Abstract

Mood is conceptualized as a long-lasting emotional state, which can have profound implications for mental and physical health. The development of neuroimaging methods has enabled significant advances towards elucidating the mechanisms underlying regulation of mood and emotion; however, our understanding of mood and emotion dysregulation in stress-related psychiatric disorders is still largely lacking. From the cognitive-affective neuroscience perspective, achieving deeper, more mechanistic understanding of mood disorders necessitates detailed understanding of specific components of neural systems involved in mood dysregulation and stabilization. In this review, we provide an overview of neural systems implicated in the development of a long-term negative mood state, as well as those related to emotion and emotion regulation, and discuss their proposed involvement in mood and anxiety disorders.

Introduction

Accumulating evidence suggests that mood states in general, and dysregulated mood states in particular, can have a profound impact on mental and physical health. Major depressive disorder (MDD) for example, commonly conceptualized as a disorder of mood dysregulation is a leading cause of worldwide disability (McKenna et al.2005). Yet, our current understanding of the mechanisms underlying mood dysregulation and stabilization is still very limited.

In the past decade, research in emotional cognitive neuroscience has begun to outline the neurocircuitry involved in higher level processes that regulate emotion (Critchley, 2005; Ochsner & Gross, 2005), and recent studies suggest that MDD patients have difficulties in recruiting brain regions involved in cognitive control and regulation of emotions (Erk et al.2010; Johnstone et al.2007). However, the link between failure to properly regulate emotional responses and mood disorder psychopathology has not been firmly established, and failed emotional regulation has also been associated with various forms of anxiety disorders (Amstadter, 2008), suggesting that failure to appropriately regulate activity in the emotion-processing networks under stressful circumstances can be seen in stress-related psychiatric disorders in general. Shared symptoms and high comorbidity repeatedly found between mood disorders and anxiety disorders (de Graaf et al.2002; Kessler et al.2005) further support the notion of potentially shared mechanisms of psychopathology. However, from the cognitive-emotional neuroscience perspective a deeper, more mechanistic understanding of MDD and of other related psychopathologies, e.g. anxiety disorders, requires delineation of the specific roles of various components of complex neural systems involved in mood and emotion regulation.

In this paper we review existing findings regarding neural mechanisms implicated in mood dysregulation and stabilization, with emphasis on these linked to the development of the most common manifestation of mood dysregulation – depression. We first consider the constructs of emotion and mood, and review recent human neuroimaging studies that examined the neurocircuitry of emotion and emotion regulation, and that of mood and mood instability. Next, we discuss the neuroimaging literature that examined recent approaches aiming to enhance emotion modulation and to improve mood symptoms. Throughout, we also attempt to highlight evidence that points towards the differences and the commonality in the neurocircuitry implicated in MDD and other stress-related psychiatric disorders.

Emotion and mood

Although the terms emotion and mood are frequently used interchangeably, and both denote an affective/motivational state, it is necessary here to clarify the distinction between the constructs of mood and emotion in considering the mechanisms of mood dysregulation and stabilization. In this review, we adopt a convention that has been used extensively in the literature to distinguish between these two terms, according to which, ‘emotion’ is understood as having a character of reactivity, usually brief, intense and circumscribed, related to a specific environmental event (Ekman, 1999). On the other hand, mood is conceived as a more stable and constant characteristic, with a tendency to be more comprehensive and not as linked to specific circumstances (Ellis & Moore, 1999). The usefulness of this distinction can be immediately evident when considering the potential differences in the neurocircuitry involved or in the psychopathology linked to these constructs. As a transient, cue-linked affective state, emotion-regulation deficits are likely to contribute to emotional arousal and exaggerated affect e.g. acute fear often present in the anxiety disorders, while mood dysregulation is more likely to lead to chronic, sustained negative moods, pervasive sadness or anhedonia, that are a hallmarks of mood disorders like MDD. This in turn, suggests that if indeed different neural mechanisms are involved in emotional modulation and mood dysregulation, understanding these differences will lead to better understanding of both common and distinct neurocircuitry involved in the psychopathology of MDD and anxiety disorders.

It is important, however, to keep in mind that while the proposed distinction is useful to highlight the differences between the two constructs, moods and emotions are generally viewed as closely interconnected. For example, moods are thought to facilitate emotional reactivity to mood-congruent stimuli. In line with this concept, a recent study demonstrated that induction of depressed mood disrupts the neurocircuitry of emotion regulation and enhances pain unpleasantness (Berna et al.2010). This result is consistent with our own findings, demonstrating that sadness enhances the experience of pain via neural activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and amygdala (Yoshino et al.2010). Interestingly, the same relationship might not hold in the presence of overt psychopathology; a recent meta-analysis indicated that depressed individuals in general report a reduced emotional response to negative stimuli (Bylsma et al.2008). Although depressive mood should potentiate negative emotional reactivity by the logic of mood facilitation, this seems to be not necessarily the case in MDD patients who have excessive negative mood. This meta-analysis as well as a recent study (Ellis et al.2009) support the emotion context insensitivity hypothesis (Rottenberg et al.2005) which predicts that individuals experiencing sad mood will show diminished reactivity to emotionally evocative stimuli and will not differentiate emotional responses across contexts. Finally, in addition to mood affecting emotional responses, transient emotional response could also affect long-term mood development. It is intuitively plausible that mood states reflect to some degree a cumulative function of experience of shorter term, emotional episodes. It is interesting to note here that, as we discuss later, it is possible that specific cognitive styles or biases are particularly relevant for both experiencing frequent negative emotion and less positive emotion, and also for the development of a longer term negative mood state seen in MDD.

Neurocircuitry of emotion and emotion regulation

The neurocircuitry of emotion has been extensively studied using functional neuroimaging approaches (Phan et al.2002, 2004b). Among the brain regions linked to emotions, the amygdala is the region that most reliably activates to stimuli that predict threat, implicating its involvement in fear and anxiety states (Etkin, 2010). For example, positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have reported amygdala activation in response to emotionally negative pictures/photographs (Britton et al.2006; Hariri et al.2002; Irwin et al.1996; Lane et al.1997; Paradiso et al.1999; Phan et al.2003; Reiman et al.1997; Taylor et al.1998), odours (Zald & Pardo, 1997), and tastes (Zald et al.1998). In concert with this proposed role, abnormalities in amygdala response have been reported in many stress-related psychiatric disorders. For example, exaggerated amygdala activation is the consistent finding in anxiety disorders, e.g. post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), social phobia, and specific phobia (Shin & Liberzon, 2010). In addition, fMRI studies have also reported excessive amygdala activation to negatively valenced stimuli in acutely depressed patients (Davidson et al.2003; Fu et al.2004; Sheline et al.2001; Surguladze et al.2005), which normalizes following antidepressant treatment (Fu et al.2004; Sheline et al.2001).

Emotional responses, however, do not occur independently of attentional resources, cognitive context, strategic goals and other complex brain functions that can influence and modify these responses. The capacity to modulate emotional responses by attention or cognitive goals has been termed ‘emotion regulation’ encompassing different types of regulatory processes that can control the physiological, behavioural, and experimental components of affective responses (Gross & Thompson, 2007). Deficits in regulatory capacity of emotions, rather than abnormal activation of primary regions involved in the generation of emotional responses could also contribute to abnormal emotional responses, expressed in exaggerated amygdala activation. The regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) have most consistently been implicated in cognitive control processes, including emotion regulation. For example, numerous fMRI studies have observed increases in activities in the ventrolateral, dorsolateral, and dorsomedial prefrontal cortices (vlPFC, dlPFC, and dmPFC) when participants were instructed to deploy cognitive strategies such as reappraisal that reduce negative emotional experience (Ochsner & Gross, 2005). However, the precise location and laterality of these PFC regions vary among studies, perhaps because of subtle differences in stimuli, emotions, or strategy used (Ochsner & Gross, 2008). In spite of these differences, the dlPFC is generally implicated in the effortful manipulation or interpretation of the stimulus (Delgado et al.2008; Ochsner et al.2002). Reduced dlPFC activation during active emotion regulation has been reported in MDD (Erk et al.2010) and social anxiety disorder (SAD) patients (Goldin et al.2009). A recent study also suggests that such reduced dlPFC activation during active regulation is associated with a lack of sustained emotion regulation effect seen in MDD (Erk et al.2010).

The vmPFC and hippocampus are also implicated in regulation of amygdala activity. The example is reported in the process of extinction learning (Quirk & Mueller, 2008). As in other types of learning, extinction occurs in three phases: acquisition, consolidation, and retrieval. Following the animal literature, recent human imaging studies distinguish between extinction acquisition and extinction retrieval by examining subjects both during extinction training as well as 24 h later (Delgado et al.2006; Rauch et al.2006). During extinction retrieval, several studies have reported significant activation of the vmPFC (Kalisch et al.2006; Milad et al.2007; Phelps et al.2004). Furthermore, Milad et al. observed that the amount of extinction retrieval was highly correlated with vmPFC activity and vmPFC thickness (Milad et al.2005). The hippocampus is also activated during extinction retrieval in studies that manipulate context (Kalisch et al.2006; Milad et al.2007), suggesting that a prefrontal-hippocampal network is involved in contextual modulation of extinction. Consistent with the idea that PTSD is related to the failure to consolidate and retrieve memory for extinction, PTSD patients exhibit deficits in extinction retention (Orr et al.2000), along with reduced vmPFC and hippocampal volume and activity, and increased amygdala activity (Bremner, 2006; Gilbertson et al.2002; Liberzon & Martis, 2006; Shin et al.2006).

To fully understand the neurocircuitry implicated in emotions and emotion regulation, data from anatomical connectivity studies have to be considered. Structural connectivity studies suggest that the dlPFC does not project directly to the amygdala (Barbas, 2000; McDonald et al.1996), and its influence on the amygdala is thought to be mediated by vmPFC (Hartley & Phelps, 2010). Recently, the idea that the lateral PFC regions engaged by cognitive emotion regulation strategies may influence the amygdala, diminishing fear through similar vmPFC connections that are thought to inhibit the amygdala during extinction have been proposed and demonstrated by identifying an overlapping region of the vmPFC across these techniques for diminishing fear (Delgado et al.2008). These results are consistent with the suggestion that vmPFC may play a general regulatory role in diminishing fear across a range of paradigms (Kim et al.2003; Urry et al.2006). Interestingly, the data from a recent study (Johnstone et al.2007) suggest the inappropriate engagement of lateral PFC-vmPFC-amygdala inhibitory circuitry during efforts to reappraise negative emotional stimuli might also be one of the factors involved in MDD pathophysiology.

Neurocircuitry of mood and mood instability

Although MDD is a complex set of symptoms, a profound change in mood is its most characteristic feature. MDD thus provides a rich context for exploring the neurocircuitry of mood and mood instability.

The area most reproducibly implicated in MDD is the subgenual ACC (sgACC). This region was initially shown to display an MDD-associated reduction in blood flow and glucose metabolism, with a corresponding reduction in grey-matter volume of the left sgACC (Drevets et al.1997). Since this initial report, reduced sgACC volume has been repeatedly replicated (Drevets et al.2008). On the other hand, imaging studies that assessed sgACC activity controlling for partial volume effects, indicated increased resting glucose metabolism or blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) activity in the sgACC and inflalimbic cortex of depressed patients (Inagaki et al.2007; Kumano et al.2007; Mah et al.2007). In line with these data, sgACC metabolism and cerebral blood flow are higher in the depressed, ummedicated phase vs. the remitted phase in MDD subjects (Drevets et al.2002; Hasler et al.2008; Mayberg et al.1999; Neumeister et al.2004). These data are consistent with observations that experimentally induced sadness increases regional blood flow in the sgACC (George et al.1995; Mayberg et al.1999). Furthermore, various MDD treatments, including antidepressant treatment (Holthoff et al.2004; Mayberg et al.2000), electroconvulsive therapy (Nobler et al.2001), and deep-brain stimulation of the sgACC (Mayberg et al.2005), lead to decreased activity of the sgACC following treatment. Although ample literature implicates sgACC as a critical structure in MDD pathology as mentioned above, it is important to emphasize that other parts of the ACC, e.g. dorsal ACC (dACC), commonly implicated in conflict monitoring (Botvinick, 2007; Yeung et al.2004), might also be involved in MDD pathophysiology (Davidson et al.2002). For example, decreased dACC activation has been repeatedly reported with neuroimaging techniques (Bench et al.1992; George et al.1997; Holmes & Pizzagalli, 2008), and evidence suggests that MDD patients might display conflict monitoring dysregulation in paradigms generating competition among response options (Ottowitz et al.2002). These deficits might be related to MDD symptomatology such as indecisiveness. Interestingly, antidepressant effects of deep-brain stimulation on sgACC have been reported to be associated with not only decreased blood flow in sgACC, but also increased blood flow in the downstream areas including dACC (Mayberg et al.2005).

In addition to pervasive sadness, MDD seems also to involve diminished activation of positive emotion. Anhedonia, or loss of pleasure or interest in previously rewarding stimuli, is seen as one of the core features of MDD (APA, 2000). Thus, MDD could involve failure to activate a positive emotional response in the appropriate context, or difficulty in sustaining a response involving positive emotion, in addition or as an alternative to abnormal regulation of negative mood states. As for the neural substrates, the nucleus accumbens (NAc), and fronto-striatal network have been implicated in reward processing (Knutson & Cooper, 2005; Knutson & Wimmer, 2007; Wise, 2002) and positive emotion regulation (Kim & Hamann, 2007). A recent study (Heller et al.2009) suggested that patients with MDD suffer from an inability to sustain reward-related activity that is reflected in the fronto-striatal network across time, and that this deficit is associated with reduced positive affect. Interestingly, the neurotransmitter which is most strongly linked to reward processing is dopamine. Previous research indicated that midbrain dopaminergic neurons show a pattern of signalling the magnitude, delay and probability of rewards (Roesch et al.2007; Schultz, 2007) and code negative motivation and aversive events (Matsumoto & Hikosaka, 2009), while MDD has been associated with abnormalities in dopaminergic function in some studies (Kapur & Mann, 1992; Nestler & Carlezon, 2006). However, for many years research into the pathophysiology of MDD focused mainly on the serotonergic system because of the efficacy of most commonly prescribed antidepressants – selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). Future research will have to further clarify dopamine's role in the pathophysiology of MDD. Recent reports of serotonin involvement in reward processing (Kranz et al.2010) further support the possible involvement of reward system abnormalities in the psychopathology of MDD. Although complex interactions between neuromodulators like serotonin and dopamine in reward systems exist, a computational theory proposed that serotonin controls the time scale of reward prediction, and that the increased rate of discounting future reward value may explain certain aspects of depressive behaviour: when future rewards have values near zero, the optimal strategy is not to act (Doya, 2002). We studied brain activations linked to the choice of delayed reward, and found increased activity in the dorsal raphe nucleus, which is the serotonergic nucleus and provides a substantial proportion of serotonin's innervation to the forebrain (Tanaka et al.2004). Considering that serotonin appears to play a major role in MDD, future experiments using delayed reward paradigms could be designed to further examine potential pathophysiology in patients with MDD.

Finally, as stated earlier, in the case of humans neither emotional nor mood states exist outside the cognitive context, and thus cognition can serve as a potent modulator of mood, and cognitive styles and biases can contribute to both the development and the alleviation of long-term negative mood states seen in MDD. Per definition, negative view of the self, of the world, and of the future, as well as recurrent and uncontrollable negative thoughts that often revolve around the self, are debilitating symptoms of depression (APA, 2000). At the same time, one of the most effective interventions for depression, cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), focuses on modifying biased interpretations and dysfunctional automatic thoughts and proposes that changes in cognition will lead to improvement of negative mood (Beck, 1976). Understanding the neurocircuitry involved in the modulatory effects of cognition on mood, e.g. negative view of the future and self, is important in understanding the neurocircuitry of mood and mood instability.

In this context, negative view of the future may be conceptualized as the enhanced anticipation of negative events. Functional neuroimaging studies, including ours, have been used to study the neurocircuitry of this phenomenon. Most of such studies have employed emotional expectancy cues that can be characterized as instantiating high levels of certainty with regard to the emotional valence of forthcoming pictures. Using this methodology, the expectation of a negative event induced activation in multiple prefrontal regions including the dlPFC (Nitschke et al.2006), vlPFC (Herwig et al.2007a, 2007b; Simmons et al.2004; Ueda et al.2003), mPFC (Ueda et al.2003), OFC (Nitschke et al.2006), ACC (Bermpohl et al.2006; Herwig et al.2007a, 2007b; Nitschke et al.2006; Ueda et al.2003), amygdala (Nitschke et al.2006; Ueda et al.2003) and insula (Nitschke et al.2006; Simmons et al.2004, 2006). Recently, using fMRI, we studied anticipation of negative condition, and demonstrated that ACC modulates preparatory activation for the coming negative event (Onoda et al.2008). Although such hypervigilance to impending threat can be adaptive by virtue of allowing an individual to prepare for and prevent aversive outcomes, it may also lead to experiencing frequent negative emotions and developing a longer term negative mood state. Although there are few studies examining neural activation associated with anticipation of emotional stimuli in MDD at the present, such studies appear to be important in understanding the potential role of cognitions in mood dysregulation.

Negative view of the self is one of the key cognition biases seen in MDD. Researchers have reliably demonstrated a depression-related bias in the self-referential processing of the negative emotional stimuli (Banos et al.2001; Bradley & Mathews, 1983; Derry & Kuiper, 1981; Dobson & Shaw, 1987). In addition, other work suggested that a negative self-focus can increase negative emotionality in both healthy controls (Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1987) and depressed patients (Ingram, 1990). Recent neuroimaging studies of healthy participants have implicated medial PFC (mPFC) in self-referential processing (Fossati et al.2003, 2004; Kelley et al.2002; Northoff & Bermpohl, 2004; Northoff et al.2006). In addition, imaging studies of self-referential encoding tasks indicate that the mPFC, the ACC and the amygdala are activated during the processing of emotional information (Fossati et al.2003, 2004; Gusnard et al.2001; Phan et al.2004a). Our study of healthy participants (Yoshimura et al.2009) also found activation in both the mPFC and rostral ACC (rACC) during the self-referential processing of negative emotional words. However, little is known about the brain function that underlies negative views of the self in depressive patients. Recently, we examined brain activation associated with the self-referential processing of negative emotional stimuli in MDD (Yoshimura et al.2010). Compared to normal controls, depressed patients showed hyperactivity in the mPFC and rACC during the self-referential processing of negative words (Fig. 1). Furthermore, the activity in these regions during self-referential processing was correlated with depressive symptom severity, and rACC activity mediated the correlation between mPFC activity and depressive symptoms. Increased functional connectivity between the rACC, mPFC, and amygdala, was found in MDD, relative to control participants, suggesting that the relationship between the mPFC, rACC, and amygdala might reflect the interaction between self-referential and negative emotional information processing in the development of depressive symptoms.

Brain activation of MDD and normal controls performing a self-referential task using positive and negative emotional words as stimuli (Yoshimura et al.2010). Axial sections display (a) the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and (b) rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), showing significant effect (second-order interaction from three-way ANOVA). Clusters of activity are overlaid on T1-weighted anatomical images. Graphs to the right of each image display signal change (parameter estimates) across each group and condition, relative to the control condition (uncorrected p<0.001, ⩾10 voxels). The light blue bar corresponds to the self-positive condition, the dark blue bar to the self-negative condition, the orange bar to the other-positive condition, and the red bar to the other-negative condition.
Fig. 1

Brain activation of MDD and normal controls performing a self-referential task using positive and negative emotional words as stimuli (Yoshimura et al.2010). Axial sections display (a) the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and (b) rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), showing significant effect (second-order interaction from three-way ANOVA). Clusters of activity are overlaid on T1-weighted anatomical images. Graphs to the right of each image display signal change (parameter estimates) across each group and condition, relative to the control condition (uncorrected p<0.001, ⩾10 voxels). The light blue bar corresponds to the self-positive condition, the dark blue bar to the self-negative condition, the orange bar to the other-positive condition, and the red bar to the other-negative condition.

Approaches to enhance emotional modulation

Although the ability to respond emotionally to salient cues is critical for adaptive human function, our ability to modify or control the nature of our emotional responses as circumstances change is equally important. Recent attention has been given to the role of such emotion regulation in the development and maintenance of stress-related psychiatric disorders. Indeed, there is an emerging consensus linking emotional dysregulation with depression (Ochsner & Gross, 2007), and with anxiety disorders (Amstadter, 2008). As a result, emotion regulation training is commonly included, explicitly or implicitly in CBT (Berking et al.2008) which is effective for anxiety disorders (Hofmann & Smits, 2008) and MDD (Simons et al.1986). Although at this point little is known about the specific emotion-regulation strategies or abilities that can be enhanced to effectively treat all stress-related psychiatric disorders, anxiety disorders in particular might involve dysfunction in the extinction of fear learning (Rauch et al.2006), and are effectively treated by extinction-based exposure therapies (Foa, 2006; Garakani et al.2006; Rothbaum & Schwartz, 2002). Recent research suggests that facilitation of extinction learning through pharmacological means may enhance the efficacy of such extinction-based therapies (Anderson & Insel, 2006; Quirk & Mueller, 2008; Ressler et al.2004; Walker et al.2002). However, neural mechanisms involved in this type of facilitation are not well outlined and thus functional neuroimaging studies examining these processes are urgently needed.

Among the cognitive strategies to modulate emotion, reappraisal has proved effective for down-regulating intense negative emotions (Ochsner & Gross, 2004), and is proposed to mirror the cognitive processes used during CBT. One of the goals of CBT is to enable the patient to form more realistic evidence-based appraisals of situation, thereby regulating the associated emotional responses (Allen et al.2008). Such a process probably assumes abnormally high activation in the amygdala as a ‘generator’ of specific symptoms and relies on strengthening of the lateral PFC-vmPFC-amygdala inhibitory circuitry as described earlier. Consistent with this suggestion, a recent study reported that fMRI activation in response to fearful faces in the amygdala and sgACC a, a subregion of the vmPFC, predicts success of CBT in PTSD patients (Bryant et al.2008). The efficacy of such treatment may rely on the functional integrity of this neural circuitry and the success with which individuals are able to engage these regulatory mechanisms.

Approaches to improve mood symptoms

MDD is usually treated with either medication or an evidence-based psychotherapy. Among the medications, antidepressants which potentiate serotonin neurotransmission are the first-line treatement for MDD. Recently there has been growing interest in the neurotorophic actions of antidepressants, (Manji et al.2001), and it has been observed that SSRIs have a direct influence on adult neurogenesis (Jacobs et al.2000), particularly in the dentate gyrus within the hippocampus (Malberg et al.2000; Sheline et al.2003). Such actions may be able to reverse structural and cellular deficits associated with depression and may facilitate learning and memory processes in which serotonin has also been shown to have a central role (Meneses, 2003). It is important to emphasize that dopamine, implicated in reward processing as mentioned above and in memory processing (Takahashi et al.2008), might also be involved in the antidepressant action. Historically, amphetamines were one of the first agents used to combat symptoms of depression (Warneke, 1990). Indeed, several pharmacological agents that stimulate dopamine have antidepressant-like effects (Papakostas, 2006), and sertraline [arguably a more effective SSRI agent (Cipriani et al.2009)] increases the extracellular levels of not only of serotonin but also of dopamine in the NAc and striatum of rats (Kitaichi et al.2010). Serotonin and dopamine interact interdependently, and learning and memory depends at least in part on short- or long-lasting changes of synapses affected by the modulatory influence of serotonin and dopamine occurring at the synaptic level. Interestingly, it has recently been suggested that positive re-biasing of automatic processing produced by acute antidepressant treatment might, in an interpersonal environment, lead to changes in the strategic processing and behaviour associated with conscious emotional processing which becomes translated into improved mood (Harmer, 2008). In our own studies conducted under three different tryptophan conditions (depletion, Trp−; loading, Trp+; and control), which induced changes in total plasma tryptophan levels correlated with the cerebrospinal serotonin levels (Carpenter et al.1998; Williams et al.1999), we suggested that serotonin may adjust the rate of delayed reward discounting (Tanaka et al.2007; Schweighofer et al.2008). In these studies, we observed significant differences in activation of the striatum for reward prediction at different time scales that was modulated by serotonin level (Tanaka et al.2007) (Fig. 2). Thus, SSRIs may allow a different perspective for our ongoing evaluation of the future, at least in part through the improved ability to predict future reward. Such modulatory effect by serotonin and dopamine on learning and memory could play a role in improving mood, with the time delay factor that is also characteristic of the antidepressant effects of SSRI biases (Harmer, 2008). In other words, the therapeutic actions of antidepressants, at least partially, can be attributed to their effects on learning rather than direct effects on mood itself, and this mechanism can help to explain in part why the effects of antidepressants are seen some time after initiation of treatment. Interestingly, a recent study reported that behavioural activation therapy for MDD, results in improved functioning of reward neurocircuitry, including the dorsal striatum during reward anticipation (Dichter et al.2009).

Regression analysis of BOLD signal by expected future reward with different discount rates (Tanaka et al.2007). Voxels within the striatum (3D mesh surface) showing a significant correlation (p<0.001 in one-sample t test, uncorrected for the multiple comparison, n=12 subjects) with V(t) at different settings of γ are shown with colour codes (red: γ=0.6; orange: 0.7; yellow: 0.8; green: 0.9; cyan: 0.95; blue: 0.99). Red- to yellow-coded voxels, correlated with reward prediction at shorter scales, are predominantly located in the ventral part of the striatum (ventral putamen and nucleus accumbens), while the green- to blue-coded voxels, correlated with reward prediction at longer time scales, are located in the dorsal part of the striatum (dorsal putamen and caudate body). V(t) is the reward value at time t. γ is the discount factor, and a small γ (high discounting rate) leads to an inability to select a delayed reward over a smaller immediate reward.
Fig. 2

Regression analysis of BOLD signal by expected future reward with different discount rates (Tanaka et al.2007). Voxels within the striatum (3D mesh surface) showing a significant correlation (p<0.001 in one-sample t test, uncorrected for the multiple comparison, n=12 subjects) with V(t) at different settings of γ are shown with colour codes (red: γ=0.6; orange: 0.7; yellow: 0.8; green: 0.9; cyan: 0.95; blue: 0.99). Red- to yellow-coded voxels, correlated with reward prediction at shorter scales, are predominantly located in the ventral part of the striatum (ventral putamen and nucleus accumbens), while the green- to blue-coded voxels, correlated with reward prediction at longer time scales, are located in the dorsal part of the striatum (dorsal putamen and caudate body). V(t) is the reward value at time t. γ is the discount factor, and a small γ (high discounting rate) leads to an inability to select a delayed reward over a smaller immediate reward.

In addition to antidepressant medication, other treatments are also effective in improving mood symptoms of MDD. These include, CBT and other forms of psychotherapy, such as interpersonal therapy (de Mello et al.2005), electroconvulsive therapy (UKECT Review Group, 2003), electrical stimulation of the vagus nerve (Nahas et al.2005), and chronic stimulation of the sgACC (Mayberg et al.2005).

Among these treatments, CBT, along with antidepressant medication has been the focus of the most intensive research efforts, both with regard to the outcome it produces, and the mechanisms that might explain its effect. A recent review suggested that CBT effectively exercises the PFC, possibly yielding increased inhibitory function of this region, while antidepressant medications might target amygdala function more directly (DeRubeis et al.2008). CBT may bring about the conscious, volitional changes in the way that patients process emotion-relevant information, whereas antidepressants bring about the more automatic unconscious modulatory effects on learning, memory, and cognitive biases. This difference might play a role in the return of depressive symptoms after medication is stopped. It may explain, at least in part, the difference between CBT and antidepressant therapy in terms of their effects in the early and later phase of the illness such as the enduring effect of CBT (DeRubeis et al.2008). At a psychological level, the aim of CBT for depression can be seen as teaching patients to see things in a broader perspective and to incorporate more contexts into their analysis of emotional information. As proposed recently (Bar, 2009), restructuring the ability for broad perspective (and thus more associative processing) may be important in general to elicit improvements of mood symptoms. However, there is initial evidence that general emotional support may also beneficially alter an individual's appraisal of a potentially stressful event. Recently, we demonstrated that emotional support enhances PFC activity, which in turn may lead to a weakened affective response in the ACC (Onoda et al.2009). It is possible that ‘emotional support’ is perceived as positive social interaction, and social interaction has a well-established role in regulation of neuroendocrine stress response (Foley & Kirschbaum, 2010), that in turn has been implicated in MDD pathophysiology (Gold et al.2002; Holsboer, 2000).

Summary

In the current review, we briefly outlined neural regions implicated in the development of a long-term negative mood state, as well as those linked to emotions and emotional regulation, and discussed their potential relevance to the pathophysiology of mood and anxiety disorders. Summary observations we consider important for better understanding of the neural mechanisms of mood dysregulation and stabilization are as follows (for key brain regions see Fig. 3): (1) Exaggerated amygdala activation seems to be the consistent finding common for both mood and anxiety disorders. (2) A large and growing body of research implicates the ventromedial and dorsolateral sectors of the PFC as key neural substrates underlying emotion regulation. (3) Pharmacological agents that facilitate extinction learning or that facilitate improved ability to predict future reward could serve as adjuncts to CBT for anxiety disorders and MDD, respectively; however, functional neuroimaging studies examining effects of these strategies on brain activation patterns are urgently needed. (4) Involvement of the sgACC implicated in regulation of negative emotional state is especially prominent in neuroimaging studies of MDD. (5) In addition to suggested emotion regulation deficiency, diminished activation of positive emotion and negative cognitive biases appear to be the potential contributors to the development of a long-term negative mood state seen in MDD, implicating ventral striatum and ACC circuitry in these processes. (6) Approaches to restructure the ability for broad perspective, to alter self-referential thinking or even to change perception of social support may elicit improvements of mood symptoms by altering the activity of lateral and medial regions of PFC. We hope that the present review can help us to better understand the mechanisms of mood dysregulation and stabilization from the perspective of emotional-cognitive neuroscience.

Magnetic resonance imagines showing the key brain areas in emotion and mood regulation. (a) The hippocampus and amygdala; (b) the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC), rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and the dorsal raphe nucleus; (c) dorsal striatum and ventral striatum, (d) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC). Areas in yellow lettering are those we consider more relevant to long-term mood regulation and MDD pathophysiology than emotion regulation and anxiety disorder pathophysiology in this review.
Fig. 3

Magnetic resonance imagines showing the key brain areas in emotion and mood regulation. (a) The hippocampus and amygdala; (b) the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC), rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and the dorsal raphe nucleus; (c) dorsal striatum and ventral striatum, (d) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC). Areas in yellow lettering are those we consider more relevant to long-term mood regulation and MDD pathophysiology than emotion regulation and anxiety disorder pathophysiology in this review.

Acknowledgements

We thank our colleagues and collaborators including Shinpei Yoshimura, Atuo Yoshino, Keiichi Onoda, Kazutaka Ueda, Saori C. Tanaka, Nicolas Schweighofer, and Kenji Doya, for their critical contributions to the work described in this review.

Statement of Interest

None.

References

Allen
LB
McHugh
RK
Barlow
DH
(
2008
).
Emotional Disorders: a Unified Protcol
.
New York
:
Guilford Press
.

Amstadter
A
(
2008
).
Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders
.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders
22
,
211
221
.

Anderson
KC
Insel
TR
(
2006
).
The promise of extinction research for the prevention and treatment of anxiety disorders
.
Biological Psychiatry
60
,
319
321
.

APA
(
2000
).
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision
.
Washington DC
:
American Psychiatric Association
.

Banos
RM
Medina
PM
Pascual
J
(
2001
).
Explicit and implicit memory biases in depression and panic disorder
.
Behaviour Research and Therapy
39
,
61
74
.

Bar
M
(
2009
).
A cognitive neuroscience hypothesis of mood and depression
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
13
,
456
463
.

Barbas
H
(
2000
).
Connections underlying the synthesis of cognition, memory, and emotion in primate prefrontal cortices
.
Brain Research Bulletin
52
,
319
330
.

Beck
AT
(
1976
).
Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders
.
New York
:
International University Press
.

Bench
CJ
Friston
KJ
Brown
RG
Scott
LC
et al. (
1992
).
The anatomy of melancholia – focal abnormalities of cerebral blood flow in major depression
.
Psychological Medicine
22
,
607
615
.

Berking
M
Wupperman
P
Reichardt
A
Pejic
T
et al. (
2008
).
Emotion-regulation skills as a treatment target in psychotherapy
.
Behaviour Research and Therapy
46
,
1230
1237
.

Bermpohl
F
Pascual-Leone
A
Amedi
A
Merabet
LB
et al. (
2006
).
Dissociable networks for the expectancy and perception of emotional stimuli in the human brain
.
Neuroimage
30
,
588
600
.

Berna
C
Leknes
S
Holmes
EA
Edwards
RR
et al. (
2010
).
Induction of depressed mood disrupts emotion regulation neurocircuitry and enhances pain unpleasantness
.
Biological Psychiatry
67
,
1083
1090
.

Botvinick
MM
(
2007
).
Conflict monitoring and decision making: reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function
.
Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience
7
,
356
366
.

Bradley
B
Mathews
A
(
1983
).
Negative self-schemata in clinical depression
.
British Journal of Clinical Psychology
22
,
173
181
.

Bremner
JD
(
2006
).
Traumatic stress: effects on the brain
.
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience
8
,
445
461
.

Britton
JC
Phan
KL
Taylor
SF
Welsh
RC
et al. (
2006
).
Neural correlates of social and nonsocial emotions: an fMRI study
.
Neuroimage
31
,
397
409
.

Bryant
RA
Felmingham
K
Kemp
A
Das
P
et al. (
2008
).
Amygdala and ventral anterior cingulate activation predicts treatment response to cognitive behaviour therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder
.
Psychological Medicine
38
,
555
561
.

Bylsma
LM
Morris
BH
Rottenberg
J
(
2008
).
A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder
.
Clinical Psychology Review
28
,
676
691
.

Carpenter
LL
Anderson
GM
Pelton
GH
Gudin
JA
et al. (
1998
).
Tryptophan depletion during continuous CSF sampling in healthy human subjects
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
19
,
26
35
.

Cipriani
A
Furukawa
TA
Salanti
G
Geddes
JR
et al. (
2009
).
Comparative efficacy and acceptability of 12 new-generation antidepressants: a multiple-treatments meta-analysis
.
Lancet
373
,
746
758
.

Critchley
HD
(
2005
).
Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration
.
Journal of Comparative Neurology
493
,
154
166
.

Davidson
RJ
Irwin
W
Anderle
MJ
Kalin
NH
(
2003
).
The neural substrates of affective processing in depressed patients treated with venlafaxine
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
160
,
64
75
.

Davidson
RJ
Pizzagalli
D
Nitschke
JB
Putnam
K
(
2002
).
Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience
.
Annual Review of Psychology
53
,
545
574
.

de Graaf
R
Bijl
RV
Smit
F
Vollebergh
WA
et al. (
2002
).
Risk factors for 12-month comorbidity of mood, anxiety, and substance use disorders: findings from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
159
,
620
629
.

de Mello
MF
de Jesus Mari
J
Bacaltchuk
J
Verdeli
H
et al. (
2005
).
A systematic review of research findings on the efficacy of interpersonal therapy for depressive disorders
.
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience
255
,
75
82
.

Delgado
MR
Nearing
KI
Ledoux
JE
Phelps
EA
(
2008
).
Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction
.
Neuron
59
,
829
838
.

Delgado
MR
Olsson
A
Phelps
EA
(
2006
).
Extending animal models of fear conditioning to humans
.
Biological Psychology
73
,
39
48
.

Derry
PA
Kuiper
NA
(
1981
).
Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
90
,
286
297
.

DeRubeis
RJ
Siegle
GJ
Hollon
SD
(
2008
).
Cognitive therapy vs. medication for depression: treatment outcomes and neural mechanisms
.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
9
,
788
796
.

Dichter
GS
Felder
JN
Petty
C
Bizzell
J
et al. (
2009
).
The effects of psychotherapy on neural responses to rewards in major depression
.
Biological Psychiatry
66
,
886
897
.

Dobson
KS
Shaw
BF
(
1987
).
Specificity and stability of self-referent encoding in clinical depression
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
96
,
34
40
.

Doya
K
(
2002
).
Metalearning and neuromodulation
.
Neural Networks
15
,
495
506
.

Drevets
WC
Bogers
W
Raichle
ME
(
2002
).
Functional anatomical correlates of antidepressant drug treatment assessed using PET measures of regional glucose metabolism
.
European Neuropsychopharmacology
12
,
527
544
.

Drevets
WC
Price
JL
Simpson
JR
Todd
RD
et al. (
1997
).
Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders
.
Nature
386
,
824
827
.

Drevets
WC
Savitz
J
Trimble
M
(
2008
).
The subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in mood disorders
.
CNS Spectrums
13
,
663
681
.

Ekman
P
(
1999
).
Basic emotions
. In:
Dalgleish
T
Power
MJ
(Eds),
Handbook of Cognition and Emotion
(pp.
45
60
).
New York
:
John Wiley & Sons
.

Ellis
AJ
Beevers
CG
Wells
TT
(
2009
).
Emotional dysregulation in dysphoria: support for Emotion Context Insensitivity in response to performance-based feedback
.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
40
,
443
454
.

Ellis
HC
Moore
BA
(
1999
).
Mood and memory
. In:
Dalgleish
T
Power
MJ
(Eds),
Handbook of Cognition and Emotion
(pp.
191
210
).
New York
:
John Wiley & Sons
.

Erk
S
Mikschl
A
Stier
S
Ciaramidaro
A
et al. (
2010
).
Acute and sustained effects of cognitive emotion regulation in major depression
.
Journal of Neuroscience
30
,
15726
15734
.

Etkin
A
(
2010
).
Functional neuroanatomy of anxiety: a neural circuit perspective
.
Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences
2
,
251
277
.

Foa
EB
(
2006
).
Psychosocial therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder
.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
67
(
Suppl. 2
),
40
45
.

Foley
P
Kirschbaum
C
(
2010
).
Human hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis responses to acute psychosocial stress in laboratory settings
.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
35
,
91
96
.

Fossati
P
Hevenor
SJ
Graham
SJ
Grady
C
et al. (
2003
).
In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
160
,
1938
1945
.

Fossati
P
Hevenor
SJ
Lepage
M
Graham
SJ
et al. (
2004
).
Distributed self in episodic memory: neural correlates of successful retrieval of self-encoded positive and negative personality traits
.
Neuroimage
22
,
1596
1604
.

Fu
CH
Williams
SC
Cleare
AJ
Brammer
MJ
et al. (
2004
).
Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: a prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study
.
Archives of General Psychiatry
61
,
877
889
.

Garakani
A
Mathew
SJ
Charney
DS
(
2006
).
Neurobiology of anxiety disorders and implications for treatment
.
Mount Sinai Journal of Medicine
73
,
941
949
.

George
MS
Ketter
TA
Parekh
PI
Horwitz
B
et al. (
1995
).
Brain activity during transient sadness and happiness in healthy women
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
152
,
341
351
.

George
MS
Ketter
TA
Parekh
PI
Rosinsky
N
et al. (
1997
).
Blunted left cingulate activation in mood disorder subjects during a response interference task (the Stroop)
.
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
9
,
55
63
.

Gilbertson
MW
Shenton
ME
Ciszewski
A
Kasai
K
et al. (
2002
).
Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma
.
Nature Neuroscience
5
,
1242
1247
.

Gold
PW
Drevets
WC
Charney
DS
(
2002
).
New insights into the role of cortisol and the glucocorticoid receptor in severe depression
.
Biological Psychiatry
52
,
381
385
.

Goldin
PR
Manber
T
Hakimi
S
Canli
T
et al. (
2009
).
Neural bases of social anxiety disorder: emotional reactivity and cognitive regulation during social and physical threat
.
Archives of General Psychiatry
66
,
170
180
.

Gross
JJ
Thompson
RA
(
2007
).
Emotion regulation: conceptula foundations
. In:
Gross
JJ
(Ed.),
Handbook of Emotion Regulation
(pp.
3
24
).
New York
:
Guilford Press
.

Gusnard
DA
Akbudak
E
Shulman
GL
Raichle
ME
(
2001
).
Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
98
,
4259
4264
.

Hariri
AR
Tessitore
A
Mattay
VS
Fera
F
et al. (
2002
).
The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a comparison of faces and scenes
.
Neuroimage
17
,
317
323
.

Harmer
CJ
(
2008
).
Serotonin and emotional processing: does it help explain antidepressant drug action?
Neuropharmacology
55
,
1023
1028
.

Hartley
CA
Phelps
EA
(
2010
).
Changing fear: the neurocircuitry of emotion regulation
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
35
,
136
146
.

Hasler
G
Fromm
S
Carlson
PJ
Luckenbaugh
DA
et al. (
2008
).
Neural response to catecholamine depletion in unmedicated subjects with major depressive disorder in remission and healthy subjects
.
Archives of General Psychiatry
65
,
521
531
.

Heller
AS
Johnstone
T
Shackman
AJ
Light
SN
et al. (
2009
).
Reduced capacity to sustain positive emotion in major depression reflects diminished maintenance of fronto-striatal brain activation
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
106
,
22445
22450
.

Herwig
U
Baumgartner
T
Kaffenberger
T
Bruhl
A
et al. (
2007
a).
Modulation of anticipatory emotion and perception processing by cognitive control
.
Neuroimage
37
,
652
662
.

Herwig
U
Kaffenberger
T
Baumgartner
T
Jancke
L
(
2007
b).
Neural correlates of a ‘pessimistic’ attitude when anticipating events of unknown emotional valence
.
Neuroimage
34
,
848
858
.

Hofmann
SG
Smits
JA
(
2008
).
Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials
.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
69
,
621
632
.

Holmes
AJ
Pizzagalli
DA
(
2008
).
Response conflict and frontocingulate dysfunction in unmedicated participants with major depression
.
Neuropsychologia
46
,
2904
2913
.

Holsboer
F
(
2000
).
The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
23
,
477
501
.

Holthoff
VA
Beuthien-Baumann
B
Zundorf
G
Triemer
A
et al. (
2004
).
Changes in brain metabolism associated with remission in unipolar major depression
.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica
110
,
184
194
.

Inagaki
M
Yoshikawa
E
Kobayakawa
M
Matsuoka
Y
et al. (
2007
).
Regional cerebral glucose metabolism in patients with secondary depressive episodes after fatal pancreatic cancer diagnosis
.
Journal of Affective Disorders
99
,
231
236
.

Ingram
RE
(
1990
).
Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: review and a conceptual model
.
Psychological Bulletin
107
,
156
176
.

Irwin
W
Davidson
RJ
Lowe
MJ
Mock
BJ
et al. (
1996
).
Human amygdala activation detected with echo-planar functional magnetic resonance imaging
.
Neuroreport
7
,
1765
1769
.

Jacobs
BL
van Praag
H
Gage
FH
(
2000
).
Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry: a novel theory of depression
.
Molecular Psychiatry
5
,
262
269
.

Johnstone
T
van Reekum
CM
Urry
HL
Kalin
NH
et al. (
2007
).
Failure to regulate: counterproductive recruitment of top-down prefrontal-subcortical circuitry in major depression
.
Journal of Neuroscience
27
,
8877
8884
.

Kalisch
R
Korenfeld
E
Stephan
KE
Weiskopf
N
et al. (
2006
).
Context-dependent human extinction memory is mediated by a ventromedial prefrontal and hippocampal network
.
Journal of Neuroscience
26
,
9503
9511
.

Kapur
S
Mann
JJ
(
1992
).
Role of the dopaminergic system in depression
.
Biological Psychiatry
32
,
1
17
.

Kelley
WM
Macrae
CN
Wyland
CL
Caglar
S
et al. (
2002
).
Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
14
,
785
794
.

Kessler
RC
Chiu
WT
Demler
O
Merikangas
KR
et al. (
2005
).
Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication
.
Archives of General Psychiatry
62
,
617
627
.

Kim
H
Somerville
LH
Johnstone
T
Alexander
AL
et al. (
2003
).
Inverse amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to surprised faces
.
Neuroreport
14
,
2317
2322
.

Kim
SH
Hamann
S
(
2007
).
Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
19
,
776
798
.

Kitaichi
Y
Inoue
T
Nakagawa
S
Boku
S
et al. (
2010
).
Sertraline increases extracellular levels not only of serotonin, but also of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and striatum of rats
.
European Journal of Pharmacology
647
,
90
96
.

Knutson
B
Cooper
JC
(
2005
).
Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction
.
Current Opinion in Neurology
18
,
411
417
.

Knutson
B
Wimmer
GE
(
2007
).
Splitting the difference: how does the brain code reward episodes
?
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
1104
,
54
69
.

Kranz
GS
Kasper
S
Lanzenberger
R
(
2010
).
Reward and the serotonergic system
.
Neuroscience
166
,
1023
1035
.

Kumano
H
Ida
I
Oshima
A
Takahashi
K
et al. (
2007
).
Brain metabolic changes associated with predispotion to onset of major depressive disorder and adjustment disorder in cancer patients – a preliminary PET study
.
Journal of Psychiatric Research
41
,
591
599
.

Lane
RD
Fink
GR
Chau
PM
Dolan
RJ
(
1997
).
Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses
.
Neuroreport
8
,
3969
3972
.

Liberzon
I
Martis
B
(
2006
).
Neuroimaging studies of emotional responses in PTSD
.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
1071
,
87
109
.

Mah
L
Zarate
CA
Singh
J
Duan
YF
et al. (
2007
).
Regional cerebral glucose metabolic abnormalities in bipolar II depression
.
Biological Psychiatry
61
,
765
775
.

Malberg
JE
Eisch
AJ
Nestler
EJ
Duman
RS
(
2000
).
Chronic antidepressant treatment increases neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus
.
Journal of Neuroscience
20
,
9104
9110
.

Manji
HK
Drevets
WC
Charney
DS
(
2001
).
The cellular neurobiology of depression
.
Nature Medicine
7
,
541
547
.

Matsumoto
M
Hikosaka
O
(
2009
).
Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals
.
Nature
459
,
837
841
.

Mayberg
HS
Brannan
SK
Tekell
JL
Silva
JA
et al. (
2000
).
Regional metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression: serial changes and relationship to clinical response
.
Biological Psychiatry
48
,
830
843
.

Mayberg
HS
Liotti
M
Brannan
SK
McGinnis
S
et al. (
1999
).
Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
156
,
675
682
.

Mayberg
HS
Lozano
AM
Voon
V
McNeely
HE
et al. (
2005
).
Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression
.
Neuron
45
,
651
660
.

McDonald
AJ
Mascagni
F
Guo
L
(
1996
).
Projections of the medial and lateral prefrontal cortices to the amygdala: a Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin study in the rat
.
Neuroscience
71
,
55
75
.

McKenna
MT
Michaud
CM
Murray
CJ
Marks
JS
(
2005
).
Assessing the burden of disease in the United States using disability-adjusted life years
.
American Journal of Preventive Medicine
28
,
415
423
.

Meneses
A
(
2003
).
A pharmacological analysis of an associative learning task: 5-HT(1) to 5-HT(7) receptor subtypes function on a pavlovian/instrumental autoshaped memory
.
Learning and Memory
10
,
363
372
.

Milad
MR
Quinn
BT
Pitman
RK
Orr
SP
et al. (
2005
).
Thickness of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in humans is correlated with extinction memory
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
102
,
10706
10711
.

Milad
MR
Wright
CI
Orr
SP
Pitman
RK
et al. (
2007
).
Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert
.
Biological Psychiatry
62
,
446
454
.

Nahas
Z
Marangell
LB
Husain
MM
Rush
AJ
et al. (
2005
).
Two-year outcome of vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for treatment of major depressive episodes
.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
66
,
1097
1104
.

Nestler
EJ
Carlezon
WA
(
2006
).
The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression
.
Biological Psychiatry
59
,
1151
1159
.

Neumeister
A
Nugent
AC
Waldeck
T
Geraci
M
et al. (
2004
).
Neural and behavioral responses to tryptophan depletion in unmedicated patients with remitted major depressive disorder and controls
.
Archives of General Psychiatry
61
,
765
773
.

Nitschke
JB
Sarinopoulos
I
Mackiewicz
KL
Schaefer
HS
et al. (
2006
).
Functional neuroanatomy of aversion and its anticipation
.
Neuroimage
29
,
106
116
.

Nobler
MS
Oquendo
MA
Kegeles
LS
Malone
KM
et al. (
2001
).
Decreased regional brain metabolism after ect
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
158
,
305
308
.

Northoff
G
Bermpohl
F
(
2004
).
Cortical midline structures and the self
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
8
,
102
107
.

Northoff
G
Heinzel
A
de Greck
M
Bermpohl
F
et al. (
2006
).
Self-referential processing in our brain–a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self
.
Neuroimage
31
,
440
457
.

Ochsner
KN
Bunge
SA
Gross
JJ
Gabrieli
JD
(
2002
).
Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion
.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
14
,
1215
1229
.

Ochsner
KN
Gross
JJ
(
2004
).
Thinking makes it so: a social cognitive neuroscience approach to emotion regulation
. In:
Baumeister
RF
Vohs
KD
(Eds),
Handbook of Self-regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications
(pp.
229
255
).
New York
:
The Guilford Press
.

Ochsner
KN
Gross
JJ
(
2005
).
The cognitive control of emotion
.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
9
,
242
249
.

Ochsner
KN
Gross
JJ
(
2007
).
The neural architecture of emotion regulation
. In:
Gross
JJ
(Ed.),
Handbook of Emotion Regulation
(pp.
87
109
).
New York
:
The Guilford Press
.

Ochsner
KN
Gross
JJ
(
2008
).
Cognitive emotion regulation: insights from social, cognitive and affective neuroscience
.
Current Directions in Psychological Science
17
,
153
158
.

Onoda
K
Okamoto
Y
Nakashima
K
Nittono
H
et al. (
2009
).
Decreased ventral anterior cingulate cortex activity is associated with reduced social pain during emotional support
.
Social Neuroscience
4
,
443
454
.

Onoda
K
Okamoto
Y
Toki
S
Ueda
K
et al. (
2008
).
Anterior cingulate cortex modulates preparatory activation during certain anticipation of negative picture
.
Neuropsychologia
46
,
102
110
.

Orr
SP
Metzger
LJ
Lasko
NB
Macklin
ML
et al. (
2000
).
De novo conditioning in trauma-exposed individuals with and without posttraumatic stress disorder
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
109
,
290
298
.

Ottowitz
WE
Dougherty
DD
Savage
CR
(
2002
).
The neural network basis for abnormalities of attention and executive function in major depressive disorder: implications for application of the medical disease model to psychiatric disorders
.
Harvard Review of Psychiatry
10
,
86
99
.

Papakostas
GI
(
2006
).
Dopaminergic-based pharmacotherapies for depression
.
European Neuropsychopharmacology
16
,
391
402
.

Paradiso
S
Johnson
DL
Andreasen
NC
O'Leary
DS
et al. (
1999
).
Cerebral blood flow changes associated with attribution of emotional valence to pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral visual stimuli in a PET study of normal subjects
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
156
,
1618
1629
.

Phan
KL
Taylor
SF
Welsh
RC
Decker
LR
et al. (
2003
).
Activation of the medial prefrontal cortex and extended amygdala by individual ratings of emotional arousal: a fMRI study
.
Biological Psychiatry
53
,
211
215
.

Phan
KL
Taylor
SF
Welsh
RC
Ho
SH
et al. (
2004
a).
Neural correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: a trial-related fMRI study
.
Neuroimage
21
,
768
780
.

Phan
KL
Wager
T
Taylor
SF
Liberzon
I
(
2002
).
Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI
.
Neuroimage
16
,
331
348
.

Phan
KL
Wager
TD
Taylor
SF
Liberzon
I
(
2004
b).
Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions
.
CNS Spectrums
9
,
258
266
.

Phelps
EA
Delgado
MR
Nearing
KI
LeDoux
JE
(
2004
).
Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC
.
Neuron
43
,
897
905
.

Pyszczynski
T
Greenberg
J
(
1987
).
Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: a self-awareness theory of reactive depression
.
Psychological Bulletin
102
,
122
138
.

Quirk
GJ
Mueller
D
(
2008
).
Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
33
,
56
72
.

Rauch
SL
Shin
LM
Phelps
EA
(
2006
).
Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research – past, present, and future
.
Biological Psychiatry
60
,
376
382
.

Reiman
EM
Lane
RD
Ahern
GL
Schwartz
GE
et al. (
1997
).
Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
154
,
918
925
.

Ressler
KJ
Rothbaum
BO
Tannenbaum
L
Anderson
P
et al. (
2004
).
Cognitive enhancers as adjuncts to psychotherapy: use of D-cycloserine in phobic individuals to facilitate extinction of fear
.
Archives of General Psychiatry,
61
,
1136
1144
.

Roesch
MR
Calu
DJ
Schoenbaum
G
(
2007
).
Dopamine neurons encode the better option in rats deciding between differently delayed or sized rewards
.
Nature Neuroscience
10
,
1615
1624
.

Rothbaum
BO
Schwartz
AC
(
2002
).
Exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder
.
American Journal of Psychotherapy
56
,
59
75
.

Rottenberg
J
Gross
JJ
Gotlib
IH
(
2005
).
Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
114
,
627
639
.

Schultz
W
(
2007
).
Multiple dopamine functions at different time courses
.
Annual Review of Neuroscience
30
,
259
288
.

Schweighofer
N
Bertin
M
Shishida
K
Okamoto
Y
et al. (
2008
).
Low-serotonin levels increase delayed reward discounting in humans
.
Journal of Neuroscience
28
,
4528
4532
.

Sheline
YI
Barch
DM
Donnelly
JM
Ollinger
JM
et al. (
2001
).
Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study
.
Biological Psychiatry
50
,
651
658
.

Sheline
YI
Gado
MH
Kraemer
HC
(
2003
).
Untreated depression and hippocampal volume loss
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
160
,
1516
1518
.

Shin
LM
Liberzon
I
(
2010
).
The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
35
,
169
191
.

Shin
LM
Rauch
SL
Pitman
RK
(
2006
).
Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD
.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
1071
,
67
79
.

Simmons
A
Matthews
SC
Stein
MB
Paulus
MP
(
2004
).
Anticipation of emotionally aversive visual stimuli activates right insula
.
Neuroreport
15
,
2261
2265
.

Simmons
A
Strigo
I
Matthews
SC
Paulus
MP
et al. (
2006
).
Anticipation of aversive visual stimuli is associated with increased insula activation in anxiety-prone subjects
.
Biological Psychiatry
60
,
402
409
.

Simons
AD
Murphy
GE
Levine
JL
Wetzel
RD
(
1986
).
Cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy for depression. Sustained improvement over one year
.
Archives of General Psychiatry
43
,
43
48
.

Surguladze
S
Brammer
MJ
Keedwell
P
Giampietro
V
et al. (
2005
).
A differential pattern of neural response toward sad vs. happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder
.
Biological Psychiatry
57
,
201
209
.

Takahashi
H
Kato
M
Takano
H
Arakawa
R
et al. (
2008
).
Differential contributions of prefrontal and hippocampal dopamine D(1) and D(2) receptors in human cognitive functions
.
Journal of Neuroscience
28
,
12032
12038
.

Tanaka
SC
Doya
K
Okada
G
Ueda
K
et al. (
2004
).
Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops
.
Nature Neuroscience
7
,
887
893
.

Tanaka
SC
Schweighofer
N
Asahi
S
Shishida
K
et al. (
2007
).
Serotonin differentially regulates short- and long-term prediction of rewards in the ventral and dorsal striatum
.
PLoS One
2
,
e1333
.

Taylor
SF
Liberzon
I
Fig
LM
Decker
LR
et al. (
1998
).
The effect of emotional content on visual recognition memory: a PET activation study
.
Neuroimage
8
,
188
197
.

Ueda
K
Okamoto
Y
Okada
G
Yamashita
H
et al. (
2003
).
Brain activity during expectancy of emotional stimuli: an fMRI study
.
Neuroreport
14
,
51
55
.

UKECT Review Group
(
2003
).
Efficacy and safety of electroconvulsive therapy in depressive disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Lancet
361
,
799
808
.

Urry
HL
van Reekum
CM
Johnstone
T
Kalin
NH
et al. (
2006
).
Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults
.
Journal of Neuroscience
26
,
4415
4425
.

Walker
DL
Ressler
KJ
Lu
KT
Davis
M
(
2002
).
Facilitation of conditioned fear extinction by systemic administration or intra-amygdala infusions of D-cycloserine as assessed with fear-potentiated startle in rats
.
Journal of Neuroscience
22
,
2343
2351
.

Warneke
L
(
1990
).
Psychostimulants in psychiatry
.
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry
35
,
3
10
.

Williams
WA
Shoaf
SE
Hommer
D
Rawlings
R
et al. (
1999
).
Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on plasma and cerebrospinal fluid tryptophan and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in normal volunteers
.
Journal of Neurochemistry
72
,
1641
1647
.

Wise
RA
(
2002
).
Brain reward circuitry: insights from unsensed incentives
.
Neuron
36
,
229
240
.

Yeung
N
Botvinick
MM
Cohen
JD
(
2004
).
The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity
.
Psychological Review
111
,
931
959
.

Yoshimura
S
Okamoto
Y
Onoda
K
Matsunaga
M
et al. (
2010
).
Rostral anterior cingulate cortex activity mediates the relationship between the depressive symptoms and the medial prefrontal cortex activity
.
Journal of Affective Disorders
122
,
76
85
.

Yoshimura
S
Ueda
K
Suzuki
S
Onoda
K
et al. (
2009
).
Self-referential processing of negative stimuli within the ventral anterior cingulate gyrus and right amygdala
.
Brain and Cognition
69
,
218
225
.

Yoshino
A
Okamoto
Y
Onoda
K
Yoshimura
S
et al. (
2010
).
Sadness enhances the experience of pain via neural activation in the anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala: an fMRI study
.
Neuroimage
50
,
1194
1201
.

Zald
DH
Lee
JT
Fluegel
KW
Pardo
JV
(
1998
).
Aversive gustatory stimulation activates limbic circuits in humans
.
Brain
121
:
1143
1154
.

Zald
DH
Pardo
JV
(
1997
).
Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
94
,
4119
4124
.