Abstract

STUDY QUESTION

Are there more same-sex (SS) than opposite-sex (OS) pairs among dizygotic twins?

SUMMARY ANSWER

In violation of Weinberg’s Differential Rule, there are significantly more SS than OS pairs among dizygotic twins in nationally representative samples both in the UK and the USA.

WHAT IS KNOWN ALREADY

Since at least 1874, twin researchers have assumed that dizygotic twins were equally likely to be SS or OS (later formalized as Weinberg’s Differential Rule). However, recent research on the sex-specific nature of breastmilk suggests that OS twins might be at a developmental disadvantage relative to SS twins. There may therefore be evolutionary selection for the tendency to have SS twins and against the tendency to have OS twins.

STUDY DESIGN, SIZE, DURATION

We analyze two population samples (not subject to Lykken’s rule of two-thirds): the National Child Development Study in the UK (n = 17 419) from 1958 to present, and the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health in the USA (n = 20 745) from 1994 to present.

PARTICIPANTS/MATERIALS, SETTING, METHODS

We performed a one-sample t-test of the proportion of SS dizygotic twins against the theoretical null value of 0.5022.

MAIN RESULTS AND THE ROLE OF CHANCE

The proportion of SS pairs among dizygotic twins was 0.6043 (t(325) = 3.838, P = 0.00015) in the UK and 0.5739 (t(520) = 3.398, P = 0.00073) in the USA.

LIMITATIONS, REASONS FOR CAUTION

Twin type (monozygotic versus dizygotic) was classified by the mother, not by DNA typing, although supplementary data suggest that misclassification was negligible and supplementary analysis shows that potential misclassifications do not change our main conclusions.

WIDER IMPLICATIONS OF THE FINDINGS

Weinberg’s Differential Rule may be incorrect in its assumption of independence of sexes within dizygotic pairs, and there may be evolutionary selection for SS, and against OS, dizygotic pairs.

STUDY FUNDING/COMPETING INTERESTS

None.

Introduction

Before modern genetic knowledge and technology, it was impossible to determine the zygosity of same-sex (SS) twins with certainty, and it was therefore difficult to estimate accurately the population incidence of monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins. It was in this context that the French statistician Jacques Bertillon (1874) first speculated that the number of DZ twins would equal twice the number of opposite-sex (OS) twins and that the remaining twins would be MZ. Weinberg (1901) later formalized Bertillon’s idea and proposed what is now known as Weinberg’s Differential Rule (WDR).

WDR is premised on two assumptions (Fellman, 2013): (i) the probability of conception of male and female fetuses is identical (= 0.5); and (ii) the sexes of DZ twins are determined independently; therefore, DZ twins would be equally likely to be SS as OS. Despite the fact that we now know that the secondary sex ratio (the ratio of boys to girls at birth) deviates slightly from 1.00 (it is instead 1.05, 105 boys for every 100 girls), and that some have questioned its validity (James, 1979), WDR has been largely supported. For example, Fellman and Eriksson (2006) analyzed twin data from Sweden and Finland, and concluded that ‘WDR is robust and, despite its simplicity, gives reliable results when large national birth registers are considered’ (p. 254).

A couple of theoretical reasons cast doubt on the second assumption—the independent determination of sexes in DZ twins—and thus the validity of WDR itself. First, the Trivers–Willard hypothesis proposes that parental condition influences the secondary sex ratios, such that parents in good environmental and physiological condition are more likely to have sons while parents in poor environmental and physiological condition are more likely to have daughters (Trivers and Willard, 1973). Because twins share their parents and parental condition, they are more likely to be SS—either both male if the parental condition is good or both female if the parental condition is poor.

Second, recent research in developmental psychology and biology also questions the independent determination of sexes in DZ twin pairs. Several studies have shown that human and other mammalian breastmilk may be tailored for the sex of the offspring, such that mothers may produce breastmilk of different chemical composition for male and female newborns. For example, among humans, mothers both in Massachusetts, USA (Powe et al., 2010) and in Singapore (Thakkar et al., 2013) produced milk with greater energy, lipids, polyunsaturated fatty acids, phospholipids and gangliosides for sons than for daughters. In contrast, a study of Filipino mothers did not find a significant difference in fat, protein, sugars and energy between breastmilk for sons and daughters (Quinn, 2013). Consistent with the Trivers–Willard hypothesis (Trivers and Willard, 1973), economically sufficient mothers in northern Kenya produced milk with higher fat concentration for sons than for daughters, whereas relatively poor mothers produced milk with higher fat concentration for daughters than for sons (Fujita et al., 2012).

If human mothers tailor the contents of their breastmilk for the offspring of each sex to facilitate their health and growth most efficiently, then it suggests that breastmilk might not be as closely tailored for OS twins as it is for SS twins. Whether they are male or female, SS twins can benefit from the sex-tailored breastmilk just as singletons can. In contrast, the breastmilk of the mothers of OS twins cannot specifically and simultaneously be tailored for both sexes; it must be either tailored for neither sex specifically or tailored for the wrong sex for half of the OS twins. OS twins may therefore be at a developmental disadvantage relative to SS twins. Consistent with this logic, Kanazawa and Segal (2017) found that, net of sex, age, birthweight and zygosity, breastfed SS twins were on average about 1 inch taller and 12 pounds heavier than breastfed OS twins throughout adolescence and early adulthood. In contrast, never-breastfed SS twins were no taller or heavier than never-breastfed OS twins, and they even tended to be slightly shorter and lighter.

If OS twins have been at a developmental disadvantage relative to SS twins over the course of human evolutionary history, then evolutionary logic suggests that there may have been selective forces for the tendency to produce SS twins and against the tendency to produce OS twins. If so, then, in violation of the second assumption of WDR, determination of the sexes in DZ twins may not be independent, and there may be more SS DZ twins than OS DZ twins.

As early as nearly half a century ago, James (1971) noticed, in a secondary analysis of eight published studies, that there were more SS DZ twins than OS DZ twins. James’s original discovery, however, suffers from both theoretical and empirical difficulties. Theoretically, James’s (1986) explanation for the excess of SS pairs among DZ twins was entirely proximate. He argued that the sex of the fetus was largely determined by the timing of conception during the menstrual cycle and the hormonal environment of the uterus at the time of conception. Since twins are (with presumably rare exceptions in the event of superfecundation or superfetation; see Segal, 2017) conceived at the same time, they share the timing of conception and the hormonal environment of the uterus, and are therefore more likely to be SS. If correct, this may answer the how question of proximate mechanisms in terms of endocrinology, but it does not answer the why question of ultimate evolutionary function.

Empirically, all of the studies reviewed by James (1971) to demonstrate the excess of SS DZ twins rely on volunteer samples of twins, who are specifically recruited to participate in the study because they are twins. Volunteer samples in twin research suffer from Lykken’s ‘rule of two-thirds’ (Lykken et al., 1978, 1987). Typically, about two-thirds of respondents in such volunteer twin samples are female, and about two-thirds are MZ. These twins are therefore unlikely to be representative of the entire population of twins, and estimates of the relative frequencies of SS and OS pairs among DZ twins are at the very least inefficient and possibly biased.

Fellman and Eriksson (2006) found support for WDR in their analysis of twin data from Sweden and Finland. The twin registry in Finland, which includes virtually all twins born in the country (Kaprio et al., 2002), showed no excess of SS DZ twins; in the most recent data, the proportion of SS pairs among DZ twins in Finland was 0.4698 (Kaprio, personal communication). Due to the availability of comprehensive twin registries in Scandinavian and Nordic countries, a large proportion of twin studies are conducted there. To our knowledge, no test of WDR has been conducted outside of northern Europe since James’ (1971) meta-analysis nearly half a century ago.

In this paper, we use two large population samples not subject to Lykken’s rule of two-thirds—the National Child Development Study (NCDS) in the UK and the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) in the USA—to estimate the relative frequencies of SS and OS pairs among DZ twins and to test the hypothesis that there are significantly more SS DZ twins than OS DZ twins. It is important to note that the twins in both studies were conceived naturally, prior to the introduction of IVF, which occurred in the UK in 1978 and in the USA in the early 1980s (Segal, 2017). IVF might have allowed parents to choose SS or OS twins for various reasons. It is also largely responsible for the recent rise in DZ twinning in western nations (Pison et al., 2015).

Materials and Methods

National Child Development Study (UK)

Data and sampling

NCDS is a large ongoing prospectively longitudinal study that has followed a population of British respondents since birth for over half a century. The study included all babies (n = 17 419) born in Great Britain (England, Wales and Scotland) during 1 week (03–09 March 1958). The respondents were subsequently reinterviewed in 1965 (Sweep 1 at age 7; n = 15 496), 1969 (Sweep 2 at age 11; n = 18 285), 1974 (Sweep 3 at age 16; n = 14 469), 1981 (Sweep 4 at age 23; n = 12 537), 1991 (Sweep 5 at age 33; n = 11 469), 1999–2000 (Sweep 6 at age 41–42; n = 11 419), 2004–2005 (Sweep 7 at age 46–47; n = 9534), 2008–2009 (Sweep 8 at age 50–51; n = 9790) and 2013 (Sweep 9 at age 55; n = 9137). All ages in this study are in calendar years. There were more respondents in Sweep 2 than in the original sample (Sweep 0) because Sweep 2 sample included eligible children who were in the country in 1969 but not in 1958. In each sweep, personal interviews and questionnaires were administered to the respondents, to their mothers, teachers and doctors during childhood, and to their partners and children in adulthood. Virtually all (97.8%) of the NCDS respondents were Caucasian.

MZ/DZ and SS/OS status

During Sweep 1, the mother of the respondent was asked whether the respondent was a twin, and, if so, whether he/she was MZ or DZ, and, if DZ, whether he/she was SS or OS. There were 326 individual DZ twins (158 female, 168 male), of whom 129 were OS (65 female, 64 male) and 197 were SS (93 female, 104 male). Note that, unlike most studies in twin research (Segal, 2012; Frazier et al., 2014), our samples in both studies contained only one of the twins, not both.

In 2008, NCDS conducted a twin sub-study with 244 twin respondents and attempted to ascertain their zygosity by using the six ‘peas in a pod’ questions. Sample items were: ‘In childhood, which of the following would best describe you and your twin? As alike as peas in a pod/of ordinary sibling likeness,’ and ‘At school, did teachers have trouble telling you apart?’ The same questions were used by researchers in the well-known St. Thomas’s Twin Research Unit Register. Past studies show that these questions can accurately classify the zygosity of twins with nearly 100% accuracy (Cederlöf et al., 1961; Torgersen, 1979). NCDS classified 18.8% of twin respondents in the 2008 sub-study as MZ by these ‘peas in a pod’ questions, whereas 20.5% of the respondents self-reported as MZ. Thus, the extent of potential misclassification of twin zygosity by self-report appeared very small (1.7%).

Critical (null) value

WDR assumes that the probability of male conception is 0.5000, which implies the critical value—the proportion of SS pairs among DZ twins—of 0.5000: P(BB) = 0.5000 × 0.5000 = 0.2500; P(BG) = 0.5000 × 0.5000 = 0.2500; P(GB) = 0.5000 × 0.5000 = 0.2500; P(GG) = 0.5000 × 0.5000 = 0.2500; P(SS DZ) = P(BB) + P(GG) = 0.2500 + 0.2500 = 0.5000, where, B = boy, G = girl. However, we now know that the probability of male conception is 0.5122, which implies the critical value of 0.5002: P(BB) = 0.5122 × 0.5122 = 0.2623; P(BG) = 0.5122 × 0.4878 = 0.2499; P(GB) = 0.4878 × 0.5122 = 0.2499; P(GG) = 0.4878 × 0.4878 = 0.2379; P(SS DZ) = P(BB) + P(GG) = 0.2623 + 0.2379 = 0.5002. Using Swedish data, Fellman and Eriksson (2010) estimate the probability of male conception among all twins to be slightly lower at 0.5079. The critical value using their estimate, however, turns out to be exactly the same: P(BB) = 0.5079 × 0.5079 = 0.2580; P(BG) = 0.5079 × 0.4921 = 0.2499; P(GB) = 0.4921 × 0.5079 = 0.2499; P(GG) = 0.4921 × 0.4921 = 0.2422; P(SS DZ) = P(BB) + P(GG) = 0.2580 + 0.2422 = 0.5002. We therefore used the critical value of 0.5002 in our one-sample t-tests with both samples.

Add Health (USA)

Data and sampling

Add Health is a large on-going, nationally representative, and prospectively longitudinal study of young Americans. A sample of 20 745 adolescents was personally interviewed in their homes in 1994–1995 (Wave I; mean age = 15.6 years). They were again interviewed in 1996 (Wave II; n = 14 738; mean age = 16.2 years), 2001–2002 (Wave III; n = 15 197; mean age = 22.0 years) and 2007–2008 (Wave IV; n = 15 701; mean age = 29.1 years). Additional details of sampling and study design are provided at http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design.

MZ/DZ and SS/OS status

Add Health assessed the zygosity of each twin during Wave I by asking the mother, ‘In your opinion, are _____ and _____ identical twins or fraternal twins?’ The mother could respond as follows: 1 = definitely identical, 2 = probably identical, 3 = probably fraternal or 4 = definitely fraternal. We collapsed ‘definitely’ and ‘probably’ and created a binary measure of zygosity. There were 521 individual DZ twins (270 female, 251 male), of whom 222 were OS (112 female, 110 male) and 299 were SS (158 female, 141 male). Add Health conducted DNA analysis at Wave III and discovered that only 9% (n = 34) of the twins’ zygosity was misclassified by mothers’ self-report at Wave I (Harris et al., 2006, p. 992).

Statistical analysis

We performed one-sample t-tests, with the critical value of 0.5002, with both samples, using SPSS 23 (IBM, NY, USA). We adopted the conventional level of statistical significance (α = 0.05).

Results

National Child Development Study (UK)

The proportion of SS pairs among DZ twins in the UK was 0.6043, which was significantly higher than the critical value of 0.5002 (t(325) = 3.838, P = 0.00015). Fellman (2013) and Fellman and Eriksson (2006) suggest that the variances of MZ and DZ twinning rates might be larger than previously assumed, at least by a factor of 2, and thus that it might be necessary to multiply the standard deviation of MZ and DZ twinning rates by at least 2. Applying the ‘Fellman correction’ yields t(325) = 2.714, P = 0.00701.

Add Health (USA)

The proportion of SS pairs among DZ twins in the USA was 0.5739, which was significantly higher than the critical value of 0.5002 (t(520) = 3.398, P = 0.00073). Applying the Fellman correction yields t(520) = 2.403, P = 0.01660.

Discussion

The proportion of SS pairs among DZ twins was significantly higher than expected by chance in both NCDS in the UK and Add Health in the USA. These findings are consistent with our prediction that there are more SS than OS twins because OS twins are at a developmental disadvantage relative to SS twins, and there may be selection for the tendency to produce SS twins and against the tendency to produce OS twins. We hasten to add, however, that, while our empirical results are consistent with our evolutionary hypotheses, they do not provide direct evidence of the evolutionary selection that we posit. There may potentially be other mechanisms that produce a preponderance of SS pairs among DZ twins.

Further consistent with the evolutionary selection mechanism based on the Trivers–Willard hypothesis (Trivers and Willard, 1973), the analysis of the Add Health data showed that, in a sample of SS twins (n = 511), controlling for zygosity, mother’s education (as an indicator of social class) significantly predicted the sex of the twin (b = 0.110, P = 0.011). Mothers with more education were significantly more likely to have male SS twins and mothers with less education were significantly more likely to have female SS twins. The same pattern was not evident in the NCDS data, however. The greater social welfare system in the UK compared to in the USA might account for the national difference and why social class has a smaller impact on the offspring sex in the UK. There is a greater degree of income transfer from the wealthy to the poor, and thus the class differences by education are less pronounced, in the UK than in the USA.

A potential limitation of our data is that zygosity was assessed by the respondent’s mother, which is not always accurate (Segal, 2011). If many MZ twins, who are always SS, are misclassified as DZ, then it would artificially inflate the proportion of SS pairs among DZ twins, as the proportion of OS pairs is always accurate because they cannot be misclassified as MZ. However, both the DNA analysis and questionnaire data showed a very small departure from mothers’ reports, increasing our confidence in the findings. Further, our calculations (Supplementary Data) show that our conclusion that there are more SS than OS pairs among DZ twins is extremely robust under a wide range of possible misclassifications of twins by zygosity.

Our results are inconsistent with earlier studies by Fellman and Eriksson (2006) and Kaprio et al. (2002). It is not immediately obvious what accounts for the divergent empirical findings. As noted above, their data come from Scandinavian and Nordic nations whereas ours come from Anglo-Saxon nations. Genetic and/or environmental differences between these nations might potentially account for the divergent findings. More research is necessary to explore and replicate the relative frequencies of SS and OS pairs among DZ twins with different samples.

The Trivers–Willard hypothesis has been supported in numerous species, such as the Venezuelan opossum (Austad and Sunquist, 1986), red deer (Clutton-Brock et al., 1986) and spider monkey (Symington, 1987), and mothers’ breastmilk appears to be tailored for the sex of the offspring in other species, such as macaques (Hinde, 2007, 2009; Hinde et al., 2013), cows (Hinde et al., 2014), Iberian red deer (Landete-Castillejos et al., 2005), bank voles (Koskela et al., 2009) and tammar wallabies (Robert and Braun, 2012). We would therefore predict that there are higher-than-expected proportions of SS versus OS twins and multiple births in nonhuman species as well. At the same time, more human data are clearly necessary to establish whether there is indeed an excess of SS pairs among DZ twins.

Supplementary data

Supplementary data are available at Human Reproduction online.

Acknowledgements

We thank Jay Belsky, Johan Fellman, William H. James, Jaakko Kaprio, Lauren A. Milligan, Joseph L. Nedelec, Jena Pincott, Jeroen Smits, Emily Tammam and Glenn E. Weisfeld for their comments on earlier drafts.

Authors’ role

S.K. conducted the data analyses and wrote the first draft. S.K. and N.L.S. contributed to revisions. D.d.M. proposed the original idea and performed the calculations presented in the Supplementary Data.

Funding

None.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Austad
SN
,
Sunquist
ME
.
Sex ratio manipulation in the common opossum
.
Nature
1986
;
324
:
58
60
.

Bertillon
J
.
Des combinaisons de sexe dans les grossesses gémellaires (doubles ou triples), de leur cause et de leur caractère ethnique
.
Bull Soc Anthropol Paris
1874
;
9
:
267
290
.

Cederlöf
R
,
Friberg
L
,
Jonsson
E
,
Kaij
L
.
Studies on similarity diagnosis in twins with the aid of mailed questionnaires
.
Acta Genet Stat Med
1961
;
11
:
338
362
.

Clutton-Brock
TH
,
Albon
DD
,
Guinness
FE
.
Great expectations: maternal dominance, sex ratios and offspring reproductive success in red deer
.
Anim Behav
1986
;
34
:
460
471
.

Fellman
J
.
Statistical analyses of monozygotic and dizygotic twinning rates
.
Twin Res Hum Genet
2013
;
16
:
1107
1111
.

Fellman
J
,
Eriksson
AW
.
Weinberg’s differential rule reconsidered
.
Hum Biol
2006
;
78
:
253
275
.

Fellman
J
,
Eriksson
AW
.
Secondary sex ratio in multiple births
.
Twin Res Hum Genet
2010
;
13
:
101
108
.

Frazier
TW
,
Thompson
L
,
Youngstrum
EA
,
Law
P
,
Hardan
EY
,
Eng
C
,
Morris
N
.
A twin study of heritable and shared environmental contributions to autism
.
J Autism Dev Disord
2014
;
44
:
2013
2025
.

Fujita
M
,
Roth
E
,
Lo
Y-J
,
Hurst
C
,
Vollner
J
,
Kendell
A
.
In poor families, mothers’ milk is richer for daughters than sons: a test of Trivers-Willard hypothesis in agropastoral settlements in northern Kenya
.
Am J Phys Anthropol
2012
;
149
:
52
59
.

Harris
KM
,
Halpern
CT
,
Smolen
A
,
Haberstick
BC
.
The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) twin data
.
Twin Res Hum Genet
2006
;
9
:
988
997
.

Hinde
K
.
First-time macaque mothers bias milk composition in favor of sons
.
Curr Biol
2007
;
17
:
R958
R959
.

Hinde
K
.
Richer milk for sons but more milk for daughters: sex-biased investment during lactation varies with maternal life history in rhesus macaques
.
Am J Hum Biol
2009
;
21
:
512
519
.

Hinde
K
,
Carpenter
AJ
,
Clay
JS
,
Bradford
BJ
.
Holsteins favor heifers, not bulls: biased milk production programmed during pregnancy as a function of fetal sex
.
PLoS One
2014
;
9
:
e86169
.

Hinde
K
,
Foster
AB
,
Landis
LM
,
Rendina
D
,
Oftedal
OT
,
Power
ML
.
Daughter dearest: sex-biased calcium in mother’s milk among rhesus macaques
.
Am J Phys Anthropol
2013
;
151
:
144
150
.

James
WH
.
Excess of like sexed pairs of dizygotic twins
.
Nature
1971
;
232
:
277
278
.

James
WH
.
Is Weinberg’s differential rule valid?
Acta Genet Med Gemmellol
1979
;
28
:
69
71
.

James
WH
.
Hormonal control of sex ratio
.
J Theor Biol
1986
;
180
:
427
441
.

Kanazawa
S
,
Segal
NL
.
Same-sex twins are taller and heavier than opposite-sex twins (but only if breastfed): possible evidence for sex bias in human breast milk
.
J Exp Child Psychol
2017
;
156
:
186
191
.

Kaprio
J
,
Pulkkinen
L
,
Rose
RJ
.
Genetic and environmental factors in health-related behaviors: studies on Finnish twins and twin families
.
Twin Res
2002
;
5
:
366
371
.

Koskela
E
,
Mappes
T
,
Niskanen
T
,
Rutkowska
J
.
Maternal investment in relation to sex ratio and offspring number in a small mammal—a case for Trivers and Willard theory?
J Anim Ecol
2009
;
78
:
1007
1014
.

Landete-Castillejos
T
,
García
A
,
López-Serrano
FR
,
Gallego
L
.
Maternal quality and differences in milk production and composition for male and female Iberian red deer calves (Cervus elaphus hispanicus)
.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol
2005
;
57
:
267
274
.

Lykken
DT
,
McGue
M
,
Tellegen
A
.
Recruitment bias in twin research: the rule of two-thirds reconsidered
.
Behav Genet
1987
;
17
:
343
362
.

Lykken
DT
,
Tellegen
A
,
DeRubeis
R
.
Volunteer bias in twin research: the rule of two-thirds
.
Soc Biol
1978
;
25
:
1
9
.

Pison
G
,
Monden
C
,
Smits
J
.
Twinning rates in developed countries: trends and explanations
.
Popul Dev Rev
2015
;
41
:
629
649
.

Powe
CE
,
Knott
CD
,
Conklin-Brittain
N
.
Infant sex predicts breast milk energy content
.
Am J Hum Biol
2010
;
22
:
50
54
.

Quinn
EA
.
No evidence for sex biases in milk macronutrients, energy, or breastfeeding frequency in a sample of Filipino mothers
.
Am J Phys Anthropol
2013
;
152
:
209
216
.

Robert
KA
,
Braun
S
.
Milk composition during lactation suggests a mechanism for male biased allocation of maternal resources in the tammar wallaby (Macropus eugenii)
.
PLoS One
2012
;
7
:
e51099
.

Segal
NL
.
Zygosity assessment by self-report: research implications
.
Twin Res Hum Genet
2011
;
14
:
104
106
.

Segal
NL
.
Born Together–Reared Apart: The Landmark Minnesota Twin Study
.
Cambridge
:
Harvard University Press
,
2012
.

Segal
NL
.
Twin Mythconceptions: False Beliefs, Fables, and Facts About Twins
.
London
:
Academic Press
,
2017
.

Symington
MM
.
Sex ratio and maternal rank in wild spider monkeys: when daughters disperse
.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol
1987
;
20
:
421
425
.

Thakkar
SK
,
Giuffrida
F
,
Cruz-Hernandez
C
,
de Castro
CA
,
Mukherjee
R
,
Tran
L-A
,
Destaillats
F
.
Dynamics of human milk nutrient composition of women from Singapore with a special focus on lipids
.
Am J Hum Biol
2013
;
25
:
770
779
.

Torgersen
S
.
The determination of twin zygosity by means of a mailed questionnaire
.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol
1979
;
28
:
225
236
.

Trivers
RL
,
Willard
DE
.
Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring
.
Science
1973
;
179
:
90
92
.

Weinberg
W
.
Beiträge zur Physiologie und Pathologie der Mehrlingsgeburten beim Menschen
.
Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere
1901
;
88
:
346
430
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/about_us/legal/notices)

Supplementary data