Abstract

For centuries, people have asked questions to hand-held pendulums and interpreted their movements as responses from the divine. These movements occur due to the ideomotor effect, wherein priming or thinking of a motion causes muscle movements that end up swinging the pendulum. By associating particular swinging movements with “yes” and “no” responses, we investigated whether pendulums can aid decision-making and which personality traits correlate with this performance. Participants (N=80) completed a visual detection task in which they searched for a target letter among rapidly presented characters. In the verbal condition, participants stated whether they saw the target in each trial. In the pendulum condition, participants instead mentally “asked” a hand-held pendulum whether the target was present; particular motions signified “yes” and “no”. We measured the accuracy of their responses as well as their sensitivity and bias using signal detection theory. We also assessed four personality measures: locus of control (feelings of control over one’s life), transliminality (sensitivity to subtle stimuli), need for cognition (preference for analytical thinking), and faith in intuition (preference for intuitive thinking). Overall, locus of control predicted verbal performance and transliminality predicted pendulum performance. Accuracy was low in both conditions (verbal: 57%, pendulum: 53%), but bias was higher in the verbal condition (d=1.10). We confirmed this bias difference in a second study (d=0.47, N=40). Our results suggest that people have different decision strategies when using a pendulum compared to conscious guessing. These findings may help explain why some people can answer questions more accurately with pendulums and Ouija boards. More broadly, identifying the differences between ideomotor and verbal responses could lead to practical ways to improve decision-making.

Highlights

  • By associating movements with responses, some people use pendulums to aid decision-making.

  • Participants completed a visual detection task and responded either verbally or with a pendulum.

  • Those who were sensitive to subtle stimuli (high transliminality) performed best with pendulums.

  • Participants showed less response bias when using a pendulum compared to verbal guessing.

Introduction

Pendulums magnify subtle movements. If one holds a pendulum and thinks of a particular motion, subtle muscle movements will initiate the swinging of the pendulum in that direction. These movements usually occur without perceived conscious control (Easton and Shor 1976; Gordon and Rosenbaum 1984). As a result, for centuries people have interpreted these movements as responses from the unconscious – or the divine. In some cases, people can answer questions more accurately with muscle movements than they can with conscious guessing (Gauchou et al. 2012). The personality traits that predict this accuracy, however, remain unknown. The present study thus explores several traits and their relation to ideomotor performance.

Hand-held pendulums swing seemingly on their own due to ideomotor movements, subtle muscle movements caused by thinking of a motion. Similar mechanisms likely underlie Ouija boards, automatic writing, dowsing rods, and other ideomotor tools intended to bypass conscious analysis and reduce bias (Spitz 1997; Wegner 2003). Hypnotherapists have used pendulums clinically to probe unconscious material (Ewin 2009); magicians have used them to retrieve information from people such as the location of hidden objects (Spitz 1997; Banachek 2002). Others use pendulums in an attempt to aid decision-making – from choosing which vegetables are fresh to deciding which house to buy or even who to marry (Lundstrom 2010).

Fortunately for those making drastic decisions this way, ideomotor responses can be more accurate than chance alone. For example, Gauchou et al. (2012) tested whether ideomotor responses can reflect implicit knowledge when using a Ouija board. Participants held a small pointer or planchette on a board ascribed with “yes” and “no” responses. The experimenter asked various questions that participants earlier claimed not to know (e.g. “Did Operation Desert Storm occur in the 1980s?”). Without the participants’ perceived control, they moved the planchette toward the “yes” or “no” areas of the board, answering the questions. Their responses were more accurate when using the Ouija board (65%) than when responding verbally (50%). By following their involuntary muscle movements, it seemed that participants could express their implicit knowledge.

These ideomotor phenomena vary from person to person. For some, pendulums barely move; for others, they immediately swing in a consistent direction (Karlin et al. 2007). During our pilot testing, some participants found the pendulum movement mundane while others found it mystical: one even stayed behind to privately ask the pendulum questions about her life. Nevertheless, we know of only two individual factors that may underlie ideomotor differences: gender and hypnotic suggestibility. Women produce larger ideomotor movements than men in some studies (Easton and Shor 1976) but not in others (Wegner et al. 1998). Hypnotic suggestibility – how easily one follows suggestions under hypnosis – also positively correlates with pendulum movement (Eysenck and Furneaux 1945; Karlin et al. 2007). To uncover more of these factors, we explored four personality measures that may predict ideomotor response:

  • Locus of control measures feelings of control over one’s life (Duttweiler 1984). People with an internal locus tend to take responsibility for their actions; those with an external locus tend to believe that situational forces or luck determine their life events. We predicted that people with a more external locus of control would perform better, since they may be more likely to let the pendulum swing without consciously interfering with it (cf. Lundstrom 2010; Gauchou et al. 2012). Similarly, people with an external locus of control may be more suggestible (Burger 1981) which should promote pendulum movement (Eysenck and Furneaux 1945; Karlin et al. 2007).

  • Transliminality measures the threshold at which stimuli reach conscious awareness, as measured by a self-report questionnaire (Lange et al. 2000). People with higher transliminality can detect subtle internal or external stimuli such as briefly presented images (Crawley et al. 2002; Olson et al. 2015). Transliminality also correlates with absorption, mysticism, and paranormal beliefs (Lange et al. 2000). Since pendulum users claim that people need to be sensitive to their thoughts and muscle movements (Nielsen and Polansky 1987; Lundstrom 2010), we predicted that those higher in transliminality would show more accurate ideomotor responses. In addition, since transliminality correlates with paranormal beliefs, high transliminality people may be more open to the atypical activity of asking questions to a pendulum.

  • Need for cognition measures the tendency to engage in and enjoy thinking (Cacioppo and Petty 1982). We expected that these analytical people would perform worse with the pendulum since they may try to consciously interfere with the ideomotor responses (cf. Lundstrom 2010).

  • Faith in intuition measures reliance on intuitive decision-making (Cacioppo and Petty 1982). If ideomotor responses can express implicit knowledge (Gauchou et al. 2012), those who trust their intuition may perform better with the pendulum.

In this paper, we explore how these personality traits relate to ideomotor response. Participants completed two conditions of a task in which they searched for a target letter among rapidly presented characters. In the verbal condition, participants stated whether they saw the target in each trial. In the pendulum condition, they instead mentally “asked” a pendulum whether the target was present; we told them particular motions signified “yes” and “no”. Study 1 compares these verbal and pendulum responses; Study 2 tests whether these differences remain in a more difficult task. Combined, these studies explore whether people can use pendulums to access the mechanisms involved in unconscious decision-making.

Study 1

Methods

Participants

Eighty undergraduate students from McGill University completed the study for course credit. After excluding those who deviated from the task instructions (see the Analysis section), 63 participants remained. They were on average 20.3 years old (SD = 1.4) and 87% were female. Most studied psychology (65%), commonly in the second year of their studies (40%). Few had held a pendulum before (33%) or had done so only for a physics class (25%); few had used a Ouija board either (29%). Most of the participants were right-handed (86%). We chose our sample size in advance based on a power analysis (see the Analysis section).

Materials

Questionnaires

To begin the study, participants completed paper-and-pencil questionnaires testing four personality traits. To measure locus of control, we used the 28-item Internal Control Index. An example item is: “If I want something, I work hard to get it”. Each item uses a five-point Likert scale ranging from “rarely” (1) to “usually” (5). Higher scores on the questionnaire (up to 140) suggest an internal locus of control and lower scores (down to 28) suggest an external one. The scale has high internal consistency (Cronbach’s α=0.84; Duttweiler 1984); it was similar in our sample (α=0.81). Participants had an average score of 102.78 (SD = 11.07, range = 79–126), which is expected given their age and education level (Duttweiler 1984).

We then measured transliminality using the 17-item true–false Revised Transliminality Scale. An example item is: “… I have had such a heightened awareness of sights and sounds that I cannot shut them out”. Agreeing with such items implies greater sensitivity – that more near-threshold material enters conscious awareness. The scale ranges from 0 to 17 reflecting how many items were labeled as true. It has a test–retest reliability of 0.82 and good convergent validity (Houran et al. 2003). The scale also has high internal consistency (Cronbach’s α=0.82; Lange et al. 2000); it was similar in our sample (α=0.77). After a Rasch transformation (see Lange et al. 2000), participants had an average score of 22.9 (SD = 3.49) and a range of 13.7–32.5, close to the expected values (Thalbourne et al. 2003).

Finally, participants completed the 40-item Rational–Experiential Inventory which measures one’s information processing style (Epstein et al. 1996). It has two subscales: need for cognition and faith in intuition. An example item measuring need for cognition is: “I prefer complex problems to simple problems”; for faith in intuition, an example is: “I trust my initial feelings about people”. Each item ranges from “definitely not true” (1) to “definitely true of myself” (5), making each subscale range from 20 to 100. The internal consistency of both subscales is high (α=0.81 and 0.90); the values were similar in our sample (α=0.80 and 0.89). Participants had an average need for cognition score of 76.74 (SD = 10.18, range = 50–96) and a faith in intuition score of 63.99 (SD = 12.62, range = 33–96). The need for cognition score correlated with locus of control (r(60)=0.620, 95% CI [0.450,0.760]).

Equipment

After completing the questionnaires, participants entered the testing room which contained a glass table in front of a computer monitor (1920 × 1080 resolution, 24-inch BenQ, Taipei, Taiwan). Stimuli were presented using PsychoPy (version 1.83.04; Peirce 2009) at 60 Hz. On the table sat a brass pendulum with a 20 cm string (Adermark, Vancouver, Canada). A video camera (GoPro 4, San Mateo, CA) was placed 6 cm underneath the glass surface of the table to record the pendulum’s movement.

Procedure

Instructions

The experimenter explained that pendulums magnify unconscious muscle movements and can therefore reflect implicit knowledge. Based on advice from hypnotherapists and magicians, we used suggestion to associate pendulum movements with particular responses (Banachek 2002; D. Ewin, personal communication, 2014; cf. Eysenck and Furneaux 1945). In particular, the experimenter stated:

What researchers have found is that if you hold a pendulum and think of yes, it will swing up and down as if nodding its head.1 If you think of no, it will swing side to side as if shaking its head no. You don’t even have to consciously move your hand: it will just move unconsciously and the pendulum will begin to swing.2

While giving these instructions, the experimenter demonstrated the movement with the pendulum. Next, the participant held the pendulum in her right hand above the video camera, so that the tip of the pendulum was 2 cm above the table. The participant thought of the word yes and waited for vertical movement. The experimenter promoted this movement by waving her finger beside the pendulum then slowly increasing the speed (cf. pacing and leading; Easton and Shor 1977; Nash and Barnier 2012). Using suggestions common in hypnosis, the experimenter verbally reinforced the pendulum’s movement (“just like that”, “that’s right”) before repeating this procedure for the horizontal movement representing “no”.

Detection task

Participants then completed the task, which consisted of two conditions with 24 trials each. In each trial, participants would see a rapidly presented series of numbers and punctuation marks while they attempted to detect a target letter. The experimenter explained that the letter would appear in half of the trials. The stimuli were white on a 50% gray screen and measured 3 cm in height (4.3 degrees of visual angle).

Each trial began with a fixation cross followed by six distractors (numbers or punctuation marks) at 17 ms each to serve as masks (see Fig. 1). Next, 24 distractors appeared for 33 ms each with no inter-stimulus interval. In half of the trials, one of the distractors was replaced with the target: a random capital letter. The target never appeared in the first six nor the last six positions of the stream to reduce serial position effects (Potter 1976). The stream concluded with another six masks at 17 ms each.

Task design.
Figure 1.

Task design.

Notes: Participants searched for the target letter among distractors (numbers and punctuation). They then stated the target’s presence either verbally or by asking a pendulum and responding based on its movements. In the verbal task, participants stated their confidence (certain or uncertain); in the pendulum task, they stated their agreement with the pendulum’s response (agree, disagree, or uncertain).

Verbal condition

After viewing the stream, participants stated whether the target was present, then the experimenter typed this response. The median response time was 5.53 s (SD = 3.01) and there was no time limit. Participants then indicated their confidence by stating whether they were certain or uncertain about their response. Throughout this verbal condition, participants held a pen above the video camera to maintain a similar posture as in the pendulum condition (Fig. 2). We counter-balanced the order of these conditions across participants.

Setup.
Figure 2.

Setup.

Notes: In the verbal condition, participants answered verbally while holding a pen (A); in the pendulum condition, they mentally asked a pendulum then watched its movement (B).

Pendulum condition

In the pendulum condition, after each character stream, participants mentally asked the pendulum whether the target was present. As instructed, vertical movement meant “yes” and horizontal movement meant “no”. The participants watched the pendulum’s movement then verbally classified it as “yes” or “no”. The video camera recorded the movement and the experimenter noted any discrepancies between the participants’ classifications and the actual swinging. Overall, there were few discrepancies so we deferred to the participants’ judgements.3 The median response time was 20.64 s (SD = 14.81), considerably longer than in the verbal condition (Mdn =5.53 s, SD = 3.01). Participants then indicated their confidence by stating whether they agreed with, disagreed with, or were uncertain about the pendulum’s response. For example, sometimes the pendulum swung in a vertical “yes” pattern, but the participant disagreed with it and thought the correct answer should have been “no”. Measuring confidence in this way allowed us to make coarse comparisons between the two conditions.

If the pendulum was not moving in a consistent pattern, the experimenter suggested to continue focusing on its movement before stating the response. The pendulum eventually moved in every trial. After the study, we fully debriefed participants. The protocol was approved by the McGill Faculty of Medicine Institutional Review Board.

Dependent variables

In each trial, we measured accuracy: whether participants were correct about the target’s presence. We then used signal detection theory to calculate sensitivity and bias (Green and Swets 1966). Sensitivity (d) refers to how well people could detect the target; higher values mean better detection and zero values mean chance-level performance. Bias (or criterion, c) refers to the overall bias in declaring the target present or absent. Higher bias values mean a higher probability of declaring the target absent and zero values mean no bias toward either response.

Analysis

We had two sets of hypotheses. First, we expected that personality measures would predict performance. For each condition, we used mixed-effect logistic regression to predict the accuracy of each trial given the four personality measures. We chose a family-wise Type I error rate of 0.10, giving Bonferroni-corrected α values of 0.025 for each of the four predictors. Next, we tested two analogous linear models predicting average (not per-trial) sensitivity then average bias as response variables. Each of these three models constituted separate families for error control. All regressions were forced-entry. Their assumptions were reasonable besides the lack of specification error: as an exploratory study, we could not measure all (and only) relevant variables. Our logistic model for accuracy had high statistical power; our linear models for sensitivity and bias did not. For overall model fit statistics, see Table A2.

Second, we assessed how participants’ confidence in their responses related to performance between the conditions. We had five pre-specified hypotheses based on the Ouija board findings (see Appendix 1; Gauchou et al. 2012). We compared per-trial accuracy using chi-square tests as well as average sensitivity and bias using t-tests. Hypotheses about each of these dependent variables constituted a family. A family-wise α of 0.10 gave Bonferroni-corrected α values of 0.02 for each test. With our intended sample size (N=80) and assuming a 10% exclusion rate, we had 90% power to detect medium-sized effects (Cohen’s d=0.43). All assumptions were reasonable for these tests.

We excluded participants who gave the same response to over 80% of the trials in either condition. For example, if a participant responded “present” to 85% of the verbal trials, we omitted the data from both conditions. This exclusion criterion omitted participants who deviated from the task instructions by giving near-constant responses; it also allowed us to calculate signal detection theory values. Beyond these considerations, the 80% criterion was chosen arbitrarily. This criterion excluded 16 participants in the verbal condition and 3 in the pendulum condition (2 of whom were already excluded), leaving 63 remaining in total. In addition, two participants did not complete all of the questionnaires and so were excluded only from the personality analyses. Our exclusion criteria, variables, hypotheses, and analyses were pre-registered online.4

There was one difference between our pre-registered procedure and our analysis here. We initially intended to see how personality measures correlated with differences in condition performance within each participant. However, given the low performance in both conditions, we instead decided to analyze how personality measures predicted performance within each condition. This only changed the dependent variables in the personality models (from difference scores to raw scores). All other deviations from our pre-register procedure are explicitly labeled as exploratory and do not use significance testing.

Our analysis focuses on effect sizes (Cumming 2014). For mean differences, we report a robust version of Cohen’s d – symbolized as dR – which measures condition differences in standard deviations. It equals the 20% trimmed mean divided by the 20% Winsorized standard deviation (Algina et al. 2005). Square brackets throughout denote bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (Kirby and Gerlanc 2013).

The analyses used R 3.3.3 (R Core Team 2016), with packages lme4 1.1-12 for mixed-effects logistic regression, bootES 1.2 for bootstrapped effect sizes (Kirby and Gerlanc 2013), Hmisc 4.0-2 for bootstrapped confidence intervals, MuMIn 1.15.6 for logistic regression R2, and ggplot2 2.2.1 (Wickham 2009) for graphs.

Results

Overall, accuracy and sensitivity were low in both the verbal (57%[55%,60%], d=0.26[0.15,0.37]) and pendulum conditions (53%[51%,56%], d=0.12[0.04,0.21], Fig. 3A). Bias, however, was higher in the verbal condition (c=0.2[0.15,0.24]) than in the pendulum condition (c=0[0.05,0.06], t(62)=6.7, P<0.001, Fig. 3B). Thus, participants were more likely to declare the target absent in the verbal condition, yet they showed little bias in the pendulum condition. The difference was 1.096[0.76,1.54] standard deviations (dR) – a large effect. Within each participant, exploratory analyses showed that bias correlated between the conditions (r=0.323[0.010,0.600]) but we did not see a similar correlation for sensitivity (r=0.199[0.040,0.420]).

Sensitivity (A) and bias (B) by task.
Figure 3.

Sensitivity (A) and bias (B) by task.

Notes: Bias was higher in the verbal task. Dots show means, errors bars show 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals, circles show data points, and width estimates underlying distribution.

Several personality measures predicted performance. In the verbal condition, locus of control predicted sensitivity: people who reported feeling more control over their lives performed better than those who reported less control (Fig. 4A). For every one-point increase in locus of control, sensitivity (d) increased by 0.02 units (P=0.008). Need for cognition also predicted verbal performance: people with higher need for cognition scores performed less accurately (odds ratio =0.982, P=0.023).

Sensitivity in the verbal condition given locus of control (A; r=.183) and sensitivity in the pendulum condition given transliminality (B; r=.310), ignoring all other predictors.
Figure 4.

Sensitivity in the verbal condition given locus of control (A; r=.183) and sensitivity in the pendulum condition given transliminality (B; r=.310), ignoring all other predictors.

Note: Each circle shows data from one participant.

In the pendulum condition, transliminality predicted performance. People with higher transliminality scores – those more sensitive to subtle stimuli – performed better than those with lower scores (Fig. 4B). For every one-point increase in transliminality, sensitivity increased by 0.044 units (P=0.009). See Table 1 for full statistics.

Table 1.

Personality predictors of verbal and pendulum performance

DVTaskPredictorBSEzP
AccuracyVerbalLocus of control0.0170.0072.5930.010*
Transliminality0.0040.0200.1840.854
Need for cognition−0.0180.008−2.2680.023*
Faith in intuition0.0050.0051.1240.261
PendulumLocus of control0.0100.0061.6100.107
Transliminality0.0520.0202.6490.008*
Need for cognition−0.0050.008−0.6050.545
Faith in intuition0.0000.0040.0920.927
SensitivityVerbalLocus of control0.0200.0072.7610.008*
Transliminality0.0050.0210.2320.818
Need for cognition−0.0190.008−2.2260.030
Faith in intuition0.0050.0051.0370.304
PendulumLocus of control0.0080.0051.5690.122
Transliminality0.0440.0162.7100.009*
Need for cognition−0.0020.006−0.3110.757
Faith in intuition0.0000.004−0.0450.964
BiasVerbalLocus of control0.0000.0020.1270.900
Transliminality−0.0110.007−1.4280.159
Need for cognition0.0000.0030.0050.996
Faith in intuition−0.0030.002−1.7680.082
PendulumLocus of control−0.0020.003−0.6250.534
Transliminality0.0070.0100.7300.468
Need for cognition0.0050.0041.1920.238
Faith in intuition−0.0050.002−2.2520.028
DVTaskPredictorBSEzP
AccuracyVerbalLocus of control0.0170.0072.5930.010*
Transliminality0.0040.0200.1840.854
Need for cognition−0.0180.008−2.2680.023*
Faith in intuition0.0050.0051.1240.261
PendulumLocus of control0.0100.0061.6100.107
Transliminality0.0520.0202.6490.008*
Need for cognition−0.0050.008−0.6050.545
Faith in intuition0.0000.0040.0920.927
SensitivityVerbalLocus of control0.0200.0072.7610.008*
Transliminality0.0050.0210.2320.818
Need for cognition−0.0190.008−2.2260.030
Faith in intuition0.0050.0051.0370.304
PendulumLocus of control0.0080.0051.5690.122
Transliminality0.0440.0162.7100.009*
Need for cognition−0.0020.006−0.3110.757
Faith in intuition0.0000.004−0.0450.964
BiasVerbalLocus of control0.0000.0020.1270.900
Transliminality−0.0110.007−1.4280.159
Need for cognition0.0000.0030.0050.996
Faith in intuition−0.0030.002−1.7680.082
PendulumLocus of control−0.0020.003−0.6250.534
Transliminality0.0070.0100.7300.468
Need for cognition0.0050.0041.1920.238
Faith in intuition−0.0050.002−2.2520.028

Notes: Locus of control and need for cognition predicted verbal performance while transliminality predicted pendulum performance. Bonferroni-corrected α values were 0.025.

Table 1.

Personality predictors of verbal and pendulum performance

DVTaskPredictorBSEzP
AccuracyVerbalLocus of control0.0170.0072.5930.010*
Transliminality0.0040.0200.1840.854
Need for cognition−0.0180.008−2.2680.023*
Faith in intuition0.0050.0051.1240.261
PendulumLocus of control0.0100.0061.6100.107
Transliminality0.0520.0202.6490.008*
Need for cognition−0.0050.008−0.6050.545
Faith in intuition0.0000.0040.0920.927
SensitivityVerbalLocus of control0.0200.0072.7610.008*
Transliminality0.0050.0210.2320.818
Need for cognition−0.0190.008−2.2260.030
Faith in intuition0.0050.0051.0370.304
PendulumLocus of control0.0080.0051.5690.122
Transliminality0.0440.0162.7100.009*
Need for cognition−0.0020.006−0.3110.757
Faith in intuition0.0000.004−0.0450.964
BiasVerbalLocus of control0.0000.0020.1270.900
Transliminality−0.0110.007−1.4280.159
Need for cognition0.0000.0030.0050.996
Faith in intuition−0.0030.002−1.7680.082
PendulumLocus of control−0.0020.003−0.6250.534
Transliminality0.0070.0100.7300.468
Need for cognition0.0050.0041.1920.238
Faith in intuition−0.0050.002−2.2520.028
DVTaskPredictorBSEzP
AccuracyVerbalLocus of control0.0170.0072.5930.010*
Transliminality0.0040.0200.1840.854
Need for cognition−0.0180.008−2.2680.023*
Faith in intuition0.0050.0051.1240.261
PendulumLocus of control0.0100.0061.6100.107
Transliminality0.0520.0202.6490.008*
Need for cognition−0.0050.008−0.6050.545
Faith in intuition0.0000.0040.0920.927
SensitivityVerbalLocus of control0.0200.0072.7610.008*
Transliminality0.0050.0210.2320.818
Need for cognition−0.0190.008−2.2260.030
Faith in intuition0.0050.0051.0370.304
PendulumLocus of control0.0080.0051.5690.122
Transliminality0.0440.0162.7100.009*
Need for cognition−0.0020.006−0.3110.757
Faith in intuition0.0000.004−0.0450.964
BiasVerbalLocus of control0.0000.0020.1270.900
Transliminality−0.0110.007−1.4280.159
Need for cognition0.0000.0030.0050.996
Faith in intuition−0.0030.002−1.7680.082
PendulumLocus of control−0.0020.003−0.6250.534
Transliminality0.0070.0100.7300.468
Need for cognition0.0050.0041.1920.238
Faith in intuition−0.0050.002−2.2520.028

Notes: Locus of control and need for cognition predicted verbal performance while transliminality predicted pendulum performance. Bonferroni-corrected α values were 0.025.

Beyond these personality measures, we also found gender differences in an exploratory analysis. Women and men differed in their sensitivity: women outperformed men in the verbal condition (dR=1.24[0.64,2.06], Fig. 5A) but not in the pendulum condition (dR=0.03[0.99,0.99], Fig. 5B). We did not see similarly strong gender differences in bias (verbal: dR=0.31[1.49,0.74]; pendulum: dR=0.16[0.87,0.9]).

Sensitivity by condition and gender.
Figure 5.

Sensitivity by condition and gender.

Notes: Women outperformed men in the verbal condition (A) but not the pendulum condition (B). Dots show means, errors bars show 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals, circles show data points, and width estimates underlying distribution.

For confidence, performance was highest when participants felt certain about their answers (see Table A1 and Fig. A1). Ideomotor response always underperformed verbal guessing, unlike the Ouija board findings (Gauchou et al. 2012). We next conducted a follow-up study to examine this discrepancy.

Study 2

Gauchou et al. (2012) found the largest difference between verbal and ideomotor performance when participants felt least certain about their responses. Namely, when guessing, participants performed best when responding with a Ouija board. To increase the uncertainty (and difficulty) of our task, we doubled the stimulus presentation speed. We then tested whether this increase in uncertainty would give results comparable to those with a Ouija board. This study also allowed us to replicate some of the findings of Study 1 (cf. Open Science Collaboration 2015).

Methods

We recruited 40 additional undergraduate students to participate. After exclusions, 34 participants remained, 59% of whom were female (compared to 87% in Study 1). Besides gender, the samples of the two studies were similar. The participants were on average 20.2 years old (SD = 0.9); many studied psychology (44%), commonly in the second (32%) or fourth year (35%) of their degree. Few had held a pendulum before (24%) and most were right-handed (85%). The rest of the methodology was identical to Study 1 except that the stimulus timing was 17 ms rather than 33 ms (see Fig. 1).

Our sample size was limited by feasibility constraints. We did not have high statistical power to predict performance based on personality, but we did have the power to test some of the large effects seen in Study 1.

Results

Accuracy was at chance level for both the verbal (51%[47%,54%]) and pendulum conditions (50%[47%,54%], Fig. 6A). The lower accuracy was likely due to the relatively brief stimulus presentation time (17 ms) which reduced visibility and caused a floor effect. As in Study 1, bias was higher in the verbal condition (c=0.18[0.11,0.24]) than in the pendulum condition (c=0.04[0.04,0.12], t(32)=2.59, P=0.014, Fig. 6B). Thus, people again showed almost no bias in the pendulum condition. The difference between the conditions was 0.466[0.14,0.9] standard deviations (dR).

Sensitivity (A) and bias (B) by condition.
Figure 6.

Sensitivity (A) and bias (B) by condition.

Notes: As in Study 1, bias was higher in the verbal condition. Dots show means, errors bars show 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals, circles show data points, and width estimates underlying distribution.

Unlike in Study 1, personality measures did not predict performance (see Table A3), possibly due to the floor effects and reduced power. Further, exploratory analyses showed that sensitivity in the verbal condition negatively correlated with sensitivity in the pendulum condition (r=0.364[0.630,0.000]). We did not see a similar correlation for bias (r=0.081[0.270,0.440]). Thus, using a more difficult task we were only able to partly reproduce the pattern of results found in Study 1. Supplementary data sets, including personality measures, reaction time, and all other dependent variables, are available online at https://osf.io/xe9mk/.

Discussion

For centuries, people have consulted hand-held pendulums in an attempt to aid decision-making. We examined which personality measures predicted performance when deciding about the presence of visual stimuli. Participants either responded verbally or by “asking” a pendulum and watching its motion after we paired particular movements with different answers.

Several personality measures predicted performance. In the verbal condition, people who felt more control over their lives (locus of control) performed better than those who felt less control. In the pendulum condition, people high in transliminality – those sensitive to subtle stimuli – performed better than those low in transliminality. Indeed, transliminality may capture some important aspects of pendulum use. Pendulum users would ideally be sensitive to their subtle movements; transliminality correlates with detection of subtle internal and external stimuli (e.g. Thalbourne and Houran 2000). Pendulum users should also be open to the idea of consulting a pendulum (Lundstrom 2010); similarly, transliminality correlates with openness to experience and paranormal beliefs (Lange et al. 2000).

Although accuracy was comparable in both conditions, pendulum responses showed relatively little bias. Both conditions of the task were difficult, which usually increases uncertainty and bias, making people more likely to declare the target absent (Green and Swets 1966). In both studies, however, bias was higher in the verbal condition but lower – around 0 – in the pendulum condition. Thus, consistent with the views of some pendulum users (e.g. Lundstrom 2010), decisions made with pendulums may be less biased – though not more accurate.

Given this difference in bias, our findings suggest that people employ a different decision strategy when using a pendulum versus responding verbally. In other words, unconscious pendulum movements are not equivalent to conscious responses; instead, something changes in the process of decision-making. These results are consistent with other studies finding different decision strategies in ideomotor versus verbal responses (e.g. Marcel 1993; Gauchou et al. 2012). Nevertheless, the largest limitation of our study is that we cannot isolate this mechanism or the cause of the differences between the conditions. Perhaps focusing attention away from the decision itself (cf. Dijksterhuis and Strick 2016), using a more introspective mindset (Wilson and Schooler 1991; Tordesillas and Chaiken 1999), or taking more time to ponder the questions could explain these differences. Or, as one reviewer suggested, merely giving the suggestion that pendulums reflect unconscious knowledge could have affected their bias. Alas, in our study, we chose a more natural method of pendulum use at the expense of causal precision.

Our results somewhat differed from those found with Ouija boards. In particular, Gauchou et al. (2012) found that ideomotor performance can exceed verbal performance; we did not see this relationship with pendulums. This could have been due to several factors, such as the difference in ideomotor tool (Ouija board vs. pendulum) or type of question asked (memory vs. visual detection). Indeed, given that we only examined perceptual decisions, it is unclear how far our findings can generalize. Future studies could explore what other types of decisions people can accurately answer through ideomotor response (Olson and Raz, in progress). Such studies could help determine the mechanisms and boundaries of unconscious decision-making.

Still, many questions remain. If people use a different decision-making strategy with a pendulum, what is its mechanism and phenomenology? Do the dynamics of the pendulum movement, such as speed or direction, predict accuracy? Will our finding of a reduced decision bias when using a pendulum generalize to real-world decisions? Answering these questions will help understand the puzzling practice of consulting a pendulum, and it may even help improve decision-making.

Authors’ Contribution

J.A.O. designed the study and analyzed the data; E.J. helped collect the data; and A.R. provided feedback. All contributed to the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the BIAL Foundation [No. 534/14 to J.A.O.] and the Joseph-Armand Bombardier Scholarship from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. This work was also supported by the Canada Research Chair program, Discovery and Discovery Acceleration Supplement grants from NSERC, and Canadian Institutes of Health Research [to A.R.].

Footnotes

1

Some pendulum users recommend calibrating the movements to the individual (Lundstrom 2010). They suggest asking the pendulum to “show yes” and “show no” rather than choosing vertical and horizontal movements in advance. In pilot testing, these “yes” and “no” movements indeed varied across participants. Alas, to reduce individual variation, we decided to keep the movements constant.

2

If the pendulum did not swing, the experimenter would state: “Sometimes it takes a bit of time – just visualise the pendulum swinging side to side, side to side.” If it still did not swing or showed little movement: “It’s okay, sometimes it takes a bit of practice.”

3

We hope to analyze the pendulum movement based on the video data in a future article (Olson and Raz, in progress).

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dabney Ewin for helping with the pendulum setup; Docky Duncan, Mathieu Landry, and Ronald Rensink for helping with the design; Jason Da Silva Castanheira, Kylar D’Aigle, Pendie Lam, Madalina Prostean, and Léah Suissa-Rocheleau for running participants; Despina Artenie, Julia Biris, Mariève Cyr, and Carina Fan for comments; and three anonymous reviewers for their feedback.

Conflict of interest statement. None declared.

References

Algina
J
,
Keselman
HJ
,
Penfield
RD.
An alternative to Cohen’s standardized mean difference effect size: a robust parameter and confidence interval in the two independent groups case
.
Psychol Methods
2005
;
10
:
317
28
. http://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.3.317.

Banachek
.
Psychophysiological Thought Reading (First).
Houston, TX
:
Magic Inspirations
,
2002
.

Burger
JM.
Locus of control, motivation, and expectancy: predicting hypnotic susceptibility from personality variables
.
J Res Pers
1981
;
15
:
523
37
. http://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(81)90048-9.

Cacioppo
JT
,
Petty
RE.
The need for cognition
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
1982
;
42
:
116
31
.

Crawley
SE
,
French
CC
,
Yesson
SA.
Evidence for transliminality from a subliminal card-guessing task
.
Perception
2002
;
31
:
887
92
.

Cumming
G.
The new statistics: why and how
.
Psychol Sci
2014
;
25
:
7
29
. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613504966.

Dijksterhuis
A
,
Strick
M.
A case for thinking without consciousness
.
Perspect Psychol Sci
2016
;
11
:
117
32
.

Duttweiler
PC.
The Internal Control Index: a newly developed measure of locus of control
.
Educ Psychol Meas
1984
;
44
:
209
21
. http://doi.org/10.1177/0013164484442004.

Easton
RD
,
Shor
RE.
An experimental analysis of the Chevreul pendulum illusion
.
J Gen Psychol
1976
;
95
:
111
25
.

Easton
RD
,
Shor
RE.
Augmented and delayed feedback in the Chevreul pendulum illusion
.
J Gen Psychol
1977
;
97
:
167
77
.

Epstein
S
,
Pacini
R
,
Denes-Raj
V
et al.
Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
1996
;
71
:
390.
http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390.

Ewin
D.
101 Things I Wish I’d Known When I Started Using Hypnosis
.
Carmarthen, UK
:
Crown House Publishing
,
2009
.

Eysenck
HJ
,
Furneaux
WD.
Primary and secondary suggestibility: an experimental and statistical study
.
J Exp Psychol
1945
;
35
:
485.

Gauchou
HL
,
Rensink
RA
,
Fels
S.
Expression of nonconscious knowledge via ideomotor actions
.
Conscious Cogn
2012
;
21
:
976
82
.

Gordon
AM
,
Rosenbaum
DA.
Conscious and subconscious arm movements: application of signal detection theory to motor control
.
Bull Psychon Soc
1984
;
22
:
214
6
.

Green
DM
,
Swets
JA.
Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics
(Vol.
1
).
New York
:
Wiley
,
1966
.

Houran
J
,
Thalbourne
MA
,
Lange
R.
Methodological note: erratum and comment on the use of the revised transliminality scale
.
Conscious Cogn
2003
;
12
:
140
4
.

Karlin
RA
,
Hill
A
,
Messer
S.
Responding and failing to respond to both hypnosis and a kinesthetic illusion, Chevreul’s pendulum
.
Int J Clin Exp Hypn
2007
;
56
:
83
98
. http://doi.org/10.1080/00207140701673118.

Kirby
KN
,
Gerlanc
D.
BootES: an R package for bootstrap confidence intervals on effect sizes
.
Behav Res Methods
2013
;
45
:
905
27
. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0330-5.

Lange
R
,
Thalbourne
MA
,
Houran
J
et al.
The revised transliminality scale: reliability and validity data from a Rasch top-down purification procedure
.
Conscious Cogn
2000
;
9
:
591
617
. http://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.2000.0472.

Lundstrom
M.
What to do When you Can’t Decide: Useful Tools for Finding the Answers Within
.
Louisville, CO
:
Sounds True
,
2010
.

Marcel
AJ.
Slippage in the unity of consciousness. In:
Bock
GR
,
Marsh
J
(eds.),
Experimental and Theoretical Studies of Consciousness: Ciba Foundation Symposium
174
,
1993
,
168
86
.

Nakagawa
S
,
Schielzeth
H.
A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models
.
Methods Ecol Evol
2013
;
4
:
133
42
. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x.

Nash
M
,
Barnier
AJ.
The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice
.
Oxford, England
:
Oxford University Press
,
2012
.

Nielsen
G
,
Polansky
J.
Pendulum Power: A Mystery You Can See, a Power You Can Feel
.
Rochester, Vermont
:
Inner Traditions/Bear & Co
.,
1987
.

Olson
JA
,
Amlani
AA
,
Raz
A
et al.
Influencing choice without awareness
.
Conscious Cogn
2015
;
37
:
225
36
. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.004.

Open Science Collaboration
.
Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science
.
Science
2015
;
349
:
aac4716
. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716.

Peirce
JW.
Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy
.
Front Neuroinform
2009
;
2
:
10
. http://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.11.010.2008.

Potter
MC.
Short-term conceptual memory for pictures
.
J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem
1976
;
2
:
509
22
.

R Core Team
. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria,
2016
.

Spitz
HH.
Nonconscious Movements: From Mystical Messages to Facilitated Communication
.
Mahwah, NJ
:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc
.,
1997
.

Thalbourne
MA
,
Houran
J.
Transliminality, the mental experience inventory and tolerance of ambiguity
.
Pers Individ Dif
2000
;
28
:
853
63
.

Thalbourne
MA
,
Crawley
SE
,
Houran
J.
Temporal lobe lability in the highly transliminal mind
.
Pers Individ Dif
2003
;
35
:
1965
74
. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00044-8.

Tordesillas
RS
,
Chaiken
S.
Thinking too much or too little? The effects of introspection on the decision-making process
.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull
1999
;
25
:
625
31
.

Wegner
DM.
The Illusion of Conscious Will
.
Cambridge, MA
:
MIT Press
,
2003
.

Wegner
DM
,
Ansfield
M
,
Pilloff
D.
The putt and the pendulum: ironic effects of the mental control of action
.
Psychol Sci
1998
;
9
:
196
9
.

Wickham
H.
Ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis
.
New York
:
Springer
,
2009
.

Wilson
TD
,
Schooler
JW.
Thinking too much: introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
1991
;
60
:
181
92
. http://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.60.2.181.

Appendix 1

Study 1 Supplementary Results

We had several pre-specified hypotheses about the relationship between performance and confidence (based on Gauchou et al. 2012):

  1. Performance would differ between the verbal and pendulum conditions.

  2. Performance would differ in the verbal condition when participants are uncertain compared to the pendulum condition overall.

  3. Performance would differ in the verbal condition when participants are uncertain compared to the pendulum condition when participants are uncertain.

  4. In the pendulum condition, performance would differ when participants agree or disagree with the pendulum’s response compared to when they are uncertain about it.

  5. In the pendulum condition, performance would differ based on whether participants agree or disagree with the pendulum’s response.

See Table A1 and Fig. A1 for tests of these hypotheses.

Table A1.

Statistics for overall performance comparisons

HypothesisMeasureTest statisticP
1. Verbal pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 3.90.048
Sensitivityt(60) = 1.9880.051
Biast(62) = 6.7<0.001*
2. Verbal uncertain pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 0.0480.827
Sensitivityt(94) = 1.940.055
Biast(107) = −4.009<0.001*
3. Verbal uncertain pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 2.30.129
Sensitivityt(78) = 0.0550.956
Biast(59) = −3.40.001*
4. Pendulum agree or disagree Pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 4.0830.043
Sensitivityt(83) = −2.4350.017*
Biast(61) = −1.0160.314
5. Pendulum agree pendulum disagreeAccuracyχ2(1) = 23.462<0.001*
Sensitivityt(69) = −5.739<0.001*
Biast(48) = −1.0870.282
HypothesisMeasureTest statisticP
1. Verbal pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 3.90.048
Sensitivityt(60) = 1.9880.051
Biast(62) = 6.7<0.001*
2. Verbal uncertain pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 0.0480.827
Sensitivityt(94) = 1.940.055
Biast(107) = −4.009<0.001*
3. Verbal uncertain pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 2.30.129
Sensitivityt(78) = 0.0550.956
Biast(59) = −3.40.001*
4. Pendulum agree or disagree Pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 4.0830.043
Sensitivityt(83) = −2.4350.017*
Biast(61) = −1.0160.314
5. Pendulum agree pendulum disagreeAccuracyχ2(1) = 23.462<0.001*
Sensitivityt(69) = −5.739<0.001*
Biast(48) = −1.0870.282

Notes: Bias (c) differed between verbal and pendulum conditions and sensitivity (d) differed in the pendulum condition based on agreement. Each test had a Bonferroni-corrected α of 0.02. For the fit statistics of the personality models, see Table A2.

Table A1.

Statistics for overall performance comparisons

HypothesisMeasureTest statisticP
1. Verbal pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 3.90.048
Sensitivityt(60) = 1.9880.051
Biast(62) = 6.7<0.001*
2. Verbal uncertain pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 0.0480.827
Sensitivityt(94) = 1.940.055
Biast(107) = −4.009<0.001*
3. Verbal uncertain pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 2.30.129
Sensitivityt(78) = 0.0550.956
Biast(59) = −3.40.001*
4. Pendulum agree or disagree Pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 4.0830.043
Sensitivityt(83) = −2.4350.017*
Biast(61) = −1.0160.314
5. Pendulum agree pendulum disagreeAccuracyχ2(1) = 23.462<0.001*
Sensitivityt(69) = −5.739<0.001*
Biast(48) = −1.0870.282
HypothesisMeasureTest statisticP
1. Verbal pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 3.90.048
Sensitivityt(60) = 1.9880.051
Biast(62) = 6.7<0.001*
2. Verbal uncertain pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 0.0480.827
Sensitivityt(94) = 1.940.055
Biast(107) = −4.009<0.001*
3. Verbal uncertain pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 2.30.129
Sensitivityt(78) = 0.0550.956
Biast(59) = −3.40.001*
4. Pendulum agree or disagree Pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 4.0830.043
Sensitivityt(83) = −2.4350.017*
Biast(61) = −1.0160.314
5. Pendulum agree pendulum disagreeAccuracyχ2(1) = 23.462<0.001*
Sensitivityt(69) = −5.739<0.001*
Biast(48) = −1.0870.282

Notes: Bias (c) differed between verbal and pendulum conditions and sensitivity (d) differed in the pendulum condition based on agreement. Each test had a Bonferroni-corrected α of 0.02. For the fit statistics of the personality models, see Table A2.

Table A2.

Personality model fit statistics

ConditionMeasureTest statisticPR2
VerbalAccuracyχ2(6)=8.519.074.009
SensitivityF(4,54)=2.399.061.151
BiasF(4,56)=2.531.050.153
PendulumAccuracyχ2(6)=9.515.049.008
SensitivityF(4,56)=1.772.147.112
BiasF(4,56)=1.711.160.109
ConditionMeasureTest statisticPR2
VerbalAccuracyχ2(6)=8.519.074.009
SensitivityF(4,54)=2.399.061.151
BiasF(4,56)=2.531.050.153
PendulumAccuracyχ2(6)=9.515.049.008
SensitivityF(4,56)=1.772.147.112
BiasF(4,56)=1.711.160.109

Notes: Accuracy uses a mixed-effect logistic model and sensitivity and bias use linear models. R2 values for accuracy account for both fixed and random factors (Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2013).

Table A2.

Personality model fit statistics

ConditionMeasureTest statisticPR2
VerbalAccuracyχ2(6)=8.519.074.009
SensitivityF(4,54)=2.399.061.151
BiasF(4,56)=2.531.050.153
PendulumAccuracyχ2(6)=9.515.049.008
SensitivityF(4,56)=1.772.147.112
BiasF(4,56)=1.711.160.109
ConditionMeasureTest statisticPR2
VerbalAccuracyχ2(6)=8.519.074.009
SensitivityF(4,54)=2.399.061.151
BiasF(4,56)=2.531.050.153
PendulumAccuracyχ2(6)=9.515.049.008
SensitivityF(4,56)=1.772.147.112
BiasF(4,56)=1.711.160.109

Notes: Accuracy uses a mixed-effect logistic model and sensitivity and bias use linear models. R2 values for accuracy account for both fixed and random factors (Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2013).

Performance by confidence.
Figure A1.

Performance by confidence.

Notes: Sensitivity was highest when participants felt confident in their verbal response or agreed with the pendulum response. Bias showed relatively little difference. Dots show means, errors bars show 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals, circles show data points, and width estimates underlying distribution.

Study 2 Supplementary Results

Table A3 shows the personality predictors (cf. Table 1) and Table A4 shows the performance differences (cf. Table A1).

Table A3.

Personality predictors of verbal and pendulum performance

DVTaskPredictorBSEzP
AccuracyVerbalLocus of control−0.0060.008−0.7820.434
Transliminality0.0020.0220.0960.923
Need for cognition0.0070.0070.9780.328
Faith in intuition0.0020.0060.3810.703
PendulumLocus of control0.0030.0080.3270.744
Transliminality−0.0290.022−1.3080.191
Need for cognition−0.0080.007−1.0900.276
Faith in intuition0.0040.0060.7350.462
SensitivityVerbalLocus of control−0.0050.005−0.8970.378
Transliminality0.0130.0160.8180.421
Need for cognition0.0050.0051.0460.305
Faith in intuition0.0020.0040.4220.677
PendulumLocus of control0.0010.0070.2000.843
Transliminality−0.0250.020−1.2300.229
Need for cognition−0.0080.007−1.2270.230
Faith in intuition0.0020.0060.4390.664
BiasVerbalLocus of control−0.0060.004−1.5920.123
Transliminality−0.0090.010−0.9020.375
Need for cognition−0.0030.003−0.9440.354
Faith in intuition0.0000.0030.0340.973
PendulumLocus of control−0.0050.004−1.2450.223
Transliminality−0.0030.011−0.2610.796
Need for cognition−0.0080.004−2.1690.039
Faith in intuition0.0000.003−0.0910.928
DVTaskPredictorBSEzP
AccuracyVerbalLocus of control−0.0060.008−0.7820.434
Transliminality0.0020.0220.0960.923
Need for cognition0.0070.0070.9780.328
Faith in intuition0.0020.0060.3810.703
PendulumLocus of control0.0030.0080.3270.744
Transliminality−0.0290.022−1.3080.191
Need for cognition−0.0080.007−1.0900.276
Faith in intuition0.0040.0060.7350.462
SensitivityVerbalLocus of control−0.0050.005−0.8970.378
Transliminality0.0130.0160.8180.421
Need for cognition0.0050.0051.0460.305
Faith in intuition0.0020.0040.4220.677
PendulumLocus of control0.0010.0070.2000.843
Transliminality−0.0250.020−1.2300.229
Need for cognition−0.0080.007−1.2270.230
Faith in intuition0.0020.0060.4390.664
BiasVerbalLocus of control−0.0060.004−1.5920.123
Transliminality−0.0090.010−0.9020.375
Need for cognition−0.0030.003−0.9440.354
Faith in intuition0.0000.0030.0340.973
PendulumLocus of control−0.0050.004−1.2450.223
Transliminality−0.0030.011−0.2610.796
Need for cognition−0.0080.004−2.1690.039
Faith in intuition0.0000.003−0.0910.928

Note: Bonferroni-corrected α values were 0.025.

Table A3.

Personality predictors of verbal and pendulum performance

DVTaskPredictorBSEzP
AccuracyVerbalLocus of control−0.0060.008−0.7820.434
Transliminality0.0020.0220.0960.923
Need for cognition0.0070.0070.9780.328
Faith in intuition0.0020.0060.3810.703
PendulumLocus of control0.0030.0080.3270.744
Transliminality−0.0290.022−1.3080.191
Need for cognition−0.0080.007−1.0900.276
Faith in intuition0.0040.0060.7350.462
SensitivityVerbalLocus of control−0.0050.005−0.8970.378
Transliminality0.0130.0160.8180.421
Need for cognition0.0050.0051.0460.305
Faith in intuition0.0020.0040.4220.677
PendulumLocus of control0.0010.0070.2000.843
Transliminality−0.0250.020−1.2300.229
Need for cognition−0.0080.007−1.2270.230
Faith in intuition0.0020.0060.4390.664
BiasVerbalLocus of control−0.0060.004−1.5920.123
Transliminality−0.0090.010−0.9020.375
Need for cognition−0.0030.003−0.9440.354
Faith in intuition0.0000.0030.0340.973
PendulumLocus of control−0.0050.004−1.2450.223
Transliminality−0.0030.011−0.2610.796
Need for cognition−0.0080.004−2.1690.039
Faith in intuition0.0000.003−0.0910.928
DVTaskPredictorBSEzP
AccuracyVerbalLocus of control−0.0060.008−0.7820.434
Transliminality0.0020.0220.0960.923
Need for cognition0.0070.0070.9780.328
Faith in intuition0.0020.0060.3810.703
PendulumLocus of control0.0030.0080.3270.744
Transliminality−0.0290.022−1.3080.191
Need for cognition−0.0080.007−1.0900.276
Faith in intuition0.0040.0060.7350.462
SensitivityVerbalLocus of control−0.0050.005−0.8970.378
Transliminality0.0130.0160.8180.421
Need for cognition0.0050.0051.0460.305
Faith in intuition0.0020.0040.4220.677
PendulumLocus of control0.0010.0070.2000.843
Transliminality−0.0250.020−1.2300.229
Need for cognition−0.0080.007−1.2270.230
Faith in intuition0.0020.0060.4390.664
BiasVerbalLocus of control−0.0060.004−1.5920.123
Transliminality−0.0090.010−0.9020.375
Need for cognition−0.0030.003−0.9440.354
Faith in intuition0.0000.0030.0340.973
PendulumLocus of control−0.0050.004−1.2450.223
Transliminality−0.0030.011−0.2610.796
Need for cognition−0.0080.004−2.1690.039
Faith in intuition0.0000.003−0.0910.928

Note: Bonferroni-corrected α values were 0.025.

Table A4.

Statistics for overall performance comparisons

HypothesisMeasureTest statisticP
1. Verbal pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 3.90.048
Sensitivityt(31) = 0.2320.818
Biast(32) = 2.590.014*
2. Verbal uncertain pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 0.0480.827
Sensitivityt(52) = −0.3410.735
Biast(59) = −2.1790.033
3. Verbal uncertain pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 2.30.129
Sensitivityt(27) = −1.6490.111
Biast(35) = −0.3820.705
4. Pend. agree or disagree Pend. uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 4.0830.043
Sensitivityt(30) = −2.4370.021
Biast(44) = 1.3720.177
5. Pendulum agree pendulum disagreeAccuracyχ2(1) = 23.462<0.001*
Sensitivityt(18) = −0.5450.592
Biast(18) = −1.4920.153
HypothesisMeasureTest statisticP
1. Verbal pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 3.90.048
Sensitivityt(31) = 0.2320.818
Biast(32) = 2.590.014*
2. Verbal uncertain pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 0.0480.827
Sensitivityt(52) = −0.3410.735
Biast(59) = −2.1790.033
3. Verbal uncertain pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 2.30.129
Sensitivityt(27) = −1.6490.111
Biast(35) = −0.3820.705
4. Pend. agree or disagree Pend. uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 4.0830.043
Sensitivityt(30) = −2.4370.021
Biast(44) = 1.3720.177
5. Pendulum agree pendulum disagreeAccuracyχ2(1) = 23.462<0.001*
Sensitivityt(18) = −0.5450.592
Biast(18) = −1.4920.153

Notes: Bias (c) differed between verbal and pendulum conditions and accuracy differed in the pendulum condition based on agreement. Each test had a Bonferroni-corrected α of 0.02.

Table A4.

Statistics for overall performance comparisons

HypothesisMeasureTest statisticP
1. Verbal pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 3.90.048
Sensitivityt(31) = 0.2320.818
Biast(32) = 2.590.014*
2. Verbal uncertain pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 0.0480.827
Sensitivityt(52) = −0.3410.735
Biast(59) = −2.1790.033
3. Verbal uncertain pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 2.30.129
Sensitivityt(27) = −1.6490.111
Biast(35) = −0.3820.705
4. Pend. agree or disagree Pend. uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 4.0830.043
Sensitivityt(30) = −2.4370.021
Biast(44) = 1.3720.177
5. Pendulum agree pendulum disagreeAccuracyχ2(1) = 23.462<0.001*
Sensitivityt(18) = −0.5450.592
Biast(18) = −1.4920.153
HypothesisMeasureTest statisticP
1. Verbal pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 3.90.048
Sensitivityt(31) = 0.2320.818
Biast(32) = 2.590.014*
2. Verbal uncertain pendulumAccuracyχ2(1) = 0.0480.827
Sensitivityt(52) = −0.3410.735
Biast(59) = −2.1790.033
3. Verbal uncertain pendulum uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 2.30.129
Sensitivityt(27) = −1.6490.111
Biast(35) = −0.3820.705
4. Pend. agree or disagree Pend. uncertainAccuracyχ2(1) = 4.0830.043
Sensitivityt(30) = −2.4370.021
Biast(44) = 1.3720.177
5. Pendulum agree pendulum disagreeAccuracyχ2(1) = 23.462<0.001*
Sensitivityt(18) = −0.5450.592
Biast(18) = −1.4920.153

Notes: Bias (c) differed between verbal and pendulum conditions and accuracy differed in the pendulum condition based on agreement. Each test had a Bonferroni-corrected α of 0.02.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]