Abstract

Silver Russell Syndrome (SRS) syndrome is an imprinting disorder involving low birth weight with complex genetics and diagnostics. Some rare SRS patients carry maternally inherited microduplications spanning the imprinted genes CDKN1C, PHLDA2, SLC22A18 and KCNQ1, suggesting that overexpression of one of more of these genes contributes to the SRS phenotype. While this molecular alteration is very rare, feeding difficulties are a very common feature of this condition. Given that SRS children also have very low body mass index, understanding the underpinning biology of the eating disorder is important, as well as potential co-occurring behavioural alterations. Here, we report that a mouse model of this microduplication exhibits a number of behavioural deficits. The mice had a blunted perception of the palatability of a given foodstuff. This perception may underpin the fussiness with food. We additionally report hypoactivity, unrelated to anxiety or motoric function, and a deficit in the appropriate integration of incoming sensory information. Importantly, using a second genetic model, we were able to attribute all altered behaviours to elevated expression of a single gene, Cdkn1c. This is the first report linking elevated Cdkn1c to altered behaviour in mice. Importantly, the findings from our study may have relevance for SRS and highlight a potentially underreported aspect of this disorder.

Introduction

Silver Russell syndrome (SRS) is a very rare genetic disorder with an approximate frequency of 1 in 300,000 (OMIM #180860). The key characteristics that define SRS are foetal growth restriction and failure to thrive postnatally, resulting in a lifelong deficit in height and weight (1–4). While growth restriction is a relatively common generic characteristic that can occur in a number of disorders, SRS patients can be further characterised by some or all of the following: a normal head circumference, which appears large for the body, a large protruding forehead with a triangular-shaped face, clinodactyly, undergrowth of one side of the body (hemihypotrophy), fasting hypoglycaemia, night sweats and excessive thinness which persists into adulthood. A recent clinical scoring system has been proposed for SRS that encompasses a number of these symptoms (4).

The cognitive and behavioural characteristics of SRS are not clear. Some studies indicate normal intelligence and cognitive ability (5–8) while others suggest specific learning difficulties (9–12). There are reports of hyperactivity, emotional problems, conduct problems as well as peer problems with similarities to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and social communication difficulties with akin to Autism Spectrum Disorders. Although some studies suggest that behavioural problems involving hyperactivity are relatively uncommon (2), one of the first descriptions of SRS reported hyperactivity (13) and one study has reported issues with maintaining attention (11). A more common behavioural observation in SRS centres on feeding issues. Parents of SRS children often express concerns about their child’s eating habits reporting poor appetite, fussy eating and the struggle to get their SRS child to gain weight. Studies reporting poor appetites and/or eating a limited variety of food and/or having issues with food textures reported frequencies of 30% (14), 60% (15), 67% (16) and 82% (17). A recent study using the Netchine-Harbison clinical scoring system reported 100% prevalence of feeding difficulties and/or BMI  < −2SDS in children with a defined SRS (epi)mutation and 92.3% of children meeting the criteria for SRS without a defined (epi)mutations (18) indicating major issues around healthy eating and weight gain.

While some researchers consider SRS a single condition, there are a wide-range of both genetic and epigenetic mutations reported in these patients which may account for differences in their presentation, as suggested in the recent clinical diagnostic scoring system (4). Approximately 70% of patients have alterations affecting human chromosome 7 or 11 while the remaining 30% are of unknown origin (4). Both chromosomes 7 and 11 contain imprinted genes, which are uniquely expressed from either the paternal or maternal chromosome as a consequence of epigenetic marks set up in the male or female germline (19). Some rare SRS patients carry maternal microduplications of 11p15 encompassing CYCLIN DEPENDENT KINASE INHIBITOR 1C (CDKN1C), POTASSIUM CHANNEL, VOLTAGE GATED KQT-LIKE SUBFAMILY Q, MEMBER 1 (KCNQ1), PLECKSTRIN HOMOLOGY-LIKE DOMAIN, FAMILY A, MEMBER 2 (PHLDA2) and SOLUTE CARRIER FAMILY 22, MEMBER 18 (SLC22A18) (20–22). CDKN1C is a maternally expressed gene (23,24) and these SRS patients are predicted to have twice the normal level of CDKN1C expression (25,26). Mutations in the CDKN1C coding region have been observed in some patients with an IMAGe syndrome (MIM 614732), another complex growth restriction disorder whose early clinical features, including IUGR, overlap with that of SRS (27,28). In contrast to the functionally inactivating mutations reported in the congenital overgrowth disorder Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome (BWS, MIM 130650) (29–33), studies suggest increased protein stability in IMAGe syndrome (28,34) consistent with a role for CDKN1C in growth restriction (35).

Loss of Cdkn1c has been extensively studied in mouse models, which have highlighted in a role in a number of key features of BWS, including foetal overgrowth and disrupted placental development (36–40). We have recently reported on the phenotype of a mouse, which models the maternal microduplications on 11p15 that are reported in some cases of SRS (41). These mice carry a bacterial artificial chromosome (BAC) transgene providing extra active copies of Cdkn1c, Slc22a18 and Phlda2 (Cdkn1cBACx1), are growth restricted in utero and remain small throughout their lives (39). In addition, these mice show neonatal hypoglycaemia, head-sparing, a significant loss of white adipose tissue all of which are reported in SRS (41). These mice also possess larger brown adipose tissue depots (41), a phenotype not yet explored in SRS. We were able to attribute all these phenotypes to the just two-fold elevated expression of Cdkn1c using a second BAC line of mice that carries exactly the same transgene, but with the transgenic expression of Cdkn1c replaced by a β-galactosidase reporter gene (Cdkn1cBACLacZ) (42). Phenotypes present in the Cdkn1cBACx1 transgenic line and absent in the Cdkn1cBACLacZ transgenic line were due to the increased expression of Cdkn1c. The presence of some defining SRS features in our unique model supported a role for elevated CDKN1C in SRS (41). As we describe here, this model also provides a unique and timely opportunity to assess behaviour in an SRS model, and more specifically in relation to elevated Cdkn1c. We found that Cdkn1cBACx1 animals were hypoactive, had impaired sensorimotor gating and a blunted hedonic response to a palatable solution. Finally, all deficits were rescued by ablation of the additional copy of Cdkn1c strengthening a causal relationship between dosage of this gene and potential behavioural abnormalities in SRS.

Results

Cdkn1cBACx1 animals display hypoactivity unrelated to motor dysfunction

In light of reports of hyperactivity in some SRS patients, we first assessed basic motor function in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals, both in terms of general activity levels and motoric competence. We assayed basal activity for 2-hours on three consecutive days using a standardised general measure where movement is detected by consecutive breaks of two infrared beams (‘run’) (43). Cdkn1cBACx1 mice were hypoactive relative to their WT littermates, overall making nearly half as many runs in any given session (main effect of GENOTYPE: F1,47 =8.49, P = 0.005) (Fig. 1A and B). This hypoactive phenotype was consistent and not related to a differential reactivity to novelty as, although all animals showed the expected reduction in activity during the first session (Fig. 1A; main effect of BIN, F23,1081 =38.6, P < 0.001) and across the three consecutive days of testing (Fig. 1B; main effect of DAY, F2,94 =14.6, P < 0.001), there was no interaction with GENOTYPE (BIN*GENOTYPE, F23,1081 =0.61, P = 0.92; DAY*GENOTYPE F2,94 =0.842, P = 0.43).
Cdkn1cBACx1 animals display hypoactivity unrelated to any motoric function deficits. (A) Locomotor activity data on the first day of testing broken down into 5-minute bins shows consistent reduced activity of Cdkn1cBACx1 mice relative to WT littermates throughout the session. (B) Cdkn1cBACx1 also displayed consistent hypoactivity across three consecutive test days. (C) All animals showed the expected increase in activity after amphetamine administration. (D) Reduced activity in Cdkn1cBACx1 mice was not related to motoric function deficits as all animals increase time before first fall following successive rotatod training session. (E) Similarly, there was no effect of genotype on latency to fall from an accelerating rotarod across a range of test speeds. Data shown is ± SEM. Main effect of GENOTYPE, *= P < 0.05, **= P < 0.01; main effect of within subject factors (BIN, SESSION, DOSE, SPEED) #= P < 0.05, ###= P < 0.001.
Figure 1.

Cdkn1cBACx1 animals display hypoactivity unrelated to any motoric function deficits. (A) Locomotor activity data on the first day of testing broken down into 5-minute bins shows consistent reduced activity of Cdkn1cBACx1 mice relative to WT littermates throughout the session. (B) Cdkn1cBACx1 also displayed consistent hypoactivity across three consecutive test days. (C) All animals showed the expected increase in activity after amphetamine administration. (D) Reduced activity in Cdkn1cBACx1 mice was not related to motoric function deficits as all animals increase time before first fall following successive rotatod training session. (E) Similarly, there was no effect of genotype on latency to fall from an accelerating rotarod across a range of test speeds. Data shown is ± SEM. Main effect of GENOTYPE, *= P < 0.05, **= P < 0.01; main effect of within subject factors (BIN, SESSION, DOSE, SPEED) #= P < 0.05, ###= P < 0.001.

The hypoactivity of Cdkn1cBACx1 mice was not related to a general impaired motoric function. Firstly, all animals showed the expected increase in number of runs in response to amphetamine (main effect of DRUG: F1,47 =5.9, P = 0.019), and this response was not different by genotype (DRUG*GENOTYPE interaction: F1,47 =0.01, P = 0.94) (Fig. 1C). Secondly, explicit measures of motoric function on the rotarod implied no general motor deficits. Animals were trained to walk on a rotating rod that accelerated across the session duration. All animals learned the task, as indicated by an increase in latency to first fall across five training sessions (main effect of SESSION, F1,27 =29.1, P < 0.001), but there was no main effect of genotype in latency to first fall across the training sessions (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,27 =1.92, P = 0.18) and no SESSION*GENOTYPE interaction (F4,108 =1.81, P = 0.13). (Fig. 1D). Moreover, during accelerating rotarod probe tests following training, there was no effect of GENOTYPE on latency to fall across five test rotation speeds (main effect of GENOTYPE: F1,27 =1.47, P = 0.24; Fig. 1E).

Emotional reactivity is not altered in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals

Emotional reactivity was assessed using the elevated plus maze test (EPM) and the open field (OF) test. In the EPM, all animals showed the expected pattern of behaviour, spending less time on the anxiogenic open arm than other areas of the maze (middle section, closed arm). Although Cdkn1cBACx1 animals spent 35% less time on the open arm of the maze on average than their WT littermates (Fig. 2A), this failed to reach significance (t(53)=1.73, P = 0.09). A lack of an altered emotional reactivity between Cdkn1cBACx1 and WT animals on the EPM test was further underlined by no effect of GENOTYPE on number of entries to the open arms (t(53)=1.49, P = 0.14) (Fig. 2B) and latency to enter open arms (t(53)=-0.43, P = 0.67) (Fig. 2C).
Emotional reactivity was intact in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals. There were no differences between Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and their WT littermates in measures on the EPM test, including (A) time spent on open arm, (B) entries into open arm and (C) latency to first enter open arm. Similarly, there were no differences between Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and their WT littermates in measures on the OF test, including (D) time spent in centre zone, (E) entries into centre zone, and (F) latency to first enter centre zone. Data shown is ± SEM.
Figure 2.

Emotional reactivity was intact in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals. There were no differences between Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and their WT littermates in measures on the EPM test, including (A) time spent on open arm, (B) entries into open arm and (C) latency to first enter open arm. Similarly, there were no differences between Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and their WT littermates in measures on the OF test, including (D) time spent in centre zone, (E) entries into centre zone, and (F) latency to first enter centre zone. Data shown is ± SEM.

Similarly, the OF test revealed no significant difference between Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and their WT littermates in the time spent in the anxiogenic inner zone of the arena (t(53)=0.59, P = 0.56; Fig. 2D). Again, there was no effect of genotype on number of entries to the inner zone (t(53)=1.41, P = 0.16; Fig. 2E) nor in latency to enter open arms (t(53)=0.23, P = 0.82; Fig. 2F). These data, from two separate tests, indicate that in emotional reactivity and anxiety behaviours do not differ between Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and their WT littermates. Consequently, the observed hypoactivity in Cdkn1cBACx1 was not explained by a difference in the state of anxiety.

Cdkn1cBACx1 animals have sensorimotor gating deficits

Impairments in sensorimotor gating have been described in association with a number of neurodevelopmental disorders including schizophrenia (44) and autism spectrum disorders (45). As like many other studies of mouse models of neuropsychiatric (46) and neurodevelopmental disorders (47), we measured the acoustic startle response (ASR) and the prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the ASR to infer changes in sensorimotor gating here. There was no difference in ASR at 105dB between Cdkn1cBACx1 and WT littermates (Fig. 3A; t(29)=1.22, P = 0.23). However, PPI of the ASR was attenuated by almost half in Cdkn1cBACx1 relative to WT littermates (Fig. 3B; main effect of GENOTYPE: F1,29 =6.264, P = 0.018). This reduction in PPI in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals followed the expected pattern of further inhibition with increasing prepulse (main effect of PRE-PULSE: F2,58 =22.926, P < 0.001), and was consistent across the range of pre-pulses used (PREPULSE*GENOTYPE F2,58 =1.244, P = 0.296). This pattern of change with increasing prepulse indicate that Cdkn1cBACx1 animals are unlikely have a general hearing impairment but that, taken together, these data suggest an impaired sensorimotor gating response relative to WT littermates.
Elevated Cdkn1c expression results in sensorimotor gating deficits. (A) Average ASR to a 105 dB pulse was not affected by genotype (B). However, PPI of the startle response was significantly blunted in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals. Data shown is ± SEM. Main effect of GENOTYPE, *= P < 0.05; main effect of PPI, ##= P < 0.01.
Figure 3.

Elevated Cdkn1c expression results in sensorimotor gating deficits. (A) Average ASR to a 105 dB pulse was not affected by genotype (B). However, PPI of the startle response was significantly blunted in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals. Data shown is ± SEM. Main effect of GENOTYPE, *= P < 0.05; main effect of PPI, ##= P < 0.01.

Impaired perceived palatability in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals

Parents of children with SRS often report feeding difficulties with the term "fussy eaters" commonly used to describe this behaviour (15). We previously reported no difference in the consumption of standard chow by Cdkn1cBACx1 mice (41). In a standard two-bottle preference test, we extend this finding to palatable foods (8% sucrose), showing that both Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and their WT littermates preferred sucrose over water (Z = 82, P = 0.01) (Fig. 4A), but with no effect of genotype (U = 75, P = 0.2). However, measures of consumption do not directly provide an indication of the hedonic response to food. To address this, we made use of lick cluster size analysis (LCA). Rodent consumption of a freely available solution is a highly stereotyped behaviour with mice and rats licking in clusters with regular intercluster intervals. LCA can be used as a proxy measure of the perceived palatability of that solution where the average lick cluster size made in response to a palatable solution is related to the hedonic properties of that solution (48). As expected, as the concentration of the sucrose solution increased, there was an overall increase in the average lick cluster size for both groups (Fig. 4B; main effect of CONCENTRATION: F3,162 =27.1, P < 0.001). However, the Cdkn1cBACx1 mice had a smaller average lick cluster sizes compared to their WT littermates (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,54 =5.11, P = 0.028), which was steady across all the four sucrose concentrations assessed (CONCENTRATION*GENOTYPE, F3,162 =1.347, P = 0.26). There was no significant difference between Cdkn1cBACx1 males and their wt littermates in total number licks (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,54 =1.281, P = 0.26) or in total number of bouts (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,54 =0.008, P = 0.93). Consistent with other measures of feeding, there was no significant difference overall consumption of the solution (Fig. 4C; main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,54 =1.515, P = 0.224), which excludes an altered satiety threshold underlying the decreased lick cluster size. Critically, there was no effect of genotype on the inter lick interval (ILI; main effect of GENOTYPE: F1,54 =1.16, P = 0.29) (data not shown). ILI is a measure of the mechanical task of the performance. As the ILI was the same in both genotypes, this excludes the difference in licking being as a result of a physical incapacity. Together, these data suggest that the Cdkn1cBACx1 mice perceived a lower palatability of a sucrose solution compared to WT littermates.
Cdkn1cBACx1 animals show a reduced perceived palatability of a sucrose solution. (A) All animals preferred sucrose to water and this was not affected by genotype. (B) Cdkn1cBACx1 animals had consistently smaller average lick cluster size compared to WT indicating altered hedonic processing. (C) Despite potentially reduced perceived palatability, consumption of a palatable solution was not different between Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and WT. (D) Cdkn1cBACx1 had significantly more Th-positive cells that wt in the periventricular hypothalamus (PVH), but not the A13 dopaminergic cell group. (E) Representative images showing Th staining. Scale bar is 100 μm. Data shown is ± SEM. Main effect of GENOTYPE, *=P ≤ 0.05; main effect of concentration ##= P < 0.01.
Figure 4.

Cdkn1cBACx1 animals show a reduced perceived palatability of a sucrose solution. (A) All animals preferred sucrose to water and this was not affected by genotype. (B) Cdkn1cBACx1 animals had consistently smaller average lick cluster size compared to WT indicating altered hedonic processing. (C) Despite potentially reduced perceived palatability, consumption of a palatable solution was not different between Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and WT. (D) Cdkn1cBACx1 had significantly more Th-positive cells that wt in the periventricular hypothalamus (PVH), but not the A13 dopaminergic cell group. (E) Representative images showing Th staining. Scale bar is 100 μm. Data shown is ± SEM. Main effect of GENOTYPE, *=P ≤ 0.05; main effect of concentration ##= P < 0.01.

To ask whether the effect of Cdkn1c on feeding was associated with alterations in the brain related to feeding and dopamine-signalling, associated with food reward processing in the brain (49), we examined two regions of the hypothalamus involved in the regulation of food intake, the A13 dopaminergic cell group in the zona incerta and periventricular hypothalamus (50–52). Expression of tyrosine hydroxylase (Th), an enzyme involved in the synthesis of dopamine and noradrenaline, was used as a marker of these cell types. There was a significant increase in tyrosine hydroxylase (Th)-positive cells, in the periventricular hypothalamus (t(8)= −2.29, P = 0.05) of Cdkn1cBACx1 with no significant change in the A13 dopaminergic cell group (t(8)=−1.48, P = 0.18) (Fig. 4D). This altered neurobiology could contribute to the changes in hedonic behaviour.

Behavioural deficits in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals are directly attributable to increased Cdkn1c dosage

The Cdkn1cBACx1 mouse models the rare 11p15 microduplication seen in a small number of SRS patients (20–22). In this mouse model, as in these rare SRS patients, more than one gene is present within the duplicated region. We wanted to delineate the contribution of these genes, in particular focusing on one, namely Cdkn1c, which has a well described role in brain function (53,54). To isolate the contribution of elevated Cdkn1c expression to the behavioural phenotypes, we utilized an animal model carrying a single copy of the transgene spanning Cdkn1c, but with the transgenic Cdkn1c copy ablated. In this model, Cdkn1c is expressed at wild type levels (39,41). Critically, none of the behavioural differences found in Cdkn1cBACx1 were replicated in this model. Specifically, there was no difference in activity between Cdkn1cBACLacZ animals and their WT littermates (Fig. 5A; main effect of GENOTYPE: F1,28 =0.46, P = 0.52). There was no difference in time spent in (t(54)=-0.14, P = 0.89) or frequency of entries to (t(54)=1.15, P = 0.25) the open arms of an EPM (Fig. 5B and C), with convergent findings from the OF test, with no difference in time spent in (t(54)=0.24, P = 0.81) or frequency of entries to (t(54)=0.18, P = 0.86) the internal zone (Fig. 5D and E). Sensorimotor gating deficits observed in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals were normal in Cdkn1cBACLacZ animals with no difference in ASR at 105 dB (Fig. 5F; t(29)= 0.58, P = 0.56) and PPI (Fig. 5G; main effect of GENOTYPE: F1,28 =2.97, P = 0.10). Finally, perceived palatability of a sucrose solution, as measured by LCA, was also equivalent between Cdkn1cBACLacZ animals and their WT littermates (Fig. 5H; main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,51 =2.9, P = 0.1) and there was no difference in consumption within the LCA test (Fig. 5I; F1,51 =2.46, P = 0.12).
Behavioural alterations in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals were absent when Cdkn1c expression was not elevated. (A) There was no difference between Cdkn1cBACLacZ animals and their WT littermates in LMA. (B–E) Duration of time spent in and number of entries into an anxiogenic zone was not affected by genotype in an (B,C) EPM or (D,E) OF. (F,G) ASR and PPI was not different between Cdkn1cBACLacZ animals and WT littermates. Average lick cluster size (H) was equivalent between groups, as was consumption (I) of a palatable sucrose solution. Data shown is ± SEM. Main effects of within subject factors (PPI, Concentration) ###= P < 0.001.
Figure 5.

Behavioural alterations in Cdkn1cBACx1 animals were absent when Cdkn1c expression was not elevated. (A) There was no difference between Cdkn1cBACLacZ animals and their WT littermates in LMA. (B–E) Duration of time spent in and number of entries into an anxiogenic zone was not affected by genotype in an (B,C) EPM or (D,E) OF. (F,G) ASR and PPI was not different between Cdkn1cBACLacZ animals and WT littermates. Average lick cluster size (H) was equivalent between groups, as was consumption (I) of a palatable sucrose solution. Data shown is ± SEM. Main effects of within subject factors (PPI, Concentration) ###= P < 0.001.

These data demonstrate that the behavioural abnormalities observed in the Cdkn1cBACx1 mouse model are attributable specifically to the elevated expression of Cdkn1c.

Discussion

In these experiments we took advantage of a transgenic mouse line that effectively models a rare molecular subtype of patients diagnosed with SRS. These mice carry a single extra copy of a transgene spanning three imprinted genes Cdkn1c, Phlda2 and Slc22a18 with the minimal duplicated region in SRS spanning one additional gene, Kcnq1. We used this model to assess relevant phenotypes concerning activity, emotional reactivity, sensorimotor gating and consumption and response to palatable food. We found selective effects, with the model being generally hypoactive with no obvious deficits in motoric ability, reduced PPI of an ASR and a reduced perceived palatability to sucrose. The extent to which discrete aspects of behaviour could be related to specific gene effects was assessed using a second model in which only Phlda2 and Slc22a18 were elevated. The absence of alterations in these specific behaviours in this second model identifies elevated Cdkn1c as the causal mutation. In addition to demonstrating the behavioural consequences of increased Cdkn1c expression, these data suggest expanding the clinical phenotype assessment of SRS to include more neurological and behavioural measures.

Increased activity has been reported in some patients with SRS, while other studies fail to report an association. Our data on the Cdkn1cBACx1 model indicated reduced, rather than increased, baseline activity. Although Cdkn1cBACx1 mice showed the normal reduction in activity over time as they habituated to the apparatus, their general hypoactivity was consistent throughout the first session and across the three consecutive daily sessions. This suggests that this locomotor activity phenotype was not related to any altered reactivity to novelty or increased anxiety. This idea was supported by data from explicit tests of emotional reactivity, the elevated plus maze and open field, where there were no differences in behaviour between Cdkn1cBACx1 mice and their WT counterparts. Furthermore, the reduced activity was not due to a physical impairment, as Cdkn1cBACx1 mice increased their locomotor activity in a similar pattern to WT after acute amphetamine administration and also showed no difference in motor learning and skill on the rotarod test. Reports of hyperactivity in individuals with SRS are infrequent, however, why there should be a discrepancy between reports of hyperactivity in some SRS patients and robust evidence of hypoactivity in the Cdkn1cBACx1 SRS mouse model is not clear. This may relate to reporting (as opposed to explicit measures of activity) and/or whether the genetic sub-types of SRS have been taken into account.

Sensorimotor gating, as measured here by PPI of the ASR, provides a measure of appropriate integration of sensory cues with a motor output. Although we found no difference in the response to an acoustic startle, Cdkn1cBACx1 mice did show a reduced PPI. Cdkn1cBACx1 mice did show the expected increase in PPI of startle in response to an increasingly loud pre-pulse tone, but there was a general reduction in the degree of inhibition. This suggests the reduced PPI is not due to hearing problems in the Cdkn1cBACx1 mice, but is more likely to be related to specific deficits in sensorimotor gating. This pattern of abnormalities is seen in a number of psychiatric disorders (44) and also autism spectrum disorders (45). Although the incidence of psychiatric illness in SRS is very rare (55), there are a growing number of studies beginning to link SRS with neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism spectrum disorders (56).

One of the more widely reported behaviours in SRS relates to difficulties with food; fussiness with regards to food, and children who are simply not interested in food. Since a key complication in SRS is the failure to gain weight and extreme skinniness, it is important to understand the biology underpinning this behaviour. We previously reported that Cdkn1cBACx1 mice possess more energy consuming brown adipose tissue but consumed the same amount of standard chow as their littermates over a 7-day measurement (41). This could account for the low BMI reported in SRS but would not explain fussy eating. Here, we show that consumption of a palatable sucrose solution was equivalent between Cdkn1cBACx1 and WT littermates. However, consumption does not provide a measurement of the hedonic response to food. In order to test this, we made use of LCA, a behavioural measure of perceived palatability (48) used previously to examine rodent models of psychiatric disorders (57) and the imprinting disorder, Prader Willi Syndrome (58). Rodents rarely show continuous consumption of a liquid. Instead they perform repeated clusters of licks separated by pauses. The number of licks in each cluster (cluster size) has a positive, monotonic relationship with the concentration of a palatable solution such as sucrose (59). Importantly, cluster size is not simply a reflection of the amount consumed. For example, although the cluster size increases with increased sucrose concentration, the amount consumed varies in an inverted U-shaped function. In addition, studies of conditioning have also shown that cluster size measures of hedonic reactions and consumption measures can dissociate (60). Therefore, the cluster size is a sensitive measure of the palatability of a solution. Here we show that, although Cdkn1cBACx1 mice consumed the same volume of a sucrose solution, they did so in smaller lick clusters. This indicates that perceived palatability in Cdkn1cBACx1 mice is reduced relative to WT littermates; for instance Cdkn1cBACx1 mice have a similar lick cluster size at 8% sucrose as WT mice at 4% sucrose (Fig. 4B). This would suggest perturbed food reward processing in the Cdkn1cBACx1 mice, with effects on the perceived hedonic value of food. In human studies, it has been demonstrated that there is a positive relationship between food palatability and amount consumed (61). In human SRS patients, it is easy to imagine how a lowered hedonic response may translate into fussiness or a disinterest in food and, consequently, reduced food intake.

One of the complexities of studying any phenotype associated with an imprinting disorder, such as SRS, is the potential involvement of multiple genes. As previously (41), we were able to address this issue using a separate control line of transgenic mice carrying the same BAC transgene, but without elevated expression of Cdkn1c (Cdkn1cBACLacZ). The behaviour of the Cdkn1cBACLacZ mice was equivalent to WT in all of the behavioural tests described here. These data implicate over-expression of Cdkn1c as the critical molecular factor underlying the hypoactivity, reduced PPI and lowered hedonic response to sucrose seen in the SRS model, the Cdkn1cBACx1 mice. This is perhaps unsurprising, given the minimal brain expression of the other BAC genes, Phlda2 and Slc22a18. In contrast, Cdkn1c has a key role in the developing nervous system through the regulation of cell cycle exit, differentiation and migration of embryonic cerebral cortical precursors (54), with loss-of-function resulting in an increased thickness of layer six of the cortex during development (62). In addition to early development, Cdkn1c has also been shown to be key to the regeneration of the adult brain through regulating adult neural stem cell quiescence (63). The functional relevance of this role in adult neural stem cells has not been explored in relation to behaviour but there may be further influences on behaviour as the mice age or under specific conditions where appropriate neurogenesis is required e.g. learning and memory (64). However, possibly most pertinent to the behaviours outlined here is the role of Cdkn1c in the developing dopaminergic system. For instance, Cdkn1c co-expression with Nurr1 promotes maturation of a midbrain dopaminergic neuronal cell line while loss-of-function in vivo results in the failure of this cell types to differentiate in a timely manner (53). Perturbation of the dopamine systems has long been linked to changes in locomotor activity (65,66) and sensorimotor gating (67,68). Dopamine signalling is also associated with food reward processing in the brain (49). We show an increase in the number of Th-positive cells in the A13 dopaminergic cell group and periventricular hypothalamus. As Th is marker for both dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons, and the observation that Cdkn1c is involved in embryonic dopaminergic cell proliferation, this indicates that the development of the dopaminergic system may be compromised in mice over-expressing Cdkn1c. Moreover, given the role of the hypothalamus in the regulation of food intake (50–52), the increased number of Th-positive cells in these regions may contribute to this phenotype.

Patients with microduplications spanning CDKN1C are very rare (20–22). The majority of SRS patients do not carry genetic mutations affecting CDKN1C or epigenetic alterations at the imprinting centre for CDKN1C (KCNQ1OT1:TSS-DMR), which lies over 200 kb away from the gene (69,70). A key question that this study raises, therefore, is the extent to which elevated CDKN1C contributes more widely to SRS. Loss of paternal silencing in mice can occur in circumstances where DNA methylation at the imprinting centre is intact. For example, when the non-coding RNA Kcnq1ot1/Lit1 is prematurely terminated (71,72), when DNA methylation is not maintained in the somatic DMR in the promotor of Cdkn1c (73–75) and when repressive histone marks are not propagated (76). Therefore biallelic expression of CDKN1C in humans could occur independent to changes in KCNQ1OT1:TSS-DMR methylation. CDKN1C levels have not been extensively examined in patients with (epi)mutations at the locus 11p15 locus or in those 40% of patients without an identified genetic or epigenetic defect. Further work is required to establish the prevalence of elevated CDKN1C in SRS and the extent to which our behavioural observations may inform the assessment of these patients. Nonetheless, there is now a considerable body of evidence from animal models and human studies to indicate a role for the imprinted CDKN1C gene in at least three very rare disorders affecting foetal growth (SRS, BWS and IMAGe) syndrome. Importantly, the new knowledge presented in this study will contribute to an improved understanding of the long-term behavioural implications for all these disorders.

In summary, we present the first characterisation of rodent behaviour in response to elevated expression of the imprinted Cdkn1c gene, which identified alteration in in locomotor activity, sensori-motor gating and the hedonic response to food. Although we do not characterise the specific neural perturbation in this model in detail, we observed alterations in a number of Th-positive neurons in the hypothalamus suggesting a direct effect on the developing central nervous system. Further work is required to clarify the relationship between elevated CDKN1C and SRS with a specific reference to neurodevelopmental problems. Indeed, different genetic sub-types of SRS may present with differing ranges of additional problems, as has been suggested previously with respect to SRS scoring system (4). Our data strongly suggest that this would be an avenue of research worth exploring. A better understanding of the pathobiology underpinning any behavioural and cognitive abnormalities, and food disorders in particular, in SRS patients will be helpful in the design of therapeutic approaches in the longer term.

Materials and Methods

Animals

The experimental line Cdkn1cBACx1 possesses one copy of a bacterial artificial chromosome that spans the Cdkn1c gene and two other genes, Phlda2 and Slc22a18. The reporter line Cdkn1cBACLacZ possesses a modified version of this BAC with a β-galactosidase reporter construct inserted into the Cdkn1c locus, disrupting Cdkn1c expression (27). Cdkn1cBACLacZ functions as a control for the experimental line to attribute any phenotypes observed specifically to the over expression of Cdkn1c (i.e. present in line Cdkn1cBACx1 and absent in line Cdkn1cBACLacZ). Both lines were maintained on a C57BL/6 background having been bred onto this background for >12 generations. One hundred and twenty-nine male mice were used for behavioural assessment: Cdkn1cBACx1 (n = 40), WT littermates (n = 36), Cdkn1cBACLacZ (n = 31) WT littermates (n = 22). For all testing minimum group size per genotype was 13. Degrees of freedom is stated in all statistical analysis reflecting the total group size.

LMA and motoric function

LMA behaviour was tested using as described (43) with two infra-red beams crossing each cage 30mm from each end and 10mm from the floor of the chamber. Beam breaks were recorded as an indication of activity, using a computer running custom written BBC Basic V6 programmes with additional interfacing by ARACHNID (Cambridge Cognition Ltd, Cambridge, U.K.). Data stored were the total number of beam-breaks in a 2 hour session. One session was carried out per day, for three consecutive days as a measure of habituation to a novel environment. Habituation to a novel arena was assayed by the decrease in beam breaks across sessions.

Following the three days of basal locomotor activity testing, animals were injected intra-peritoneally (i.p.) with saline or 1 mg/kg of D-amphetmine sulphate (Tocris Bioscience, Bristol, UK). All animals received each solution in a pseudo-randomised order, with 72 hours between each injection to allow for solution wash out. Session total consecutive beam breaks (runs) were used as an indication of amphetamine induced LMA. A rotarod task (Ugo Basile, Italy) was used to assess motoric function. Animals received five training sessions. In the test session latency to fall at a set rotating speed was recorded.

Emotional reactivity

EPM

Anxiety was measured using an EPM. Briefly, apparatus, consisting of four Perspex arms two open (175 × 78 mm) and two enclosed (190 × 80 × 150 mm) with an open roof, was used. Animals were placed centrally at the beginning of a trial and allowed to explore freely for 300 s. Activity was tracked using ETHOVISION software (Noldus, Nottingham, UK). Increased time spent in the open arm was considered less anxious behaviour.

OF test

Locomotor activity and anxiety were measured using distance moved in, and time spent in the centre of, respectively, an OF. Briefly, an OF apparatus (750 × 750 mm) was used, divided into 2 virtual zones, an inner (central 450 × 450 mm) and an outer (150 mm periphery). Animals were allowed to explore the arena freely for 600 s and activity was tracked using ETHOVISION software (Noldus, Nottingham, UK). Greater time spent in the inner zone was considered less anxious behaviour.

ASR and PPI

ASR and PPI were monitored using a SR-Lab apparatus (San Diego Instruments, U.S.A) modified for use in mice. Animals were placed in a Perspex tube (internal diameter 35mm) and white noise stimuli were presented via a speaker. Pulse-alone trials consisted of a 40ms startle stimulus and a prepulse trial consisted of a 20ms prepulse at 4, 8, or 16db above background and a 40ms 105db startle stimulus, 70ms after the prepulse. The stimuli were presented in a pseudorandom manner every 15s. Whole body startle responses were recorded as the average startle during a 65ms window timed from the onset of the startle pulse. PPI was calculated as the percentage reduction in startle between prepulse trials and pulse alone trials.

Preference testing

Mice had restricted food access for the duration of testing, having no access to food for 16 hours prior to testing. All testing took place in opaque Perspex boxes with access to the solution provided for thirty minutes, once per day. Preference testing was carried out on seven consecutive days. For three days mice were presented with two bottles of water to ensure there was no side bias. For the following four consecutive days one bottle contained an 8% (w/v) sucrose solution, which altered left-right presentation by day. Bottles were weighed before and after testing and sucrose preference was calculated as (volume sucrose consumed/total volume consumed)*100.

Lick cluster analysis

LCA testing was carried out as described (58) with food restriction as in preference testing. A contact sensitive lickometer registered each lick to the nearest 0.01 s, recorded using MED-PC software (Med Associates Inc., St. Albans, VT, USA). Animals were trained to drink 8% (w/w) sucrose in water prior to testing, until consumption had stabilised (requiring 4–14 sessions). Animals were presented with a solution of a given sucrose concentration for a minimum of four days; the order of presentation was counterbalanced for genotypes. Sessions in which data records indicated errors in lick recording or spout blockage were removed from the analysis; specifically recording fewer than 10 bouts, greater than 40 isolated single licks or average interlick intervals of <90 or >250 ms. This equated to the exclusion of 48/721 data points for Cdkn1cBACx1 animals and WT littermates and 117/752 data points for Cdkn1cBACLacZ animals. Inclusion of these data points does not alter the result. The data from the first day at each new concentration was excluded. Data were averaged across sessions at each concentration for comparison. A pause between licks of greater than 0.5 s was defined as a new cluster.

Immunohistochemistry

Animals were transcardially perfused with 10% formalin fixative (Sigma-Aldrich, UK) and brains removed. Following post-fixing overnight, brains were removed to a solution of PBS containing 30% sucrose. 40 μm sections were obtained from Bregma +2 to −2.5 on a freezing microtome. Immunostaining for Th was carried out on 1 in 5 sections (every 120 μm). Free-floating sections were incubated overnight in 1:1000 rabbit-α-Th (Abcam, UK) and section was performed using standard DAB staining (Vector labs). Number of positive cells in A13 dopamnergic cell group of the zona incerta and periventricular hypothalamus were counted on all sections.

Statistics

All statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS 20.0 (SPSS, USA). Simple effects were analysed by Students t-test. For repeated behavioural analysis a series of ANOVAs were carried out on the data with a between subject factor of GENOTYPE and within subjects factor of CONCENTRATION/DAY/BIN/PRE-PULSE INTENSITY, where appropriate. Sucrose preference data and effect of genotype on preference were analysed using Wilcoxon signed rank and Mann-Whitney U-tests, respectively. Differences in Th immunoreactivity was analysed by Students t-test.

Acknowledgements

We thank Simon Tunster for his advice and support of transgenic models.

Conflict of Interest statement. None declared.

Funding

This work was supported by a Wellcome Trust PhD studentship (Grant number WT093766MA). Funding to pay the Open Access publication charges for this article was provided by Cardiff University.

References

1

Wakeling
E.L.
(
2011
)
Silver-Russell syndrome
.
Arch. Dis. Child
.,
96
,
1156
1161
.

2

Wakeling
E.L.
Amero
S.A.
Alders
M.
Bliek
J.
Forsythe
E.
Kumar
S.
Lim
D.H.
MacDonald
F.
Mackay
D.J.
Maher
E.R.
, et al. . (
2010
)
Epigenotype-phenotype correlations in Silver-Russell syndrome
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
47
,
760
768
.

3

Eggermann
T.
Begemann
M.
Spengler
S.
Schroder
C.
Kordass
U.
Binder
G.
(
2011
)
Genetic and epigenetic findings in Silver-Russell syndrome
.
Pediatr. Endocrinol. Rev
.,
8
,
86
93
.

4

Eggermann
K.
Bliek
J.
Brioude
F.
Algar
E.
Buiting
K.
Russo
S.
Tumer
Z.
Monk
D.
Moore
G.
Antoniadi
T.
, et al. . (
2016
)
EMQN best practice guidelines for the molecular genetic testing and reporting of chromosome 11p15 imprinting disorders: Silver-Russell and Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome
.
Eur. J. Hum. Genet
.,
24
:
1377
1387
.

5

Donnai
D.
Thompson
E.
Allanson
J.
Baraitser
M.
(
1989
)
Severe Silver-Russell syndrome
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
26
,
447
451
.

6

Patton
M.A.
(
1988
)
Russell-Silver syndrome
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
25
,
557
560
.

7

Perkins
R.M.
Hoang-Xuan
M.T.
(
2002
)
The Russell-Silver syndrome: a case report and brief review of the literature
.
Pediatr. Dermatol
.,
19
,
546
549
.

8

Price
S.M.
Stanhope
R.
Garrett
C.
Preece
M.A.
Trembath
R.C.
(
1999
)
The spectrum of Silver-Russell syndrome: a clinical and molecular genetic study and new diagnostic criteria
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
36
,
837
842
.

9

Lai
K.Y.
Skuse
D.
Stanhope
R.
Hindmarsh
P.
(
1994
)
Cognitive abilities associated with the Silver-Russell syndrome
.
Arch. Dis. Child
,
71
,
490
496
.

10

Noeker
M.
Wollmann
H.A.
(
2004
)
Cognitive development in Silver-Russell syndrome: a sibling-controlled study
.
Dev. Med. Child Neurol
.,
46
,
340
346
.

11

Plotts
C.A.
Livermore
C.L.
(
2007
)
Russell-Silver syndrome and nonverbal learning disability: a case study
.
Appl. Neuropsychol
.,
14
,
124
134
.

12

Saal
H.M.
Pagon
R.A.
Pepin
M.G.
(
1985
)
Reevaluation of Russell-Silver syndrome
.
J. Pediatr
.,
107
,
733
737
.

13

Russell
A.
(
1954
)
A syndrome of intra-uterine dwarfism recognizable at birth with cranio-facial dysostosis, disproportionately short arms, and other anomalies (5 examples)
.
Proc. R. Soc. Med
.,
47
,
1040
1044
.

14

Azcona
C.
Stanhope
R.
(
2005
)
Hypoglycaemia and Russell-Silver syndrome
.
J. Pediatr. Endocrinol. Metab
.,
18
,
663
670
.

15

Blissett
J.
Harris
G.
Kirk
J.
(
2001
)
Feeding problems in Silver-Russell syndrome
.
Dev. Med. Child Neurol
.,
43
,
39
44
.

16

Marsaud
C.
Rossignol
S.
Tounian
P.
Netchine
I.
Dubern
B.
(
2015
)
Prevalence and management of gastrointestinal manifestations in Silver-Russell syndrome
.
Arch. Dis. Child
,
100
,
353
358
.

17

Netchine
I.
Rossignol
S.
Dufourg
M.N.
Azzi
S.
Rousseau
A.
Perin
L.
Houang
M.
Steunou
V.
Esteva
B.
Thibaud
N.
, et al. . (
2007
)
11p15 imprinting center region 1 loss of methylation is a common and specific cause of typical Russell-Silver syndrome: clinical scoring system and epigenetic-phenotypic correlations
.
J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab
.,
92
,
3148
3154
.

18

Azzi
S.
Salem
J.
Thibaud
N.
Chantot-Bastaraud
S.
Lieber
E.
Netchine
I.
Harbison
M.D.
(
2015
)
A prospective study validating a clinical scoring system and demonstrating phenotypical-genotypical correlations in Silver-Russell syndrome
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
52
,
446
453
.

19

Surani
M.A.
(
1998
)
Imprinting and the initiation of gene silencing in the germ line
.
Cell
,
93
,
309
312
.

20

Schonherr
N.
Meyer
E.
Roos
A.
Schmidt
A.
Wollmann
H.A.
Eggermann
T.
(
2007
)
The centromeric 11p15 imprinting centre is also involved in Silver-Russell syndrome
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
44
,
59
63
.

21

Bonaldi
A.
Mazzeu
J.F.
Costa
S.S.
Honjo
R.S.
Bertola
D.R.
Albano
L.M.
Furquim
I.M.
Kim
C.A.
Vianna-Morgante
A.M.
(
2011
)
Microduplication of the ICR2 domain at chromosome 11p15 and familial Silver-Russell syndrome
.
Am. J. Med. Genet. A
,
155A
,
2479
2483
.

22

Nakashima
S.
Kato
F.
Kosho
T.
Nagasaki
K.
Kikuchi
T.
Kagami
M.
Fukami
M.
Ogata
T.
(
2015
)
Silver-Russell syndrome without body asymmetry in three patients with duplications of maternally derived chromosome 11p15 involving CDKN1C
.
J. Hum. Genet
.,
60
,
91
95
.

23

Hatada
I.
Mukai
T.
(
1995
)
Genomic imprinting of p57KIP2, a cyclin-dependent kinase inhibitor, in mouse
.
Nat. Genet
.,
11
,
204
206
.

24

Hatada
I.
Inazawa
J.
Abe
T.
Nakayama
M.
Kaneko
Y.
Jinno
Y.
Niikawa
N.
Ohashi
H.
Fukushima
Y.
Iida
K.
, et al. . (
1996
)
Genomic imprinting of human p57KIP2 and its reduced expression in Wilms' tumors
.
Hum. Mol. Genet
.,
5
,
783
788
.

25

Xue
Y.
Shankar
S.
Cornell
K.
Dai
Z.
Wang
C.E.
Rudd
M.K.
Coffee
B.
(
2015
)
Paternal duplication of the 11p15 centromeric imprinting control region is associated with increased expression of CDKN1C in a child with Russell-Silver syndrome
.
Am. J. Med. Genet. A
,
167A
,
3229
3233
.

26

Boonen
S.E.
Freschi
A.
Christensen
R.
Valente
F.M.
Lildballe
D.L.
Perone
L.
Palumbo
O.
Carella
M.
Uldbjerg
N.
Sparago
A.
, et al. . (
2016
)
Two maternal duplications involving the CDKN1C gene are associated with contrasting growth phenotypes
.
Clin. Epigenetics
,
8
,
69.

27

Arboleda
V.A.
Lee
H.
Parnaik
R.
Fleming
A.
Banerjee
A.
Ferraz-de-Souza
B.
Delot
E.C.
Rodriguez-Fernandez
I.A.
Braslavsky
D.
Bergada
I.
, et al. . (
2012
)
Mutations in the PCNA-binding domain of CDKN1C cause IMAGe syndrome
.
Nat. Genet
.,
44
:
788
792
.

28

Hamajima
N.
Johmura
Y.
Suzuki
S.
Nakanishi
M.
Saitoh
S.
(
2013
)
Increased protein stability of CDKN1C causes a gain-of-function phenotype in patients with IMAGe syndrome
.
PloS One
,
8
,
e75137.

29

Hatada
I.
Nabetani
A.
Morisaki
H.
Xin
Z.
Ohishi
S.
Tonoki
H.
Niikawa
N.
Inoue
M.
Komoto
Y.
Okada
A.
, et al. . (
1997
)
New p57KIP2 mutations in Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome
.
Hum. Genet
.,
100
,
681
683
.

30

Hatada
I.
Ohashi
H.
Fukushima
Y.
Kaneko
Y.
Inoue
M.
Komoto
Y.
Okada
A.
Ohishi
S.
Nabetani
A.
Morisaki
H.
, et al. . (
1996
)
An imprinted gene p57KIP2 is mutated in Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome
.
Nat. Genet
,
14
,
171
173
.

31

Lee
M.P.
DeBaun
M.
Randhawa
G.
Reichard
B.A.
Elledge
S.J.
Feinberg
A.P.
(
1997
)
Low frequency of p57KIP2 mutation in Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome
.
Am. J. Hum. Genet
.,
61
,
304
309
.

32

O'Keefe
D.
Dao
D.
Zhao
L.
Sanderson
R.
Warburton
D.
Weiss
L.
Anyane-Yeboa
K.
Tycko
B.
(
1997
)
Coding mutations in p57KIP2 are present in some cases of Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome but are rare or absent in Wilms tumors
.
Am. J. Hum. Genet
.,
61
,
295
303
.

33

Engel
J.R.
Smallwood
A.
Harper
A.
Higgins
M.J.
Oshimura
M.
Reik
W.
Schofield
P.N.
Maher
E.R.
(
2000
)
Epigenotype-phenotype correlations in Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
37
,
921
926
.

34

Brioude
F.
Oliver-Petit
I.
Blaise
A.
Praz
F.
Rossignol
S.
Le Jule
M.
Thibaud
N.
Faussat
A.M.
Tauber
M.
Le Bouc
Y.
, et al. . (
2013
)
CDKN1C mutation affecting the PCNA-binding domain as a cause of familial Russell Silver syndrome
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
50
,
823
830
.

35

Eggermann
T.
Binder
G.
Brioude
F.
Maher
E.R.
Lapunzina
P.
Cubellis
M.V.
Bergada
I.
Prawitt
D.
Begemann
M.
(
2014
)
CDKN1C mutations: two sides of the same coin
.
Trends Mol. Med
.,
20
,
614
622
.

36

Zhang
P.
Liegeois
N.J.
Wong
C.
Finegold
M.
Hou
H.
Thompson
J.C.
Silverman
A.
Harper
J.W.
DePinho
R.A.
Elledge
S.J.
(
1997
)
Altered cell differentiation and proliferation in mice lacking p57KIP2 indicates a role in Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome
.
Nature
,
387
,
151
158
.

37

Yan
Y.
Frisen
J.
Lee
M.H.
Massague
J.
Barbacid
M.
(
1997
)
Ablation of the CDK inhibitor p57Kip2 results in increased apoptosis and delayed differentiation during mouse development
.
Genes Dev
.,
11
,
973
983
.

38

Takahashi
K.
Kobayashi
T.
Kanayama
N.
(
2000
)
p57(Kip2) regulates the proper development of labyrinthine and spongiotrophoblasts
.
Mol. Hum. Reprod
.,
6
,
1019
1025
.

39

Andrews
S.C.
Wood
M.D.
Tunster
S.J.
Barton
S.C.
Surani
M.A.
John
R.M.
(
2007
)
Cdkn1c (p57Kip2) is the major regulator of embryonic growth within its imprinted domain on mouse distal chromosome 7
.
BMC Dev. Biol
.,
7
,
53.

40

Tunster
S.J.
Van de Pette
M.
John
R.M.
(
2011
)
Fetal overgrowth in the Cdkn1c mouse model of Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome
.
Dis. Model. Mech
.,
4
,
814
821
.

41

Van De Pette
M.
Tunster
S.J.
McNamara
G.I.
Shelkovnikova
T.
Millership
S.
Benson
L.
Peirson
S.
Christian
M.
Vidal-Puig
A.
John
R.M.
(
2016
)
Cdkn1c Boosts the Development of Brown Adipose Tissue in a Murine Model of Silver Russell Syndrome
.
PLoS Genetics
,
12
,
e1005916.

42

John
R.M.
Ainscough
J.F.
Barton
S.C.
Surani
M.A.
(
2001
)
Distant cis-elements regulate imprinted expression of the mouse p57 (Kip2) (Cdkn1c) gene: implications for the human disorder, Beckwith–Wiedemann syndrome
.
Hum. Mol. Genet
.,
10
,
1601
1609
.

43

Dent
C.L.
Isles
A.R.
Humby
T.
(
2014
)
Measuring risk-taking in mice: balancing the risk between seeking reward and danger
.
Eur. J. Neurosci
.,
39
,
520
530
.

44

Braff
D.L.
Geyer
M.A.
(
1990
)
Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia. Human and animal model studies
.
Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
,
47
,
181
188
.

45

Perry
W.
Minassian
A.
Lopez
B.
Maron
L.
Lincoln
A.
(
2007
)
Sensorimotor gating deficits in adults with autism
.
Biol. Psychiatry
,
61
,
482
486
.

46

Kuroda
K.
Yamada
S.
Tanaka
M.
Iizuka
M.
Yano
H.
Mori
D.
Tsuboi
D.
Nishioka
T.
Namba
T.
Iizuka
Y.
, et al. . (
2011
)
Behavioral alterations associated with targeted disruption of exons 2 and 3 of the Disc1 gene in the mouse
.
Hum. Mol. Genet
.,
20
,
4666
4683
.

47

Samaco
R.C.
Fryer
J.D.
Ren
J.
Fyffe
S.
Chao
H.T.
Sun
Y.
Greer
J.J.
Zoghbi
H.Y.
Neul
J.L.
(
2008
)
A partial loss of function allele of methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 predicts a human neurodevelopmental syndrome
.
Hum. Mol. Genet
.,
17
,
1718
1727
.

48

Dwyer
D.M.
(
2012
)
EPS Prize Lecture. Licking and liking: the assessment of hedonic responses in rodents
.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove)
.,
65
,
371
394
.

49

Avena
N.M.
Bocarsly
M.E.
(
2012
)
Dysregulation of brain reward systems in eating disorders: neurochemical information from animal models of binge eating, bulimia nervosa, and anorexia nervosa
.
Neuropharmacology
,
63
,
87
96
.

50

Leibowitz
S.F.
(
1978
)
Paraventricular nucleus: a primary site mediating adrenergic stimulation of feeding and drinking
.
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav
.,
8
,
163
175
.

51

Martin
G.E.
Myers
R.D.
(
1976
)
Dopamine efflux from the brain stem of the rat during feeding, drinking and lever-pressing for food
.
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav
.,
4
,
551
560
.

52

Morton
G.J.
Cummings
D.E.
Baskin
D.G.
Barsh
G.S.
Schwartz
M.W.
(
2006
)
Central nervous system control of food intake and body weight
.
Nature
,
443
,
289
295
.

53

Joseph
B.
Wallen-Mackenzie
A.
Benoit
G.
Murata
T.
Joodmardi
E.
Okret
S.
Perlmann
T.
(
2003
)
p57(Kip2) cooperates with Nurr1 in developing dopamine cells
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A
,
100
,
15619
15624
.

54

Tury
A.
Mairet-Coello
G.
DiCicco-Bloom
E.
(
2012
)
The cyclin-dependent kinase inhibitor p57Kip2 regulates cell cycle exit, differentiation, and migration of embryonic cerebral cortical precursors
.
Cereb. Cortex
,
21
,
1840
1856
.

55

Inoue
K.
Natsuyama
T.
Miyaoka
H.
(
2014
)
Case report of schizophrenia in adolescent with Russell-Silver syndrome
.
Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci
.,
68
,
582.

56

Vardi
O.
Davidovitch
M.
Vinkler
C.
Michelson
M.
Lerman-Sagie
T.
Lev
D.
(
2012
)
Autistic regression in a child with Silver-Russell syndrome and maternal UPD 7
.
Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol
.,
16
,
95
98
.

57

Lydall
E.S.
Gilmour
G.
Dwyer
D.M.
(
2010
)
Analysis of licking microstructure provides no evidence for a reduction in reward value following acute or sub-chronic phencyclidine administration
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
209
,
153
162
.

58

Davies
J.R.
Humby
T.
Dwyer
D.M.
Garfield
A.S.
Furby
H.
Wilkinson
L.S.
Wells
T.
Isles
A.R.
(
2015
)
Calorie seeking, but not hedonic response, contributes to hyperphagia in a mouse model for Prader-Willi syndrome
.
Eur. J. Neurosci
.,
42
,
2105
2113
.

59

Davis
J.D.
Perez
M.C.
(
1993
)
Food deprivation- and palatability-induced microstructural changes in ingestive behavior
.
Am. J. Physiol
.,
264
,
R97
103
.

60

Dwyer
D.M.
(
2008
)
Microstructural analysis of conditioned and unconditioned responses to maltodextrin
.
Learning & Behavior
,
36
,
149
158
.

61

Sorensen
L.B.
Moller
P.
Flint
A.
Martens
M.
Raben
A.
(
2003
)
Effect of sensory perception of foods on appetite and food intake: a review of studies on humans
.
Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord
.,
27
,
1152
1166
.

62

Mairet-Coello
G.
Tury
A.
Van Buskirk
E.
Robinson
K.
Genestine
M.
DiCicco-Bloom
E.
(
2012
)
p57(KIP2) regulates radial glia and intermediate precursor cell cycle dynamics and lower layer neurogenesis in developing cerebral cortex
.
Development
,
139
,
475
487
.

63

Furutachi
S.
Matsumoto
A.
Nakayama
K.I.
Gotoh
Y.
(
2013
)
p57 controls adult neural stem cell quiescence and modulates the pace of lifelong neurogenesis
.
Embo. J
.,
32
,
970
981
.

64

Marin-Burgin
A.
Schinder
A.F.
(
2012
)
Requirement of adult-born neurons for hippocampus-dependent learning
.
Behav. Brain Res
.,
227
,
391
399
.

65

Amalric
M.
Koob
G.F.
(
1993
)
Functionally selective neurochemical afferents and efferents of the mesocorticolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine system
.
Prog. Brain Res
.,
99
,
209
226
.

66

Beccano-Kelly
D.A.
Volta
M.
Munsie
L.N.
Paschall
S.A.
Tatarnikov
I.
Co
K.
Chou
P.
Cao
L.P.
Bergeron
S.
Mitchell
E.
, et al. . (
2015
)
LRRK2 overexpression alters glutamatergic presynaptic plasticity, striatal dopamine tone, postsynaptic signal transduction, motor activity and memory
.
Hum. Mol. Genet
.,
24
,
1336
1349
.

67

During
S.
Glenthoj
B.Y.
Andersen
G.S.
Oranje
B.
(
2014
)
Effects of dopamine D2/D3 blockade on human sensory and sensorimotor gating in initially antipsychotic-naive, first-episode schizophrenia patients
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
39
,
3000
3008
.

68

Tournier
B.B.
Ginovart
N.
(
2014
)
Repeated but not acute treatment with (9)-tetrahydrocannabinol disrupts prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle: reversal by the dopamine D(2)/(3) receptor antagonist haloperidol
.
Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol
.,
24
,
1415
1423
.

69

Eggermann
T.
Schonherr
N.
Meyer
E.
Obermann
C.
Mavany
M.
Eggermann
K.
Ranke
M.B.
Wollmann
H.A.
(
2006
)
Epigenetic mutations in 11p15 in Silver-Russell syndrome are restricted to the telomeric imprinting domain
.
J. Med. Genet
.,
43
,
615
616
.

70

Moore
G.E.
(
2011
)
What is the evidence for causal epigenetic influences on the Silver-Russell syndrome phenotype?
.
Epigenomics
,
3
,
529
531
.

71

Mancini-Dinardo
D.
Steele
S.J.
Levorse
J.M.
Ingram
R.S.
Tilghman
S.M.
(
2006
)
Elongation of the Kcnq1ot1 transcript is required for genomic imprinting of neighboring genes
.
Genes. Dev
.,
20
,
1268
1282
.

72

Shin
J.Y.
Fitzpatrick
G.V.
Higgins
M.J.
(
2008
)
Two distinct mechanisms of silencing by the KvDMR1 imprinting control region
.
Embo J
.,
27
,
168
178
.

73

Caspary
T.
Cleary
M.A.
Baker
C.C.
Guan
X.J.
Tilghman
S.M.
(
1998
)
Multiple mechanisms regulate imprinting of the mouse distal chromosome 7 gene cluster
.
Mol. Cell. Biol
.,
18
,
3466
3474
.

74

Bhogal
B.
Arnaudo
A.
Dymkowski
A.
Best
A.
Davis
T.L.
(
2004
)
Methylation at mouse Cdkn1c is acquired during postimplantation development and functions to maintain imprinted expression
.
Genomics
,
84
,
961
970
.

75

Fan
T.
Hagan
J.P.
Kozlov
S.V.
Stewart
C.L.
Muegge
K.
(
2005
)
Lsh controls silencing of the imprinted Cdkn1c gene
.
Development
,
132
,
635
644
.

76

Mager
J.
Montgomery
N.D.
de Villena
F.P.
Magnuson
T.
(
2003
)
Genome imprinting regulated by the mouse Polycomb group protein Eed
.
Nat. Genet
.,
33
,
502
507
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.