M.
,
von Bohlen und Halbach
V.
,
von Bohlen und Halbach
O.
(
2014
)
Distribution of PCP4 protein in the forebrain of adult mice
.
Acta Histochem.
,
116
,
1056
1061
.

37

Song
H.J.
,
Park
J.
,
Seo
S.R.
,
Kim
J.
,
Paik
S.R.
,
Chung
K.C.
(
2008
)
Down syndrome critical region 2 protein inhibits the transcriptional activity of peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor beta in HEK293 cells
.
Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun.
,
376
,
478
482
.

38

Vidal-Taboada
J.M.
,
Lu
A.
,
Pique
M.
,
Pons
G.
,
Gil
J.
,
Oliva
R.
(
2000
)
Down syndrome critical region gene 2: expression during mouse development and in human cell lines indicates a function related to cell proliferation
.
Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun.
,
272
,
156
163
.

39

Yu
Y.
,
Bradley
A.
(
2001
)
Engineering chromosomal rearrangements in mice
.
Nat. Rev. Genet.
,
2
,
780
790
.

40

Bradley
A.
,
Zheng
B.
,
Liu
P.
(
1998
)
Thirteen years of manipulating the mouse genome: a personal history
.
Int. J. Dev. Biol.
,
42
,
943
950
.

41

Ramirez-Solis
R.
,
Davis
A.C.
,
Bradley
A.
(
1993
)
Gene targeting in embryonic stem cells
.
Methods Enzymol.
,
225
,
855
878
.

42

O'Gorman
S.
,
Dagenais
N.A.
,
Qian
M.
,
Marchuk
Y.
(
1997
)
Protamine-Cre recombinase transgenes efficiently recombine target sequences in the male germ line of mice, but not in embryonic stem cells
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
,
94
,
14602
14607
.

43

Bradley
A.
(
1987
) In
Robertson
E.
(ed.),
Teratocarcinomas and Embryonic Stem Cells—A Practical Approach
.
IRL Press
,
Oxford
, pp.
113
151
.

44

Olson
L.E.
,
Richtsmeier
J.T.
,
Leszl
J.
,
Reeves
R.H.
(
2004
)
A chromosome 21 critical region does not cause specific Down syndrome phenotypes
.
Science
,
306
,
687
690
.

45

Deacon
R.M.
,
Rawlins
J.N.
(
2006
)
T-maze alternation in the rodent
.
Nat. Protoc.
,
1
,
7
12
.

Author notes

The authors wish it to be known that, in their opinion, the first four authors should be regarded as joint First Authors.

W.C.M. and Y.E.Y. contributed equally to this paper.

Supplementary data