-
PDF
- Split View
-
Views
-
Cite
Cite
Clarissa A Shaw, Jean K Gordon, Understanding Elderspeak: An Evolutionary Concept Analysis, Innovation in Aging, Volume 5, Issue 3, 2021, igab023, https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igab023
- Share Icon Share
Abstract
Elderspeak is an inappropriate simplified speech register that sounds like baby talk and is used with older adults, especially in health care settings. Understanding the concept of elderspeak is challenging due to varying views about which communicative components constitute elderspeak and whether elderspeak is beneficial or harmful for older adults.
Rodgers’ evolutionary concept analysis method was used to evaluate the concept of elderspeak through identification of elderspeak’s attributes, antecedents, and consequences. A systematic search using the PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and Embase databases was completed.
Eighty-three theoretical or research articles from 1981 to 2020 were identified. Elderspeak characteristics were categorized by semantic, syntactic, pragmatic, paralinguistic, and nonverbal attributes. The primary antecedent to elderspeak is implicit ageism, in which old age cues and signs of functional or cognitive impairment led to simplified communication, usually from a younger caregiver. Research studies varied in reporting whether elderspeak facilitated or interfered with comprehension by older adults, in part depending on the operational definition of elderspeak and experimental manipulations. Exaggerated prosody, a key feature of elderspeak, was found to reduce comprehension. Elderspeak was generally perceived as patronizing by older adults and speakers were perceived as less respectful. In persons with dementia, elderspeak also increases the probability of resistiveness to care, which is an important correlate of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia.
Based on this concept analysis, a new definition of elderspeak is proposed, in which attributes that have been found to enhance comprehension are differentiated from those that do not. Recommendations for consistent operationalization of elderspeak in future research are made.
Translational significance: The concept of elderspeak has faced conceptual inconsistencies across four decades of research. This review generated a new definition of elderspeak: “Elderspeak is a form of communication overaccommodation used with older adults that: is evidenced by inappropriately juvenile lexical choices and/or exaggerated prosody; arises from implicit ageist stereotypes; carries goals of expressing care, exerting control, and/or facilitating comprehension; and may lead to negative self-perceptions in older adults and challenging behaviors in persons with dementia.” This definition can be used to guide future research and practice in the prevention of elderspeak communication to combat the patronization and infantilization of older adults.
Elderspeak is a simplified speech register used with older adults which sounds like baby talk. It is characterized by a variety of linguistic adjustments in rhythm, sound, sentence structure, and meaning, such as a high-pitched and overnurturing voice, use of inappropriate terms of endearment (e.g., sweetie), and collective pronoun substitution (e.g., we instead of you). Elderspeak occurs frequently in health care settings (Caporael, 1981; Herman & Williams, 2009) and may be enacted by a variety of health care providers, including nurses and nursing assistants (Williams et al., 2009), physicians (Schroyen et al., 2018), occupational therapists (Alden & Toth-Cohen, 2015), chiropractors (Cockrell, 2020), and social workers (Österholm & Samuelsson, 2015). The linguistic adjustments of elderspeak are similar to those found in baby talk, also called “motherese” or “infant-directed speech” (Soderstrom, 2007). Thus, elderspeak involves the displacement of baby talk from an appropriate target, a child, to an inappropriate target, an adult (Whitmer & Whitbourne, 1997).
As will be elaborated in subsequent sections, elderspeak is expressed when speakers identify old age cues and interpret them as a need to accommodate communication—often subconsciously—to facilitate the comfort and comprehension of older adults (Draper, 2005; Ryan, Hummert et al., 1995). Due to how elderspeak emerges, it is conceptualized as an ageist behavior that leads to negative self-perceptions among older adults (Giles et al., 1992; Ryan et al., 1986). According to a systematic review of 24 studies (Brown & Draper, 2003), elderspeak tends to be perceived as patronizing by older adults, but in some cases has been interpreted as nurturing by older adults (Marsden & Holmes, 2014; Whitmer & Whitbourne, 1997). Despite the largely negative perceptions of elderspeak by older adults, it has been suggested that elderspeak can be beneficial by enhancing communication comprehension (Cohen & Faulkner, 1986; Gould et al., 2002; McGuire et al., 2000). Arguably the most significant consequence of elderspeak is its impact on the behavior of persons with dementia (Cunningham & Williams, 2007; Williams, Perkhounkova et al., 2017). The use of elderspeak during care encounters doubles the probability of resistiveness to care in persons with dementia in nursing homes (Williams et al., 2009).
Elderspeak is typically adopted by health care professionals caring for older adults with the positive intentions of conveying comfort, encouraging cooperation, and enhancing comprehension (Grimme et al., 2015). However, it is unclear if elderspeak actually accomplishes these goals. Based on perceptions that elderspeak is patronizing and findings that it can lead to negative behavioral responses for persons with dementia, it has been argued that preventing elderspeak in health care is critical when providing person-centered care (Bethea & Balazs, 1997; Rousseau, 2019; Savundranayagam, 2014; Wick & Zanni, 2007). Yet, understanding the concept of elderspeak is challenging due to varying views about what constitutes elderspeak and whether elderspeak should be considered beneficial or harmful to older adults.
Based on the proposed and potentially conflicting consequences of elderspeak discussed above and elaborated below—enhanced comprehension, feelings of nurturance, negative self-esteem, and resistance to care by persons with dementia—it is evident that this phenomenon has important implications in the care of older adults. However, inconsistencies in the concept lead to misunderstanding regarding which aspects of elderspeak are potentially beneficial or detrimental. The purpose of this review is to clarify the concept of elderspeak through Rodgers’ evolutionary concept analysis method (Rodgers, 2000; Tofthagen & Fagerstrøm, 2010). We begin by outlining the theoretical underpinnings of the concept. Following this, we present an integrative review of the elderspeak literature by identifying the attributes, antecedents, and consequences of elderspeak. We end with recommendations for operationalizing and defining elderspeak for the consistency of future research.
Method
Design
Rodgers’ method for evolutionary concept analysis (Rodgers, 2000; Tofthagen & Fagerstrøm, 2010) uses an inductive and iterative approach. First, the concept and surrogate terms are identified. Second, the setting and sample for data collection are identified. In the third and fourth steps, the literature is identified and analyzed according to attributes, antecedents, consequences, surrogate terms, and related concepts relevant to the concept. Throughout this process, examples of cases to support attributes are gathered. The analysis of the literature is similar to qualitative inquiry using a thematic analysis. The final step identifies the implications and directions for further inquiry related to the concept.
Systematic Search
A systematic search was completed in August 2019 and updated in August 2020 using the PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and Embase databases. Search terms included three categories of text word searches: (a) elderspeak and its synonyms (i.e., infantiliz*, patroniz*, “baby talk,” babytalk, superlative OR overaccommodation), (b) communication and its synonyms (i.e., discourse, talk, OR speech), and (c) aging and related terms (i.e., old*, elder*, aged, aging, dementia, OR Alzheimer’s). No preset database limiters were used, in order to maximize the sensitivity of the search.
Studies were included if they were in English, focused on older adults (mean age ≥65 years), and aimed to contribute to understanding the attributes, antecedents, and/or consequences of elderspeak. Both theoretical and research articles were included. Book chapters, dissertations, conference abstracts, review articles without a systematic search, and editorials were excluded. To determine whether the articles represented the concept of elderspeak, all full-text articles were reviewed using the matrix method (Garrard, 2011) and if the attributes, antecedents, or consequences of elderspeak were not present in the article then it was excluded.
Results
Sample
A final sample of 83 articles was reviewed (Figure 1 and Supplementary Table 1). Publication years ranged from 1981 to 2020. Articles included six purely theoretical papers, 17 describing observational research in the naturalistic setting, 18 describing experimental research in the laboratory setting, 22 describing research using vignette or questionnaire approaches, 13 describing qualitative or ethnographic research, five describing intervention studies aimed at reducing elderspeak, and two describing methodologies used to measure elderspeak (Supplementary Table 1). The distinction between observational-naturalistic and experimental categories refers to the nature of how elderspeak was elicited, whereas qualitative/ethnographic studies are distinguished from observational-naturalistic studies based on the nature of the data analysis. Ethnographic analyses are further distinguished from other types of qualitative analyses because of the important role they have played in contextualizing elderspeak. Vignette/questionnaire studies were those that focused on the ratings or evaluations of elderspeak.

Defining Elderspeak Within Models of Patronizing Communication to Older Adults
To begin the literature review, we place the concept of elderspeak in its theoretical context. An influential model characterizing how elderspeak comes about is the Communication Predicament of Aging Model (CPAM; Ryan, Hummert et al., 1995). The CPAM was developed from the Communication (or Speech) Accommodation Theory, a classic behavioral theory proposing that a person will accommodate (i.e., modify) their speech based on their communication partner in order to achieve satisfactory interactions. Such accommodations are generally appropriate and enhance communication. However, in intergenerational communication, the speaker often overaccommodates their speech to an older adult due to a subjective assessment of the older adult’s assumed communication needs (Coupland et al., 1988; Ryan et al., 1986). The CPAM commences with a speaker (typically younger) stereotyping an older adult based on old-age cues (e.g., white hair, a wheelchair) leading the speaker to believe that older adults are incompetent and dependent. These cues prompt the younger speaker to simplify their language and adopt exaggerated speech patterns. This is usually perceived negatively by the older adult, which may constrain future opportunities for communication and reinforce stereotypes held by both the older adult and their younger communication partner.
The Model of Patronizing Talk supplements the CPAM by proposing that patronizing talk varies along the orthogonal dimensions of control and care. The proposed purpose of controlling talk is to establish authority or direct the behavior of the older adult, while caring talk is hypothesized to arise from a desire to appear warm and nurturing during the interaction (Hummert & Ryan, 1996). Language that is high on the control dimension and low on the care dimension appears overly directive, such as imperative sentences (e.g., “Stand up now”). Conversely, language that is low on the control dimension and high on the care dimension is considered overly personal (e.g., excessive praise, minimizing terms). Baby talk may be high on both control and care in an attempt to reconcile the actions of control while appearing caring. For example, speakers may alter the tone of their voice to be high-pitched or sing-song and add terms of endearment in order to minimize the perception of control in messages (e.g., “Stand up a minute, would you, sweetie?”). These dimensions of care and control are particularly relevant to health care settings, which require staff to simultaneously care for older adults while controlling health-related interventions.
Other theories of infantilizing communication have been developed that address the challenges of care and control occurring in health care communication. Communication with older adults in residential settings has been theorized to be either nurturing, managerial, overly personal, or focused on the adult as sick/dependent (Grainger, 1993). These categories also reflect the care–control dimension, contrasting the control of managerial talk with the inappropriate intimacy of overly personal talk and the implication of dependence that comes with nurturing discourse and a focus on sickness.
The Communication Enhancement Model (CEM) was developed to address the need for accommodation without patronization in older adults with communication declines or disorders (Ryan, Meredith et al., 1995). As in the CPAM, an unfamiliar health care provider recognizes aging cues in the older interlocutor during a communicative encounter. However, in the CEM, accommodations are hypothesized to be made based on observed individual needs rather than stereotypes of aging. In the resulting feedback loop, accommodations adapt as communication needs are continually assessed.
Surrogate Terms and Evolutionary Overview
Before reviewing the findings of elderspeak research, we provide an overview of how research in this area has evolved methodologically. The elderspeak phenomenon was first reported in 1981 in U.S. nursing homes (Ashburn & Gordon, 1981; Caporael, 1981). Ashburn and Gordon (1981) identified that staff simplified their speech when talking to residents compared to when talking to their coworkers; this was substantiated in another U.S. nursing home shortly thereafter (Culbertson & Caporael, 1983). Caporael (1981) identified that 22% of utterances from long-term care staff to residents contained some form of “baby talk” and that 75% of the baby talk to the residents could not be distinguished from baby talk directed at 2-year olds. In these early studies, it was unclear if baby talk to older adults was positive or negative because some nursing home residents preferred baby talk to nonbaby talk, while others did not (Caporael, 1981; Caporael et al., 1983). Following these studies, theoretical propositions arose suggesting that baby talk to older adults was a patronizing overaccommodation that would lead to negative social and psychological outcomes for older adults (Coupland et al., 1988; Lanceley, 1985; Ryan et al., 1986).
Over the past 40 years of elderspeak research, the concept has been referred to as baby talk (Caporael, 1981), secondary baby talk (Sachweh, 1998), infantilizing speech (Whitbourne et al., 1995), communication overaccommodation (Ryan et al., 1986), and patronizing talk (Ryan et al., 1991). The term “elderspeak” was first coined by Cohen and Faulkner in 1986. The definition of elderspeak has taken different forms, in part because it is sometimes defined by its attributes (e.g., exaggerated speech characteristics), by its antecedents (e.g., enacted due to implicit ageism), or by its consequences (e.g., perceived as patronizing). The concept was first formulated by defining the attributes as similar to baby talk. The definition then progressed to include antecedents that focused on the ageist notion that older adults need simplified communication, as described above in the CPAM. As research continued, however, negative consequences began to be considered more frequently, particularly the idea that elderspeak is patronizing and/or inappropriate and the behavioral consequences that might arise from such perceptions.
To answer the complex questions related to the psychosocial impact of elderspeak and the impact of elderspeak on comprehension, research began to shift from naturalistic observations in nursing homes to experimental studies in more controlled laboratory settings (Figure 2). With this shift in study design, the focus of research also began to shift from the initial focus on health care encounters particularly in the nursing home setting, to more general intergenerational encounters (Supplementary Figure 1). Over a decade of experimental research concentrated on how elderspeak was perceived by older adults and its impact on comprehension (e.g., studies by Kemper et al., Hummert et al., and Ryan et al.). These experimental studies indicated that elderspeak is generally portrayed as patronizing but that some syntactic modifications are likely helpful for comprehension. However, the hallmark characteristics of elderspeak that were identified in the early nursing home studies, like exaggerated prosody and childish words, were rarely enacted in these experimental environments that were not focused on health care or caregiving settings. In the early 2000s, research once again shifted back to the naturalistic setting where health care including nursing homes and adult day centers was once again targeted.

Observational research in nursing homes aimed to identify the behavioral outcomes of elderspeak in persons with dementia and intervention research aimed to reduce elderspeak by long-term care staff (studies by Williams et al.). It is in this period that elderspeak research began to focus more on persons with dementia (Supplementary Figure 2). Prior to this period, older adults with cognitive impairment tended to be excluded from research, and implications for persons with dementia were mostly theoretical (Ryan, Meredith et al., 1995).
Within the past decade, ethnographic and observational elderspeak research has continued worldwide (Supplementary Figure 3) along with some new exploration of attributes. Intervention trials have continued to investigate whether training to reduce the use of elderspeak leads to a reduction in resistiveness to care (Williams, Perkhounkova et al., 2017) or changes in views on aging (Alden & Toth-Cohen, 2015). Elderspeak has also been explored in new media and with new populations, including analyzing tweets for infantilization and ageist language (Gendron et al., 2016) and analyzing elderspeak use in children (Flamion et al., 2020).
Attributes of Elderspeak
The communication characteristics of elderspeak fall into two primary domains: verbal and nonverbal. Within the verbal domain, attributes can be further categorized into linguistic and paralinguistic dimensions. Linguistic dimensions include semantics, the content or meaning of speech; syntax or grammatical form; and pragmatics, which refers to how language is used in a given discourse context. Paralinguistic dimensions include the manner in which the linguistic message is delivered, including the rate of speech and the precision of articulation, variations in pronunciation (e.g., wuv for love), and intonational contours or stress patterns, which depends on variations in relative pitch, duration, and loudness. The nonverbal domain consists of extralinguistic dimensions, in which communication is encoded separately from the linguistic message (e.g., using body language or gestures) but is delivered alongside it or sometimes instead of it. Table 1 lists the attributes of elderspeak, categorized by these dimensions, as well as their hypothesized purpose (i.e., care, control, and comprehension). Nonverbal aspects of communication are generally not considered elderspeak per se, although nonverbal behaviors can—like elderspeak—convey attitudes of condescension, control, or care, and often accompany elderspeak. In this article, we touch on nonverbal aspects of communication briefly for the sake of completeness but focus primarily on verbal dimensions of elderspeak.
Domain . | Goal . | Attribute . |
---|---|---|
Linguistic | ||
Semantics | Care | • Childish terms: Expressions commonly used in childhood • Diminutives: Terms of endearment or pet names inappropriate of the interlocutor relationship • Collectives: Plural forms of pronoun substituted for the individual singular form |
Comprehension | • Simple vocabulary • Short words | |
Syntax | Comprehension | • Simple clauses/sentences • Short utterances • Sentence fragments |
Discourse | Care | • Minimizing words, expressions, and utterances • Exaggerated praise or politeness |
Control | • Tag questions: Question with a desired answer • Directives/imperatives • Reflectives: Phrases that requests action on behalf of someone else • Interruptions • Ignoring | |
Comprehension | • Long pauses/extra fillers | |
Paralinguistic | ||
Prosodic | Care | • Raised pitch • Excessive pitch range or sing-song intonation • Excessively soft voice |
Comprehension | • Overly loud voice • Excessively slowed speech rate • Overly exaggerated pronunciation • Overly exaggerated stress | |
Nonverbal | Control | • Eye-rolling • Standing over • Laughing at |
Care | • Patting |
Domain . | Goal . | Attribute . |
---|---|---|
Linguistic | ||
Semantics | Care | • Childish terms: Expressions commonly used in childhood • Diminutives: Terms of endearment or pet names inappropriate of the interlocutor relationship • Collectives: Plural forms of pronoun substituted for the individual singular form |
Comprehension | • Simple vocabulary • Short words | |
Syntax | Comprehension | • Simple clauses/sentences • Short utterances • Sentence fragments |
Discourse | Care | • Minimizing words, expressions, and utterances • Exaggerated praise or politeness |
Control | • Tag questions: Question with a desired answer • Directives/imperatives • Reflectives: Phrases that requests action on behalf of someone else • Interruptions • Ignoring | |
Comprehension | • Long pauses/extra fillers | |
Paralinguistic | ||
Prosodic | Care | • Raised pitch • Excessive pitch range or sing-song intonation • Excessively soft voice |
Comprehension | • Overly loud voice • Excessively slowed speech rate • Overly exaggerated pronunciation • Overly exaggerated stress | |
Nonverbal | Control | • Eye-rolling • Standing over • Laughing at |
Care | • Patting |
Domain . | Goal . | Attribute . |
---|---|---|
Linguistic | ||
Semantics | Care | • Childish terms: Expressions commonly used in childhood • Diminutives: Terms of endearment or pet names inappropriate of the interlocutor relationship • Collectives: Plural forms of pronoun substituted for the individual singular form |
Comprehension | • Simple vocabulary • Short words | |
Syntax | Comprehension | • Simple clauses/sentences • Short utterances • Sentence fragments |
Discourse | Care | • Minimizing words, expressions, and utterances • Exaggerated praise or politeness |
Control | • Tag questions: Question with a desired answer • Directives/imperatives • Reflectives: Phrases that requests action on behalf of someone else • Interruptions • Ignoring | |
Comprehension | • Long pauses/extra fillers | |
Paralinguistic | ||
Prosodic | Care | • Raised pitch • Excessive pitch range or sing-song intonation • Excessively soft voice |
Comprehension | • Overly loud voice • Excessively slowed speech rate • Overly exaggerated pronunciation • Overly exaggerated stress | |
Nonverbal | Control | • Eye-rolling • Standing over • Laughing at |
Care | • Patting |
Domain . | Goal . | Attribute . |
---|---|---|
Linguistic | ||
Semantics | Care | • Childish terms: Expressions commonly used in childhood • Diminutives: Terms of endearment or pet names inappropriate of the interlocutor relationship • Collectives: Plural forms of pronoun substituted for the individual singular form |
Comprehension | • Simple vocabulary • Short words | |
Syntax | Comprehension | • Simple clauses/sentences • Short utterances • Sentence fragments |
Discourse | Care | • Minimizing words, expressions, and utterances • Exaggerated praise or politeness |
Control | • Tag questions: Question with a desired answer • Directives/imperatives • Reflectives: Phrases that requests action on behalf of someone else • Interruptions • Ignoring | |
Comprehension | • Long pauses/extra fillers | |
Paralinguistic | ||
Prosodic | Care | • Raised pitch • Excessive pitch range or sing-song intonation • Excessively soft voice |
Comprehension | • Overly loud voice • Excessively slowed speech rate • Overly exaggerated pronunciation • Overly exaggerated stress | |
Nonverbal | Control | • Eye-rolling • Standing over • Laughing at |
Care | • Patting |
Semantic attributes
Semantics are largely encoded by lexical (i.e., word) choices. In elderspeak, lexical substitutions range from the more subtle uses, such as using a simplified vocabulary, to more egregious examples, such as using diminutive word-forms typical of child-directed speech (e.g., boo-boo, owie). Diminutives have been described as a highly reliable although infrequent index of elderspeak in experimental studies (Kemper, 1994). In observational studies in US nursing homes, diminutives were found in 53% of 80 staff–resident interactions (Williams, Shaw et al., 2017). Several extreme examples of diminutives (cutie-pie, honey-bunny, tootsie) were documented in ethnographic observations of various adult day centers in the United States (Salari, 2005; Salari & Rich, 2001).
Another commonly cited feature of elderspeak is the substitution of collective first-person pronouns (we, us) for second-person pronouns (you), such as: “It’s important that we get out of our room for awhile, dear” (Ryan et al., 2000). The inappropriate use of the collective pronoun has been posited to illustrate the speaker’s “refusal … to treat the patient as an individual” (Lanceley, 1985). The Model of Patronizing Talk (Hummert & Ryan, 1996) echoes this view, proposing that using first-person plural pronouns assert joint control over the care recipient (e.g., “We must wash ourselves,” an example from Sachweh, 1998). In nursing home contexts, collective pronoun substitution is one of the most frequently occurring aspects of elderspeak; it was present in 66% of the conversations in German nursing homes (Sachweh, 1998) and in 69% of encounters in U.S. nursing homes (Williams, Shaw et al., 2017). It was also the most common elderspeak attribute in encounters between older adults and chiropractor students, averaging three occurrences per encounter (Cockrell, 2020). By contrast, Ashburn and Gordon (1981) noted that pronoun substitution was rare in their analysis of nursing home interactions, although when it did occur, it was only in staff–resident interactions, not in conversations between volunteers and residents. Young adult speakers were also more likely to use the inclusive we in giving instructions to simulated older adults living with dementia compared to those without dementia (Kemper, Finter-Urczyk et al., 1998).
Psycholinguistic analyses have also examined the semantic complexity of language spoken to older adults, using measures of lexical diversity (i.e., the proportion of different words used) such as type–token ratio (TTR), lexical frequency, and propositional density (i.e., the proportion of words that encode basic idea units; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996). Propositional density is generally found to be lower in a speech addressed to older versus younger listeners (Kemper et al., 1996), particularly by younger speakers (Kemper et al., 1995) and when addressing cognitively impaired versus cognitively intact listeners (Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998; Kemper, Finter-Urczyk et al., 1998). Similar effects are rarely found for TTR, and when they are (Kemper et al., 1995), they are of questionable validity, given the well-known confound of TTR with sample length.
Syntactic attributes
The most common syntactic modifications in elderspeak involve shortening and/or simplifying utterances, which are measured by calculating the mean length of utterances in words and/or the mean number of clauses per utterance, respectively. More sophisticated measures of grammatical complexity have also been implemented by counting the number of certain complex syntactic structures, such as left-branching and right-branching sentences (Kemper, Othick et al., 1998). Utterances to older listeners are often found to be shorter and/or less complex than utterances to younger listeners (Culbertson & Caporael, 1983; Kemper, 1994; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996; Samuelsson et al., 2013; Schroyen et al., 2018) and shorter and less complex still when the older adult had, or was believed to have, dementia (Ashburn & Gordon, 1981; Hummert & Shaner, 1994; Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998; Kemper, Finter-Urczyk et al., 1998). Caregivers produced fewer mazes (i.e., repetitions, revisions, filled pauses, abandoned utterances) when speaking to older adult residents compared to speaking to each other, which was proposed to be a conscious attempt by the speaker to reduce the burden on the working memory of older adults (Samuelsson et al., 2013).
Pragmatic/discourse attributes
At the discourse level, elderspeak is most likely to arise as a byproduct of the interaction between care and control inherent to the caregiver–patient relationship (Grainger, 1993; Hummert & Ryan, 1996; Marsden & Holmes, 2014). Directive speech, usually in the form of imperative sentences (e.g., “Take your dress off”), is a frequent speech act that exerts control in caregiving contexts (Grainger, 1993; Hummert & Ryan, 1996; Hummert & Shaner, 1994). Staff–resident interactions have more imperatives and interrogatives than staff–staff interactions (Ashburn & Gordon, 1981). To soften commands, softening phrases are often added to mitigate the act being commanded, as in “We’re just going to put your legs in over the bath” (Grainger, 1993); “Here comes some little pills … and then a little water” (Samuelsson et al., 2013). Tag questions, questions attached to the end of a statement providing the illusion of choice, may also serve this mitigating function. Examples include “You’re ready for breakfast now, aren’t you?” (Williams, Shaw et al., 2017) and “You wouldn’t want the grey shirt, would you?”(Samuelsson et al., 2013). Tag questions occurred in almost half of the interactions between nursing home staff and residents (Williams, Shaw et al., 2017), particularly when younger speakers spoke to older listeners (Kemper et al., 1995). Another example is when the listener is beseeched to carry out a command to appease the speaker, such as “Bess, stand up for me” (Caporael & Culbertson, 1986, p. 104). Williams, Shaw et al. (2017) documented such reflective forms in 14% of nursing home staff–resident interactions.
In structured experimental studies, wherein the choice of speech acts is more constrained than in naturalistic studies, other aspects of discourse illustrate the pragmatic characteristics of language directed at older adults. For example, Kemper et al. conducted a series of studies using referential communication tasks in which the listener followed instructions to reproduce a pattern or follow a map (Kemper et al., 1995, 1996; Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998; Kemper, Finter-Urczyk et al., 1998; Kemper, Othick et al., 1998). Results across these and other studies were fairly consistent, showing that younger speakers produced more instructions, location checks, and repetitions when speaking to older rather than younger listeners (Kemper et al., 1995, 1996; Schroyen et al., 2018) and when speaking to listeners who were assumed to be, or simulated to be, living with dementia (Ashburn & Gordon, 1981; Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998; Kemper, Finter-Urczyk et al., 1998). When older speakers were included (Kemper et al., 1995, 1996; Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998), they generally maintained consistent communication patterns with younger and older listeners, indicating that old-age cues did not lead to overaccommodations by older speakers.
Paralinguistic attributes
Prosodic characteristics are considered hallmark attributes of elderspeak (Caporael & Culbertson, 1986; Cohen & Faulkner, 1986; O’Connor & Rigby, 1996; Sachweh, 1998). A high-pitched register may be adopted, vocal volume may be inappropriately loud, and speech may be overarticulated. Intonation patterns (i.e., the melody and rhythm) of utterances are often exaggerated. Altered prosody has been identified in a variety of settings and activities. Exaggerated intonation patterns occurred in almost a quarter of utterances directed to residents in long-term care facilities (Caporael, 1981; Caporael & Culbertson, 1986) and were found to be indistinguishable from teaching assistants at nursery schools in the United States (Caporael, 1981). Young adults were judged to speak at a higher pitch and to sound more babyish when speaking with their grandparents than with their parents (Montepare et al., 1992). In written scripts, patronizing communication was rated as likely being more shrill, high-pitched, and exaggerated compared to nonpatronizing communication (Ryan et al., 1991; Ryan, Maclean et al., 1994; Ryan, Meredith et al., 1994).
Prosodic changes have also been identified across cultures within both formal and informal caregiving encounters (Cavallaro et al., 2016; Plejert et al., 2014; Sachweh, 1998; Samuelsson et al., 2013; Small et al., 2009). High pitch and exaggerated intonation were found in approximately half of the conversations observed between nurses and residents in German nursing homes (Sachweh, 1998). Changes in prosody were the most common form of elderspeak in interactions between social workers and persons with dementia in Sweden (Österholm & Samuelsson, 2015) and were found to be a key component of expressions that were intended to be soothing (Plejert et al., 2014).
A limitation to studies evaluating the paralinguistic qualities of elderspeak is that ratings of vocal qualities can be unreliable. For example, interrater reliability on aspects of vocal quality has ranged as low as 29% (Caporael, 1981). However, with extensive training, the coding of prosody can be maintained at above 90% (Williams et al., 2018). To address this, some researchers have made use of acoustic analysis to provide objective measures of prosody, such as comparing the mean pitch and pitch variability of speech to younger and older listeners or cognitively intact and impaired listeners (Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998; Kemper, Finter-Urczyk et al., 1998; Kemper, Othick et al., 1998; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996; Samuelsson et al., 2013; Schroyen et al., 2018; Yazdanpanah et al., 2019). Such studies have rarely demonstrated prosodic accommodations to older adults, regardless of cognitive status (Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998; Kemper, Finter-Urczyk et al., 1998; Kemper, Othick et al., 1998; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996; Schroyen et al., 2018), so do not appear to reflect the subjective impression of altered intonation patterns. However, a more recent experimental study using acoustic analysis did show that younger adults spoke with a higher pitch to simulated older adults than to younger adults (Hehman et al., 2012). A small, naturalistic study in a Swedish nursing home (Samuelsson et al., 2013) also demonstrated differences in mean pitch and pitch range between staff–resident and staff–staff dyads.
This variability may be accounted for, in part, by methodological differences. Studies in controlled laboratory settings often evaluate conversations between strangers about predefined and superficial topics (e.g., the giving of directions). By contrast, in naturalistic long-term care settings, speakers are usually familiar caregivers who perform intimate care. Thus, experimental studies may not reflect the prosodic variation that occurs in naturalistic conversation in health care settings.
Nonverbal attributes
Relatively little research has investigated nonverbal attributes of communicative interactions with older adults. One experimental study focused on nonverbal behaviors in interactions between caregivers and older adults by providing written scripts of elderspeak and asking participants to infer nonverbal behaviors (Ryan, Maclean et al., 1994). Scripts categorized as patronizing were associated with negative nonverbal behaviors such as rolling the eyes, shaking the head, crossing arms, and standing over the older adult. By contrast, scripts categorized as neutral were associated with positive nonverbal characteristics such as making eye contact, smiling, and crouching to the level of the older adult. Nevertheless, the natural occurrence of nonverbal features of elderspeak communication remains relatively unattested in research.
Additional attributes
A number of other linguistic and paralinguistic features that cannot be unambiguously attributed to semantic, syntactic, or pragmatic domains have been measured to characterize elderspeak at the discourse level. In speaking to older adults, Kemper et al. have found that more words and/or utterances are produced overall (Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998; Kemper, Othick et al., 1998; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996), and this was likely a consequence of providing more instructions and repetitions to help them complete the task. Similarly, the rate of speech has generally been shown to be slower for speech directed to older rather than younger listeners (Hehman et al., 2012; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996; Samuelsson et al., 2013; Schroyen et al., 2018).
The “emotional tone” of interactions embodies the relationship between care, respect, and control (Schnabel et al., 2020; Williams, 2006; Williams & Herman, 2011; Williams et al., 2003, 2012, 2018). Emotional tone includes controlling communication (i.e., being directive, bossy, patronizing, dominating, and controlling) and person-centered communication (i.e., being nurturing, affirming, respectful, supportive, polite, caring, and warm; Williams et al., 2012). Changes in emotional tone generally occur in conjunction with other semantic, pragmatic, and paralinguistic attributes of elderspeak (Williams, 2006; Williams et al., 2003, 2018).
Antecedents to Elderspeak
The antecedent to virtually all occurrences of elderspeak is the perception—likely subconscious—that accommodation is needed during a communication encounter with an older adult. Younger speakers are more likely to alter their communication when speaking to older adults (Thimm et al., 1998), whereas older adults tend to maintain consistent communication whether talking to older adult peers or younger adults (Hummert et al., 1998; Kemper, 1994; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996; Small et al., 2009). According to the CPAM, it is the perception of old-age cues that prompts alteration of communication. Recent research indicates that this implicit ageism is a pervasive problem in that children learn elderspeak as a form of communication with older adults at a young age (Flamion et al., 2020). However, overaccommodation is not solely due to an age differential; there are individual characteristics of both the older adult and the younger communication partner that, along with contextual conditions, make elderspeak more likely.
Older adult characteristics
Beyond old-age cues, the next greatest antecedent to overaccommodation is the perception that the older adult has a cognitive and/or function impairment. Both younger and older adult raters agreed that overaccommodation would be more likely to occur with frail, cognitively impaired, functionally dependent, or “less competent” rather than healthy older adults (Balsis & Carpenter, 2005; Chen et al., 2017; Hummert & Shaner, 1994; Hummert et al., 1998; Savundranayagam et al., 2007; Thimm et al., 1998). Long-term care nursing assistants reported elderspeak to be more appropriate with older or cognitively impaired residents (Lombardi et al., 2014).
More directly, elderspeak has been observed to occur more frequently with residents who had greater functional and cognitive impairments (Caporael et al., 1983; Sachweh, 1998; Williams, 2006; Williams et al., 2009). Emotional tone has also been rated as less person-centered for hospitalized patients with greater functional decline (Schnabel et al., 2020), suggesting that tone was based on more stereotyped perceptions of these patients. Diminutives (Kemper, 1994) and collectives (Kemper, Finter-Urczyk et al., 1998) were used more frequently with older adults with simulated dementia, although this was not the case in all studies (Thimm et al., 1998). Older adults are also more likely to overaccommodate their communication when speaking with persons with dementia or persons with functional declines (Kemper, Ferrell et al., 1998), even though they tend not to alter their communication to other cognitively intact older adults (Hummert et al., 1998; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996).
In naturalistic studies conducted in Germany and Singapore, it was observed that elderspeak was almost exclusively directed at female nursing home residents (Cavallaro et al., 2016; Sachweh, 1998), although it should be noted that these studies primarily included female residents. This differs from studies in the United States, where males and females reported receiving similar amounts of baby talk in both residential and community settings (O’Connor & Rigby, 1996). However, older females and those reporting higher levels of succorance (dependence on others for protection and sympathy) also reported receiving more elderspeak, suggesting that age and dependence may matter more than gender (O’Connor & Rigby, 1996). Male older adults were coded to receive more tag questions than female older adults in simulated encounters with chiropractic students, but these differences were not demonstrated with diminutives, collective pronoun substitution, or reflectives (Cockrell, 2020). Race has only been directly analyzed in one experimental study, which demonstrated no differences in the amount of patronizing communication to African Americans and European Americans (Atkinson & Sloan, 2017).
Speaker characteristics
The roles of the speaker’s age and gender have also shown effects on the occurrence of elderspeak, although findings have been inconsistent. As noted above, older adults are less likely than younger adults to use elderspeak (Hummert et al., 1998; Kemper, 1994; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996). By contrast, studies tend to show that elderspeak may be enacted regardless of the speaker’s gender (Edwards & Noller, 1998; Kemper, 1994; Thimm et al., 1998), although one study in German nursing homes found that elderspeak was perpetrated more often by female staff (Sachweh, 1998). By contrast, a different study using German speakers demonstrated that males use more elderspeak with older adults with cognitive impairments, while females use elderspeak equally between older adults with and without cognitive impairment (Thimm et al., 1998). The identification of the gender differences of the speaker in observational settings is limited by highly skewed distributions; that is, the samples of caregivers observed have been primarily female.
Care staff tend to fall on either end of the elderspeak usage spectrum, as frequent users or rare users. For example, elderspeak from formal caregivers to nursing home residents with dementia varied from 4% to 99% of the communication used in bathing care (Herman & Williams, 2009). Alongside age and gender, there may be a variety of personal and cultural factors contributing to this variable use of elderspeak. In one study, baby talk to residents was used more frequently by nursing home staff than by volunteers (Ashburn & Gordon, 1981). The authors concluded that elderspeak arises because caregivers take on a parental role to residents. A similar conclusion was drawn when middle-aged caregivers used the most elderspeak, suggesting that elderspeak arises when staff also have personal experience as parents (Sachweh, 1998). Psychological factors may also contribute. For example, informal spousal caregivers reporting lower life satisfaction were found to be more likely to enact patronizing communication than caregivers with a higher life satisfaction (Edwards & Noller, 1998). Such individual factors should be explored further in future research.
Contextual and dyadic characteristics
Contextual factors, including environmental factors and dyadic characteristics, may also serve as antecedents to elderspeak. For example, convents were found to have extremely low rates of elderspeak, leading to the hypothesis that elderspeak depends on the culture of the community (Corwin, 2017). Somewhat unexpectedly, however, nursing home residents did not report receiving more elderspeak than cognitively intact older adults residing in the community (O’Connor & Rigby, 1996). A possible explanation for this is the role of familiarity. Elderspeak was perceived to be more appropriate within familiar communication dyads (Lombardi et al., 2014) and with residents who were either very well liked or disliked, although the nature of the elderspeak differed in each case (Sachweh, 1998). Young adults who did not regularly interact with older adults exhibited more patronizing speech and used a higher pitch than young adults who regularly interacted with older adults, which may be related to views that all older adults need overaccommodative communication (Hehman et al., 2012). Older adults also reported that speakers were more likely to produce elderspeak if they had little knowledge of or experience with older adults, if they were ignorant of or indifferent to older adults, and if they held little respect for older adults (Giles et al., 1993). Similarly, assisted living residents believed that they were spoken to with infantilizing communication because they were looked down upon and thought to be like children (Williams & Warren, 2009).
Even if it arises from implicit ageism, elderspeak is likely used by the speaker in the hope of creating a positive dyadic encounter. Elderspeak tends to be enacted with good or practical intentions, not from a place of patronization or malevolence. In long-term care, nursing assistants reported that they used elderspeak to make residents feel comfortable, enhance comprehension, and enhance cooperation (Grimme et al., 2015). Despite these positive intentions, cognitively intact older adults usually perceive elderspeak as patronizing (see Perceptions below), even though they acknowledge that younger speakers are trying to be helpful (Giles et al., 1993).
Consequences
Although elderspeak is typically well-intentioned and thought to enhance comprehension and demonstrate caring, it is unclear whether it actually achieves these goals. Understanding the consequences of elderspeak is critical to deciding whether it is appropriate to use with older adults. The consequences of elderspeak are explored below based on three outcomes for older adults: comprehension, perception, and behavior (Supplementary Table 1).
Impact of elderspeak on comprehension
Kemper et al. conducted a series of studies designed to identify whether and which attributes of elderspeak spontaneously adopted by speakers aided comprehension in older adults as they followed map directions (Kemper et al., 1995, 1996). Across studies, comprehension was generally optimized by a combination of decreases in the rate of speech, sentence length, and sentence complexity and increases in the number of words and utterances, redundancy, and semantic content. However, there was a limit to the effectiveness of these accommodations. In repeated communication interactions with older adults, younger adults’ speech became more simplified, slower, and more repetitious, but comprehension was not enhanced (Kemper, Othick et al., 1998). The major limitation of these experiments was that improved comprehension was associated with a cluster of spontaneous modifications, making it difficult to identify which specific characteristics enhanced comprehension.
To meet this limitation, a follow-up series of wayfinding experiments were completed in which individual attributes were manipulated (Kemper & Harden, 1999). The first experiment compared neutral speech to speech with syntactic simplifications (i.e., reduced length and complexity of utterances) and semantic elaborations (i.e., repeated/expanded instructions and comprehension checks). Each of these alone resulted in improved comprehension for older adults but combining syntactic and semantic manipulations did not aid comprehension further. The next experiment compared neutral speech to speech with either syntactic simplifications, exaggerated prosody (i.e., high pitch, pauses before key directions, slow speaking rate, prolonged vowels), or both. Comprehension in older adults improved with syntactic simplifications but not exaggerated prosody. When syntactic simplifications were combined with exaggerated prosody, the comprehension gains were lost, suggesting that the use of exaggerated prosody negates any benefits of syntactic simplification.
In contrast to these results, some studies suggest that prosodic aspects of elderspeak (i.e., enhanced focal stress and slowed speech rate) may enhance comprehension (Cohen & Faulkner, 1986; Gould et al., 2002). After listening to scripts read aloud with or without focal stress, older adults were able to recall more information with added focal stress (Cohen & Faulkner, 1986; Gould et al., 2002). However, the so-called “elderspeak” in these studies did not necessarily constitute the concept of elderspeak. There is a range of normal variation of parameters such as stress pattern and rate of speech, which are routinely modified in a variety of contexts (e.g., in noisy environments) to enhance comprehension. For example, communication was described as “careful,” so it probably did not exemplify the exaggerated intonation that is typically considered characteristic of elderspeak (Gould et al., 2002). Such studies illustrate the conceptual confusion surrounding elderspeak, assuming that any speech style modifications (i.e., accommodation) intended to facilitate comprehension in older adults constitute elderspeak (i.e., overaccommodation).
Other experimental studies evaluated the impact of exaggerated prosodic aspects of elderspeak on comprehension (Hehman & Bugental, 2015). Specifically, the effects of high pitch, loudness, and slowness on the completion of a block design task were examined. Older adults receiving the elderspeak condition not only performed worse on the task; they were also the only group to experience an increase in cortisol levels following the task. Therefore, not only did exaggerating prosody worsen comprehension, but it also created a systemic stress response for the older adults. A decrement in comprehension related to greater caregiver pitch range has also been found for persons with dementia (Small et al., 2009). It can thus be inferred that when prosody veers outside the normal range, its impact is generally negative.
In experiments that evaluated both comprehension and the perception of comprehension, older adults perceived a decline in comprehension with elderspeak even if comprehension was actually improved (Kemper & Harden, 1999; Kemper et al., 1995, 1996). In the wayfinding experiments, older adults did not report communication challenges when receiving directions from other older adults (who did not produce elderspeak), but did report challenges when receiving directions from young adults (who did produce elderspeak). This was despite improvements in comprehension when speaking with younger adults (Kemper et al., 1995, 1996). Additionally, as communication was increasingly simplified by younger adults, older adults’ ratings of communication also worsened, even though their performance did not decline (Kemper, Othick et al., 1998). Perceived reception was best when neutral prosody was used relative to exaggerated prosody (Kemper & Harden, 1999). Even though performance did not decline, the perception of lower performance could lead to reduced future performance, due to stereotype threat, in which older adults tend to perform more poorly when age-related stereotypes are triggered. In support of this, older adults with better attitudes toward aging and more positive personal intergenerational interactions seem to be somewhat protected from the harmful effects of patronizing communication on comprehension during intergenerational interactions (Hehman & Bugental, 2015).
Perception of elderspeak
A series of early studies evaluated perceptions of elderspeak primarily using written vignettes of interactions. In such studies, elderspeak is enacted using lexical and syntactic modifications, such as pet names (e.g., sweetie), plural pronouns (e.g., let’s stop being fussy), and patronizing phrases (e.g., now, now). Caregivers communicating with elderspeak were rated as less respectful, competent, nurturing, considerate, warm, supportive, intelligent, confident, helpful, and trustworthy and more dominant, patronizing, incompetent, and unfriendly (Balsis & Carpenter, 2005; Giles et al., 1993; Harwood & Giles, 1996; Harwood et al., 1993; La Tourette & Meeks, 2000; O’Connor & Rigby, 1996; O’Connor & St Pierre, 2004; Ryan et al., 1991; Ryan, Hamilton et al., 1994; Ryan, Meredith et al., 1994). Although these studies demonstrated that elderspeak is perceived negatively, some ratings were completed by naïve young adult raters rather than older adults (Ryan et al., 1991; Ryan, Maclean et al., 1994; Ryan, Meredith et al., 1994). Interestingly, in experiments that included both young and older adult raters, the young adults generally rated elderspeak as more patronizing than older adults did (Edwards & Noller, 1993; Giles et al., 1993; Whitmer & Whitbourne, 1997). Nonnaïve script raters (i.e., formal caregivers) have similarly reported that staff persons using patronizing communication are less respectful, helpful, and competent (Savundranayagam et al., 2007). A drawback of these studies is that it is unclear the extent to which perceptions of written scripts align with perceptions of spoken speech.
More importantly, studies evaluating the perception of older adults themselves confirm that elderspeak is viewed as patronizing (Hummert & Mazloff, 2001). For example, grandparents reported being less satisfied with and less close to grandchildren who overaccommodate communication (Harwood, 2000). Both community-dwelling older adults and nursing home residents reported disliking infantilizing communication (Whitbourne et al., 1995) and rated nurses who used such speech as less respectful, nurturing, benevolent, and competent, even when they were talking to individuals with dementia (La Tourette & Meeks, 2000). Community-dwelling older adults had more negative views of elderspeak than did older adults residing in long-term care (O’Connor & St Pierre, 2004). Older adults have also reported more positive views of elderspeak when used in the hospital setting compared to the community setting (Hummert & Mazloff, 2001).
By contrast, some studies have reported positive perceptions of elderspeak by older adults. Cognitively intact older adults in a rehabilitation hospital perceived exaggerated intonation more positively than normal communication (Whitmer & Whitbourne, 1997). Elderspeak may be rated more positively by older adults who value high succorance (O’Connor & Rigby, 1996), when the communication partners are more familiar (Hummert & Mazloff, 2001), and may be appreciated more as residents’ functional ability declines (Caporael et al., 1983). An ethnographic study in New Zealand nursing homes reported that residents found elderspeak to be positive and to lead to meaningful social relationships between caregivers and residents (Marsden & Holmes, 2014). Another ethnographic study from South Africa proposed that infantilization is a form of posturing that makes intimate care practices acceptable (Makoni & Grainger, 2002). However, in other ethnographic work from Singapore and the United States, elderspeak was viewed as patronizing by older adults (Cavallaro et al., 2016; Salari, 2005). Both American and Thai older adults reported lower self-esteem when overaccommodation was used by younger adults, although the relationship was only significant for Americans (Keaton et al., 2017). Such inconsistencies may arise from cultural differences, but the cultural impact of elderspeak has been relatively unexplored, particularly in the setting of multilingual environments (Yazdanpanah et al., 2019).
Impact of elderspeak on behavior
Behavior was first studied as a consequence of elderspeak by evaluating audio recordings of dyadic care interactions in German nursing homes (Sachweh, 1998). The majority of residents involved in the interactions showed no discernable behavioral reaction to elderspeak, either positive or negative. However, the clearest reactions—both positive and negative—came from residents with dementia. Ethnographic research has reported that elderspeak to persons with dementia often results in fighting, withdrawal, and subsequent poor health (Salari, 2005), or in crying and silence from residents (Cavallaro et al., 2016). Recent research has focused on identifying the impact of specific elderspeak attributes on resistiveness to care in nursing homes (Yazdanpanah et al., 2019).
The impact of elderspeak on the behavior of persons with dementia has been more systematically evaluated in a series of studies by Williams et al. A single-subject case study of dyadic care interactions in a nursing home (Cunningham & Williams, 2007) demonstrated a strong correlation between instances of elderspeak and resistiveness to care (Cunningham & Williams, 2007) and between a controlling emotional tone and resistiveness to care (Williams & Herman, 2011). A subsequent larger study found that elderspeak by nursing home staff doubled the probability of resistiveness to care by persons with dementia (Williams et al., 2009). Next, a randomized cross-over trial was conducted to further understand the causal relationship between elderspeak and resistiveness to care by comparing caregivers who received training to those who did not (Williams, Perkhounkova et al., 2017). Following training, elderspeak by formal caregivers declined by 13.6 percentage points and resistiveness to care by residents with dementia declined by 15.3 percentage points. In a follow-up analysis, nursing homes that received training demonstrated a reduction in antipsychotic medication administration relative to statewide medication rates (Shaw et al., 2018). These findings reflect the negative impact that elderspeak can have on the well-being of persons with dementia and the potential for training to ameliorate this effect.
Related Concepts
An essential component of an evolutionary concept analysis is the identification of related concepts, as this helps define the boundaries of the construct (Rodgers, 2000; Tofthagen & Fagerstrøm, 2010). Elderspeak shares some attributes with communication accommodations for other populations, notably infants and individuals with communication impairments. Other related concepts include behaviors beyond the spoken message that are patronizing to older adults.
Infant-directed speech
Elderspeak communication was first identified by comparing it to speech directed to children (Caporael, 1981), also known as “motherese” or, more recently, “infant/child-directed speech” (Golinkoff et al., 2015; Soderstrom, 2007). Infant-directed speech shares with elderspeak the features of raised and variable pitch contours, as well as a variety of simplifications, such as limited vocabulary and shortened utterances (Golinkoff et al., 2015). Similar to the evolution of elderspeak research, early research on child-directed speech focused on the distinction between modifications intended to enhance clarity and capture attention (i.e., facilitate comprehension) and modifications intended to express affection and establish intimacy (i.e., care). However, unlike elderspeak, it is not posited that infant-directed speech is intended to exert control. Similar patterns of speech may occur in communicative interactions with romantic partners (Bombar & Littig, 1996) or pets (Burnham, 2002; Mitchell, 2001).
Clear speech
Clear speech is a speech style in which speakers overarticulate to maximize the intelligibility of their speech for listeners with hearing loss (Smiljanić & Bradlow, 2009) for example, or by speakers with motor speech disorders such as dysarthria (Hustad & Weismer, 2007). Clear speech shares many properties with elderspeak, such as speaking more slowly, pausing, and prolonging vowels (Lam et al., 2012; Uchanski, 2005), but does not generally involve the semantic or pragmatic aspects of elderspeak. Its singular goal is to enhance comprehension.
Infantilization with older adults and other vulnerable populations
Aside from elderspeak, older adults can be made to feel patronized by other interactional behaviors that would be considered nonperson-centered. Person-centered communication focuses on recognition, negotiation, facilitation, and validation which elderspeak violates along with other common communication behaviors with older adults (Savundranayagam, 2014; Savundranayagam & Moore-Nielsen, 2015). For example, the use of praise, politeness, cheering, tricking, and restricting choices have been identified in contexts with elderspeak and similarly represent unequal power dynamics between the older adults and their conversation partners (Backhaus, 2009; Jansson, 2016; Nilsson et al., 2018). Older adults and persons with dementia can be positioned as inferior not only through elderspeak but also when caregivers and health care providers ignore and speak for them (Österholm & Samuelsson, 2015). Topics may be restricted for older adults compared to nonolder adults such as with discussions of sexual side effects of cancer treatments that were limited for older adults but not for younger adult patients (Schroyen et al., 2018). Written materials can also trigger stereotype threat (Barber & Mather, 2014), resulting in the perception of a patronizing and authoritarian tone (Yardley et al., 2006).
Lastly, attributes of elderspeak are not necessarily unique to encounters with older adults. Similar attributes have been found in communication with persons speaking a foreign language (DePaulo & Coleman, 1986; Rothermich et al., 2019), persons with intellectual or physical disabilities (Fox & Giles, 1996), and in painful health care encounters with nonolder adults (Borders et al., 2013). However, these are not considered elderspeak because the attributes arise outside of the CPAM and not due to implicit ageism. More research is needed to determine the effects of infantilization on these other populations.
Discussion
The most important antecedents of elderspeak have been well defined and operationalized. Elderspeak occurs in interactions with older adults and is enacted based on old-age cues, evidenced by how younger adults simplify communication when speaking to older adults but not to their peers. As the CPAM posits, this form of overaccommodation arises from ageist stereotypes that older adults are incompetent and dependent (Ryan et al., 1986; Ryan, Hummert et al., 1995). Although it is based on ageist views, elderspeak typically does not come from a place of malevolence; instead, it is an implicit strategy that is often intended to convey care to older adults (Grimme et al., 2015; Hummert & Shaner, 1994; Lombardi et al., 2014). Although the antecedents are clear, the concept of elderspeak has been characterized by inconsistencies in its attributes and the consequences of these attributes.
The specific attributes used to operationalize elderspeak depend to some degree on the purpose of the study. Experimental studies have mostly focused on the impact of syntactic simplifications and semantic elaborations on comprehension in older adults. However, research on the psychosocial and behavioral consequences of elderspeak has focused on childish vocabulary and exaggerated prosody. The attributes that appear also depend on the contexts in which elderspeak is elicited. In simulated or experimental environments, lexical and pragmatic aspects of elderspeak seem to occur less spontaneously than changes in syntax. However, more naturalistic studies that have been conducted worldwide across the past four decades confirm the presence of childish words and exaggerated prosody during care encounters.
Similarly, studies focusing on intergenerational communication alone tend to find mostly changes in syntactic attributes, whereas studies in health care contexts demonstrate more prosodic and semantic attributes of elderspeak. This provides evidence that a caring context, especially where intimate care is provided, elicits different aspects of elderspeak than experimental contexts. Each method has advantages and disadvantages. Experimental studies can isolate attributes of elderspeak, contributing to our understanding of the impacts of different characteristics. Data captured in more naturalistic settings may offer a greater understanding of the most commonly occurring attributes and consequences of elderspeak.
There have also been diverse findings regarding the impact of elderspeak on comprehension, which have largely arisen from differences in how it is defined. There is evidence that simplification—specifically reducing syntactic and semantic complexity—enhances comprehension but that other modifications (e.g., shortening utterances without reducing complexity) have little or no impact. Perhaps the most important finding is that exaggerated prosody may reduce both actual and perceived comprehension, eradicating the benefits of more helpful attributes like reducing complexity (Kemper & Harden, 1999). These findings provide further support for the CPAM by identifying that an infantilizing prosodic pattern diminishes perceived competence even when comprehension is not reduced. It is thus important to distinguish between normal variations in intonational contour that serve to provide emphasis and thus tend to enhance comprehension (Ashburn & Gordon, 1981) and prosodic contours that are so exaggerated that they are perceived as patronizing. That such accommodations are a matter of degree with no clear cutoff between what is helpful and what is perceived to be patronizing contributes to the lack of clarity surrounding elderspeak. Additional research is needed on this important relationship between perception and comprehension to help disentangle how personal views of elderspeak affect comprehension.
Operationalizing Elderspeak
In operationalizing attributes of elderspeak, we propose to remove (or consider separately) attributes that are within the range of normal variation in adult-to-adult speech. Many of these have been repeatedly noted to enhance comprehension, including reduced syntactic complexity, repetitions, verifications, and focal stress. Among the attributes to retain are those that are exaggerated—this is the essence of overaccommodation—and thus give rise to perceptions of patronization. These include pragmatic, paralinguistic, and semantic attributes, such as vocabulary choice and exaggerated prosodic contours (see Table 1 for examples). Prosodic attributes have not only been shown to worsen comprehension but have also been key attributes evaluated in the studies concluding that elderspeak increases the probability of resistiveness to care in persons with dementia. Studies measuring elderspeak that have only evaluated syntactic attributes and have concluded that elderspeak is helpful for comprehension in health care environments may be misleading, because the implication is that all aspects of elderspeak must be facilitative. Accommodation itself is not harmful, but overaccommodation can be detrimental to the self-esteem of the older adult and the success of the communicative interaction.
Taking these findings into account, we propose the following definition of elderspeak: “Elderspeak is a form of communication overaccommodation used with older adults that: is evidenced by inappropriately juvenile lexical choices and/or exaggerated prosody; arises from implicit ageist stereotypes; carries goals of expressing care, exerting control, and/or facilitating comprehension; and may lead to negative self-perceptions in older adults and challenging behaviors in persons with dementia.” This definition includes, but distinguishes among, the attributes, antecedents, and consequences of elderspeak. Note that elderspeak may be accompanied by other verbal modifications, such as syntactic and semantic simplifications, but our definition clearly identifies inappropriate modifications (i.e., overaccommodations) as its core attributes.
Limitations
This review provides a comprehensive overview of elderspeak research from the past 40 years. Although 83 studies were identified and analyzed, weaknesses in the research were noted. First, the conceptual inconsistencies noted in elderspeak research that led to this review may arise in part from limitations of the reviewed studies, as we have discussed in the review above. Many of the naturalistic studies drew conclusions from small samples with uncontrolled potential confounds. The ecological validity of some of the experimental studies may be questioned. Nevertheless, it was important for the purpose of the evolutionary concept analysis to represent as completely as possible the range of research that has contributed to our current understanding of elderspeak.
Another shortcoming is that much of the research on elderspeak has been completed by a few research groups using relatively nondiverse samples. The understanding of elderspeak would be strengthened with replication in other samples, including a targeted focus on diverse populations. Finally, with this being an evolutionary review, conflicting findings may arise due to cohort effects. Perceptions on language and aging have likely changed over the past 40 years, which reinforces the need for early research to be replicated.
Future Directions for Research and Education
Based on the limitations outlined above, there is clearly a need for replication and greater systematicity in future research. Our new definition is a proposed step in that direction. Future research needs to continue to investigate which aspects of communication are patronizing to older adults. However, this needs to be done in the context of consequences. Simply identifying elderspeak is fruitless without understanding what impact it has on older adults, including the negative outcomes of elderspeak on cognitively intact older adults as well as persons with dementia outside of the long-term care setting. In cognitively intact older adults, there is a critical need for updated research on the nature of elderspeak and its consequences such as the impact on psychosocial well-being.
The majority of research on elderspeak and older adults has focused on general intergenerational encounters with either the experimental or survey designs. This highlights the need for naturalistic studies on health care encounters between older adults and formal care providers on important psychosocial outcomes like depression. In addition, as baby boomers age, the nature of older adulthood is changing both quantitatively and qualitatively. Increasing numbers of older adults may “normalize” primary age-related changes, and baby boomers are more assertive in their approach to health care, which may have implications for communication (Kahana & Kahana, 2014). For individuals with dementia, there continues to be a critical need for research to understand the ramifications of elderspeak on important outcomes like behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia.
Identifying the attributes of elderspeak also needs to be completed in the context of identifying the antecedents to elderspeak. As discussed above, attributes traditionally called elderspeak but that have positive outcomes should no longer be considered elderspeak. Similarly, attributes that do not arise as part of the CPAM model should not be considered elderspeak. For example, tag questions have recently been identified as having multiple functions and may not always meet the definition of elderspeak (Basque et al., 2020). Thus, as specified in the definition above, attributes should be considered based on their context within the CPAM (i.e., arising from implicit ageism) and within the context of the Model of Patronizing Talk (i.e., arising due to the interplay of care and control).
Beyond ageism, the antecedents to elderspeak have remained relatively unstudied. Research has generally focused on young college-aged adults communicating with real or simulated older adults in laboratory settings or on formal caregivers in the long-term care setting. Elderspeak and its consequences should be continued to be explored in a variety of settings (e.g., hospital, community) with a variety of speakers (e.g., informal caregivers, volunteers), so that important antecedents such as familiarity and gender can further be analyzed because there has been a lack of exploration on these important relationships to date. This is important for targeted intervention and education development. Currently, the only evidence-based intervention for elderspeak reduction is targeted toward long-term care facilities in the United States (Williams, 2006; Williams, Perkhounkova et al., 2017). Educational programs on elderspeak reduction are needed for a variety of settings as research expands.
Enhancing awareness of age-related stereotypes and their relationship to elderspeak can help reduce the impact of this implicit bias. Cost-effective educational interventions focused on elderspeak reduction have not only increased knowledge of elderspeak among health care providers, but also demonstrated subsequent reductions in resistiveness to care by persons with dementia and facility-wide antipsychotic medication administration (Shaw et al., 2018; Williams, Abd-Hamid et al., 2017; Williams, Ayyagari et al., 2017; Williams, Perkhounkova et al., 2017). However, not all educational interventions have demonstrated success and some have actually exacerbated ageist views of older adults, indicating that an evidence-based approach to education about elderspeak is needed to change attitudes and improve care (Alden & Toth-Cohen, 2015). Furthermore, the use of elderspeak is highly individualized, with some care providers rarely enacting its attributes and others doing so repeatedly (Herman & Williams, 2009). Educational interventions thus may need to be targeted specifically to those who use elderspeak.
Conclusions
Consistent with the CEM and alongside person-centered care, we advocate for an individualized approach to communication accommodation based on specific needs. Affirming communication is the goal (Williams et al., 2005), and elderspeak is not considered respectful by many older adults and may lead to harmful behaviors in persons with dementia. However, not all older adults find elderspeak to be patronizing and some aspects of speech accommodation may facilitate comprehension in certain circumstances; therefore, individual communication preferences as well as needs should be assessed (Hummert & Mazloff, 2001; Yazdanpanah et al., 2019). Interventions should thus not only educate formal caregivers on avoiding elderspeak, but more importantly, target person-centered communication techniques based on individual preferences (Williams et al., 2004).
Although speakers should be careful not to overaccommodate, it is also important to avoid underaccommodation, as a lack of attention to communication needs can also have detrimental effects on comprehension and well-being (Gasiorek & Giles, 2012; Scott & Caughlin, 2015). More research is needed on how speakers can achieve appropriate and individualized levels of accommodation and best to train the appropriate use of accommodation; however, this review has identified that avoiding elderspeak is a beneficial strategy for communicating with older adults. Communication is a crucial component of person-centered care to older adults, and care providers must thus be aware that their implicit biases may inadvertently lead to patronizing communication patterns, such as elderspeak.
Funding
This work is supported by the National Institute of Nursing Research (F31NR018580). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Conflict of Interest
None declared.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Kristine Williams, PhD, RN, FNP-BC, FGSA, FAAN for her guidance and feedback on this manuscript.
Comments