Abstract

Background

Cigarette smoking is a well-known public health concern, and there is an urgent need to develop new treatments to reduce smoking or facilitate abstinence. One factor that is known to contribute to relapse is stress, making the stress response an important target for treatment. The neuropeptide oxytocin (OT) is believed to have stress-reducing effects, and in addition there is evidence that it reduces drug craving. The purpose of the present study was to examine the effects of intranasal OT on stress-induced cigarette craving in regular smokers after 12 h of abstinence.

Method

Daily smokers (n = 48) completed a stress induction task and a nonstressful control task at two different sessions, receiving intranasal OT (40 IU) or placebo (PBO) before or after the task. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of three groups: Group PP (n = 16) received PBO before and after the stress/control tasks, Group OP (n = 16) received OT before the tasks and PBO after, and Group PO (n = 16) received PBO before the tasks and OT shortly after completing the tasks. Cigarette craving as well as subjective and physiological responses to stress was assessed.

Results

OT did not alter responses to stress, whether it was administered before or after the stressful task, on measures of cigarette craving, anxiety, heart rate, blood pressure, and cortisol levels.

Conclusions

The current study findings do not support several previous reports that OT reduced either stress or drug craving.

Implications

This study finds a null result of the neuropeptide oxytocin on stress-induced cigarette craving. Reporting null findings is part of the process of identifying potential treatments for addictive disorders.

Introduction

Cigarette smoking continues to be a major public health concern. Despite declines in smoking over the past decade, an estimated 29.7 million people in the United States still smoke cigarettes daily,1 and of those who try to quit, <1 in 10 succeed.2–4 Among the many possible reasons for the high rates of relapse, one recognized risk factor is stress. Stress increases craving and rates of smoking and increases the likelihood of returning to smoking during a quit attempt.5,6 Thus, a medication that reduces reactivity to acute or chronic stress would be of great value as an aid in quitting. Here, we explore the potential for the neuropeptide oxytocin (OT) as a stress-dampening aid to reduce cigarette craving.

OT has been proposed as a novel treatment for addiction.7–9 In animal models, OT reduces conditioned place preference established by methamphetamine10,11 and decreases self-administration of cocaine, opiate, and methamphetamine.12–14 In humans, Miller et al.15 reported that OT reduced craving for tobacco cigarettes induced by smoking-related cues in smokers who had abstained for 12 h. There is also evidence that the effects of OT on craving may be related to its stress-reducing properties: McRae-Clark et al.16 reported that intranasal OT reduced craving for cannabis in regular users who were exposed to the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). Indeed, in studies not related to drug taking, there are reports that OT reduces stress when administered before public speaking tasks.17,18

Several studies support the observation that acute stress increases cigarette craving and smoking.19 In a naturalistic survey study of 1500 smokers, McKee et al.20 found that relapse was related to stress resulting from a change of residence or adverse financial events. In laboratory studies, acute stress consistently increases craving for cigarettes among daily smokers.21–23 Buchmann and colleagues (2010) found that stress increases cigarette craving and that stress-induced craving was positively correlated with cortisol response to stress. Given that aspects of the stress response (eg, cortisol, anxiety ratings) can covary with cigarette craving, pharmacological agents that can reduce stress responses might help dampen stress-induced cigarette craving.

The purpose of the present study was to examine the effects of intranasal OT (40 IU) on cigarette craving induced by the TSST, a stressful public speaking task, under controlled laboratory conditions. This dose was selected based on prior studies that have found it to be effective at reducing stress-induced craving16 and smoking behavior.24 We examined the effects of OT administered before or shortly after the TSST. By administering OT before the task, we could determine whether OT dampened the experience of the stressor itself. By administering OT soon after the task, we could determine whether OT dampened the responses induced by the TSST (ie, recovery). We hypothesized that (1) the speech task would increase smoking craving, (2) OT administered before the speech task would reduce responses to the TSST and stress-induced craving, and (3) OT administered just after the TSST would dampen the stress responses (ie, hasten the return to normal after stress).

Methods

Study Design

This study used a mixed between- and within-subjects design to examine the effects of intranasal OT on stress-induced cigarette craving. Daily smokers participated in two sessions during which they completed a stress task and a nonstressful control task, each after abstaining from smoking for 12 h. During the 3-h sessions, subjects received nasal sprays containing OT (40 IU) or placebo (PBO) before and after completing the stress task or nonstressful control task. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of three groups who received (1) PBO before and just after the tasks at both sessions (Group PP), (2) OT before and PBO after the tasks (Group OP), or (3) PBO before and OT after the tasks (Group PO; Table 1). Within each group, participants received the same drug (OT or PBO) in the same order at both sessions. The verbal stress task consisted of the TSST25 and the nonstressful control task consisted of a simple conversation with a research assistant. The two tasks occurred in counterbalanced order across subjects. At regular intervals throughout the sessions, participants rated craving for cigarettes and mood, had cardiovascular measures recorded, and provided saliva samples. The primary outcome measure was craving for cigarettes. We were also interested in changes in mood, and cardiovascular and salivary cortisol response to the TSST.

Table 1.

Details of Study Design for the Three Subject Groups (order of TSST or control was counterbalanced across subjects)

Session 1Session 2
Spray 1TSST or controlSpray 2Spray 1TSST or controlSpray 2
Group PPPlaceboPlaceboPlaceboPlacebo
Group OPOxytocinPlaceboOxytocinPlacebo
Group POPlaceboOxytocinPlaceboOxytocin
Session 1Session 2
Spray 1TSST or controlSpray 2Spray 1TSST or controlSpray 2
Group PPPlaceboPlaceboPlaceboPlacebo
Group OPOxytocinPlaceboOxytocinPlacebo
Group POPlaceboOxytocinPlaceboOxytocin

TSST = Trier Social Stress Test.

Table 1.

Details of Study Design for the Three Subject Groups (order of TSST or control was counterbalanced across subjects)

Session 1Session 2
Spray 1TSST or controlSpray 2Spray 1TSST or controlSpray 2
Group PPPlaceboPlaceboPlaceboPlacebo
Group OPOxytocinPlaceboOxytocinPlacebo
Group POPlaceboOxytocinPlaceboOxytocin
Session 1Session 2
Spray 1TSST or controlSpray 2Spray 1TSST or controlSpray 2
Group PPPlaceboPlaceboPlaceboPlacebo
Group OPOxytocinPlaceboOxytocinPlacebo
Group POPlaceboOxytocinPlaceboOxytocin

TSST = Trier Social Stress Test.

Participants

Daily cigarette smokers were recruited from the University of Chicago and surrounding area. Participants were 18–35 years old and healthy apart from cigarette smoking. Screening included a physical exam and clinical interview, and a current and lifetime nonmedical drug use history. Participants were required to have a minimum of a high school education and fluency in English to participate. Potential participants with Major Axis I psychiatric disorders,26 serious medical conditions, regular medications, current or past year substance dependence (excluding nicotine dependence), and contraindications for intranasal drug administration (eg, prior nasal surgery) were excluded. In addition, women taking hormonal birth control and women who were pregnant, planning to become pregnant, or lactating were excluded. Women completed their study sessions during the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle to minimize variability in stress responses and fluctuations in endogenous OT related to hormone levels.27,28 Five additional participants did not complete both study sessions for reasons unrelated to the experimental procedure; only participants who completed both sessions (n = 48, 21 women) were included in the analyses. Participants provided written informed consent and received monetary compensation for completing the study. This study was approved by the University of Chicago Biological Sciences Division Institutional Review Board.

Procedure

Orientation

Participants attended a 30-min orientation session prior to the experimental sessions to discuss study logistics and obtain informed consent. They were told that the purpose of the study was to explore the effects intranasal OT, a naturally produced hormone, on responses to verbal tasks. Participants were made aware that the tasks would differ on the 2 days and that they might be video recorded. Participants were told to abstain from alcohol for 24 h before each session, recreational drugs for 48 h, marijuana for 7 days, and cigarettes or other nicotine containing products for 12 h. Normal caffeine intake was permitted. Participants were informed that they would be tested for recent drug use before each session. Participants also completed practice versions of cigarette craving and mood questionnaires and provided information on their smoking habits, including the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND).29

Experimental Sessions

Participants completed two 3-h experimental sessions beginning at noon, 3–7 days apart. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of the three groups described earlier (Group OP: OT before task and PBO after, Group PO: PBO before task and OT after, and Group PP: placebo in both sprays). On the two sessions, they received either OT (40 IU) or PBO nasal sprays under double-blind conditions before and shortly after a verbal task (TSST or control). Task order was counterbalanced.

Upon arriving at each session, recent drug use was assessed using a breathalyzer (Alco-sensorIII, Intoximeters, St Louis, MO), urine drug test (ToxCup, Branan Medical Corporation, Irvine, CA), and expired-air CO level (Bedfont Scientific piCO+ Smokerlyzer, coVita, Santa Barbara, CA). Following previous studies, 12 ppm of CO in expired-air was used as a cutoff for a positive test.30 Women were tested for pregnancy (AimStickPBD, hCG professional, Craig Medical Distribution, Vista, CA). Positive drug tests resulted in rescheduling that session. Next, participants completed baseline cigarette craving and mood questionnaires, and baseline cardiovascular measures; baseline saliva samples, to measure salivary cortisol, were obtained with unflavored cotton Salivettes® (Sarsdedt, Inc, Newton, NC). At 1 pm, participants received their first nasal spray containing OT (40 IU for Group OP) or PBO (for the other two groups), which took 10 min. Twenty minutes later, they completed cigarette craving and mood questionnaires, and cardiovascular measures, and provided a saliva sample. Then at 1:40 pm, they received instructions for the verbal task to be completed that day (either TSST or control; described in the following) and had 10 min to prepare for the task. Prior studies have demonstrated the effects of OT on public speaking tasks approximately 1 h after administration.17,18 The task itself occurred from 1:50 pm to 2:00 pm. At 2:01 pm subjects completed cigarette craving and mood questionnaires, as well as cardiovascular measures, and at 2:10 pm, saliva samples were collected. At 2:11 pm a second set of nasal sprays (OT 40 IU for Group PO and PBO for the other two groups) was administered, at 2:20 pm a saliva sample was collected, and at 2:50 pm participants completed cigarette craving and mood questionnaires, and cardiovascular measures, and provided final saliva samples. After the second session, participants were compensated for their participation and debriefed.

Drug

The Investigational Drug Pharmacy at the University of Chicago prepared the OT and PBO sprays. OT sprays consisted of 40 IU Pitocin (OT Injection USB; Monarch Pharmaceuticals; concentration 10 IU Pitocin/1 mL) transferred into four 1-mL syringes and administered with nasal atomizers (MAD300 by LMA, Inc, San Diego, CA). PBO sprays were Ocean Spray Nasal Spray (Valeant Pharmaceuticals, Bridgewater, NJ). Participants received 2 mL administered to each nostril >10 min at each set of sprays.

Trier Social Stress Test

The TSST began 50 min after the first nasal spray and followed the procedure outlined by Kirschbaum et al. The interval of 50 min was used based on Gossen et al.,31 showing peak plasma concentrations at 30–90 min following intranasal OT administration, and in accordance with the protocol used in Heinrichs et al.17 First, participants were told about the task for the day and given 10 min to prepare. They were told that they would be giving a 5-min speech describing their qualifications for their ideal job, and performing a 5-min mental arithmetic test to assess working memory capacity (counting down from 6233 by 13) in front of two “judges.” Participants were informed that their interview would be recorded. The two confederates were unknown to the subjects and were trained to maintain neutral facial expressions and minimize nonverbal behavior. The primary confederate was male and the same across all subjects. During the speech and mental arithmetic test, participants were prompted to continue or to start over when they paused or made an error. Participants were debriefed on the TSST after completing all study requirements.

Control Verbal Task

The control task began 50 min after the first nasal spray and involved speaking with a research assistant and playing a computer game. Before the task, participants were given 10 min to prepare for a discussion of a movie, program, play, or book that they recently viewed or read. They then spoke with a research assistant that they had not previously met about their chosen topic for 5 min. Following the conversation, they played Solitaire on the computer for 5 min. This task has been previously used by other studies in our lab.32–34

Dependent Measures

Cigarette Craving

Craving was measured with subscales of the Short Tobacco Craving Questionnaire (S-TCQ).35 The S-TCQ is a validated 12-item scale assessing subjective tobacco craving that includes subscales about anticipation of positive outcomes from smoking (Expectancy; eg, “I would enjoy a cigarette right now”) and relief from negative mood (Emotionality; eg, “I would be less irritable now if I could smoke”).

Mood

To assess mood during the experimental sessions, participants completed the Profile of Mood States (POMS).36 The POMS is a standardized 72-item questionnaire consisting of adjectives that describe current positive and negative mood states. Participants rate how much they feel each adjective at the moment on a scale ranging from “Not at All” to “Extremely.” We focused on the anxiety subscale because we would expect the TSST to increase feelings of anxiety, whereas the control task should not increase anxiety.

Salivary Cortisol

Saliva samples were collected during the session in which participants completed the TSST at baseline, 20 min after the first spray, and 10, 20, and 50 min after the TSST. Saliva samples were assayed for cortisol by the Core Laboratory at the University of Chicago Hospitals General Clinical Research Center (Salimetrics LLC, State College, PA; sensitivity = 0.003 μg/dL). Saliva samples from four participants could not be analyzed because of insufficient quantities of saliva.

Cardiovascular Measures

Heart rate (HR) and blood pressure were collected at six timepoints throughout the session using a standard blood pressure cuff (Omron Healthcare, Inc, Lake Forest, IL). From these values, mean arterial pressure (MAP) was calculated using the formula: [systolic BP + (2 × diastolic BP)]/3.

Statistical Analyses

Data were analyzed using SPSS statistical software program version 22 (SPSS, Inc, IBM, Chicago, IL). Power was calculated using G*Power 3.37 With the current sample size and study design, we were adequately powered (>80%) to detect effects in the small to moderate range. This study addressed two main questions: (1) Does OT reduce stress-induced cigarette craving when it is administered before the TSST, and (2) does OT reduces stress-induced cigarette craving when it is administered after the TSST (ie, during recovery)? For both questions, we used repeated measures ANOVA (group, task, time) to examine changes in cigarette craving after OT and stress. For the first question, we examined the change in cigarette craving from before to after the study tasks (TSST vs control task) comparing the OP versus PP groups only. For the second question, we examined the change in cigarette craving after the task (but before the second spray) to craving reported 20 min after the spray, comparing the PO and PP groups only.

These and other analyses provided information on secondary questions. First, did OT alone affect mood, physiology, or craving independently of the TSST? To answer this question, we examined mood, craving, cortisol, HR, and MAP before and after the first spray in the OP and PP groups using repeated measures ANOVA (group, time). Second, did OT alter psychological or physiological responses to the TSST on measures of mood, cortisol, HR, and MAP? To answer this question, we compared these measures before and after the TSST in the OP and PP groups, using repeated measures ANOVA (group, time).

Results

Demographic Information

Participant demographic information and smoking characteristics are presented in Table 2. Participants in the three drug groups did not differ significantly in age, education, body mass index, current drug use, or average daily cigarette use. The groups also did not differ in FTND scores, F(2, 45) = 1.00, p = .375.

Table 2.

Demographic Information, Current Drug Use and Smoking Characteristics for the Three Groups: PP (n =16) Placebo Before and After Stress Task, OP (n = 16) Oxytocin Before and Placebo After Stress Task, and PO (n = 16) Placebo Before and Oxytocin After Stress Task

Drug groupPPOPPO
Sex
 Male/female9/79/79/7
Age (years)26.6 (0.9)25.3 (1.1)26.4 (1.0)
Education (years)14.6 (0.4)13.9 (0.3)13.6 (0.3)
BMI26.8 (1.6)24.7 (1.1)25.3 (1.6)
Race
 Caucasian37.5 (6)56.3 (9)31.3 (5)
 African American43.8 (7)18.8 (3)31.5 (5)
 Asian6.3 (1)6.3 (1)
 Mixed race12.5 (2)25.0 (4)18.8 (3)
 Unknown12.5 (2)
Current drug usea
 Drinking occasions per week2.4 (0.6); n = 133.0 (0.3); n = 163.4 (0.5); n = 15
 Average number of drinks per occasion3.1 (0.4); n = 154.1 (0.5); n = 164.1 (0.5); n = 16
 Cannabis use in past month (days)15.9 (3.5); n = 1315.4 (3.4); n = 1112.5 (2.8); n = 14
Smoking characteristics (past month)
 Average daily cigarettes8.3 (0.9)8.0 (1.3)8.7 (1.1)
 Maximum daily cigarettes13.8 (1.7)13.5 (2.0)13.7 (1.5)
 Minimum daily cigarettes5.1 (0.9)5.2 (0.9)3.6 (0.8)
 Average weekly cigarettes59.2 (5.1)62.4 (9.0)54.5 (7.6)
 FTND score3.5 (0.5)2.6 (0.5)3.0 (0.5)
Drug groupPPOPPO
Sex
 Male/female9/79/79/7
Age (years)26.6 (0.9)25.3 (1.1)26.4 (1.0)
Education (years)14.6 (0.4)13.9 (0.3)13.6 (0.3)
BMI26.8 (1.6)24.7 (1.1)25.3 (1.6)
Race
 Caucasian37.5 (6)56.3 (9)31.3 (5)
 African American43.8 (7)18.8 (3)31.5 (5)
 Asian6.3 (1)6.3 (1)
 Mixed race12.5 (2)25.0 (4)18.8 (3)
 Unknown12.5 (2)
Current drug usea
 Drinking occasions per week2.4 (0.6); n = 133.0 (0.3); n = 163.4 (0.5); n = 15
 Average number of drinks per occasion3.1 (0.4); n = 154.1 (0.5); n = 164.1 (0.5); n = 16
 Cannabis use in past month (days)15.9 (3.5); n = 1315.4 (3.4); n = 1112.5 (2.8); n = 14
Smoking characteristics (past month)
 Average daily cigarettes8.3 (0.9)8.0 (1.3)8.7 (1.1)
 Maximum daily cigarettes13.8 (1.7)13.5 (2.0)13.7 (1.5)
 Minimum daily cigarettes5.1 (0.9)5.2 (0.9)3.6 (0.8)
 Average weekly cigarettes59.2 (5.1)62.4 (9.0)54.5 (7.6)
 FTND score3.5 (0.5)2.6 (0.5)3.0 (0.5)

Values represent percent (n) or mean (SEM).

BMI = body mass index; FTND = Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence.

aSample sizes for current drug use indicate the number of participants who reported any alcohol and cannabis use in the past month. Mean values are based on only those who reported using.

Table 2.

Demographic Information, Current Drug Use and Smoking Characteristics for the Three Groups: PP (n =16) Placebo Before and After Stress Task, OP (n = 16) Oxytocin Before and Placebo After Stress Task, and PO (n = 16) Placebo Before and Oxytocin After Stress Task

Drug groupPPOPPO
Sex
 Male/female9/79/79/7
Age (years)26.6 (0.9)25.3 (1.1)26.4 (1.0)
Education (years)14.6 (0.4)13.9 (0.3)13.6 (0.3)
BMI26.8 (1.6)24.7 (1.1)25.3 (1.6)
Race
 Caucasian37.5 (6)56.3 (9)31.3 (5)
 African American43.8 (7)18.8 (3)31.5 (5)
 Asian6.3 (1)6.3 (1)
 Mixed race12.5 (2)25.0 (4)18.8 (3)
 Unknown12.5 (2)
Current drug usea
 Drinking occasions per week2.4 (0.6); n = 133.0 (0.3); n = 163.4 (0.5); n = 15
 Average number of drinks per occasion3.1 (0.4); n = 154.1 (0.5); n = 164.1 (0.5); n = 16
 Cannabis use in past month (days)15.9 (3.5); n = 1315.4 (3.4); n = 1112.5 (2.8); n = 14
Smoking characteristics (past month)
 Average daily cigarettes8.3 (0.9)8.0 (1.3)8.7 (1.1)
 Maximum daily cigarettes13.8 (1.7)13.5 (2.0)13.7 (1.5)
 Minimum daily cigarettes5.1 (0.9)5.2 (0.9)3.6 (0.8)
 Average weekly cigarettes59.2 (5.1)62.4 (9.0)54.5 (7.6)
 FTND score3.5 (0.5)2.6 (0.5)3.0 (0.5)
Drug groupPPOPPO
Sex
 Male/female9/79/79/7
Age (years)26.6 (0.9)25.3 (1.1)26.4 (1.0)
Education (years)14.6 (0.4)13.9 (0.3)13.6 (0.3)
BMI26.8 (1.6)24.7 (1.1)25.3 (1.6)
Race
 Caucasian37.5 (6)56.3 (9)31.3 (5)
 African American43.8 (7)18.8 (3)31.5 (5)
 Asian6.3 (1)6.3 (1)
 Mixed race12.5 (2)25.0 (4)18.8 (3)
 Unknown12.5 (2)
Current drug usea
 Drinking occasions per week2.4 (0.6); n = 133.0 (0.3); n = 163.4 (0.5); n = 15
 Average number of drinks per occasion3.1 (0.4); n = 154.1 (0.5); n = 164.1 (0.5); n = 16
 Cannabis use in past month (days)15.9 (3.5); n = 1315.4 (3.4); n = 1112.5 (2.8); n = 14
Smoking characteristics (past month)
 Average daily cigarettes8.3 (0.9)8.0 (1.3)8.7 (1.1)
 Maximum daily cigarettes13.8 (1.7)13.5 (2.0)13.7 (1.5)
 Minimum daily cigarettes5.1 (0.9)5.2 (0.9)3.6 (0.8)
 Average weekly cigarettes59.2 (5.1)62.4 (9.0)54.5 (7.6)
 FTND score3.5 (0.5)2.6 (0.5)3.0 (0.5)

Values represent percent (n) or mean (SEM).

BMI = body mass index; FTND = Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence.

aSample sizes for current drug use indicate the number of participants who reported any alcohol and cannabis use in the past month. Mean values are based on only those who reported using.

Effects of OT Independent of Stress

Independently of the TSST, OT did not affect any subjective or physiological measure assessed 20 min after the first spray. That is, the OP group did not differ from the PP group before the TSST, and the PP and PO groups did not differ from each other comparing between or within groups before each task. Thus, compared with PBO, OT had no effect on self-reported ratings including anxiety or cigarette craving, or on measures of cardiovascular function.

Effectiveness of Stress Induction and Effect of Stress on Craving

The TSST produced its expected effects on mood, cardiovascular function, and cortisol. Compared with the control task, the TSST significantly increased POMS anxiety (task × time), F(4, 172) = 20.30, p < .001, and MAP (task × time), F(4, 188) = 4.24, p < .01, and within the TSST session, salivary cortisol increased from before to after the TSST, F(4, 172) = 7.66, p < .001. These effects subsided during the hour following the TSST. The TSST had its predicted effects on cigarette craving. Compared with the control task, the TSST increased scores on both the TCQ Emotionality scale (task × time), F(2, 54) = 12.2, p < .001 (Figure 1), and the TCQ Expectancy scale (task × time) F(2, 54) = 7.9, p < .01, in all three groups. Means and standard errors for cigarette craving by task and group are presented in Table 3.

Mean ± (SEM) ratings of cigarette craving (Short Tobacco Craving Questionnaire [S-TCQ] Emotionality) before and after the control task and Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). The arrows indicate the timing of nasal sprays, and the gray bar indicates when the control task or TSST occurred. The pre-task measurement was taken 20 min after the first spray, the post-task measurement was taken immediately after the task, and the third measurement occurred 40 min after the second spray. The PP group received placebo at both sprays, the OP group received oxytocin at the first spray and placebo at the second spray, and the PO group received placebo at the first spray and oxytocin at the second spray. The TSST significantly increased cigarette craving compared with the control task, and there were no group differences in ratings of craving. Oxytocin had no significant effect in either the OP or PO group.
Figure 1.

Mean ± (SEM) ratings of cigarette craving (Short Tobacco Craving Questionnaire [S-TCQ] Emotionality) before and after the control task and Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). The arrows indicate the timing of nasal sprays, and the gray bar indicates when the control task or TSST occurred. The pre-task measurement was taken 20 min after the first spray, the post-task measurement was taken immediately after the task, and the third measurement occurred 40 min after the second spray. The PP group received placebo at both sprays, the OP group received oxytocin at the first spray and placebo at the second spray, and the PO group received placebo at the first spray and oxytocin at the second spray. The TSST significantly increased cigarette craving compared with the control task, and there were no group differences in ratings of craving. Oxytocin had no significant effect in either the OP or PO group.

Table 3.

Means (SEM) of Subjective and Cardiovascular Responses by Group and Task

GroupControlTSST
Pre-controlPost-controlPost-spray #2Pre-TSSTPost-TSSTPost-spray #2
TCQ expectancy
 Group PP12.9 (1.2)12.5 (1.3)11.5 (1.4)13.3 (1.1)15.6 (1.0)**14.5 (1.2)
 Group OP14.5 (1.4)14.6 (1.5)15.2 (1.4)13.8 (1.8)16.5 (1.7)*15.7 (1.5)
 Group PO14.4 (1.5)14.9 (1.6)14.9 (1.6)14.9 (1.5)17.4 (1.2)*16.8 (1.2)
POMS anxiety
 Group PP6.4 (1.4)4.5 (0.8)5.8 (1.0)5.7 (1.0)11.4 (1.5)**5.4 (1.0)
 Group OP7.4 (1.3)6.6 (1.3)5.8 (1.1)4.9 (1.0)14.8 (2.2)**7.8 (1.1)
 Group PO5.9 (0.8)6.0 (1.1)5.5 (0.7)5.6 (1.2)9.6 (1.7)**5.6 (0.8)
Heart rate
 Group PP65.2 (2.5)62.9 (2.2)65.3 (2.0)64.8 (2.4)65.9 (2.4)64.4 (2.0)
 Group OP70.0 (3.0)68.7 (3.2)66.1 (3.1)71.9 (2.2)68.1 (3.1)70.4 (3.4)
 Group PO65.2 (1.9)65.1 (2.1)62.8 (2.0)69.9 (1.8)66.6 (3.4)65.4 (2.6)
Blood pressure (MAP)
 Group PP92.5 (3.3)92.4 (2.7)90.1 (2.0)87.9 (2.0)98.7 (2.1)**89.1 (1.7)
 Group OP84.4 (1.7)88.0 (2.0)*88.0 (2.3)83.6 (1.8)91.1 (2.6)**84.4 (2.0)
 Group PO94.4 (3.8)94.6 (3.5)92.7 (2.9)92.8 (2.8)97.8 (2.9)#93.9 (3.0)
GroupControlTSST
Pre-controlPost-controlPost-spray #2Pre-TSSTPost-TSSTPost-spray #2
TCQ expectancy
 Group PP12.9 (1.2)12.5 (1.3)11.5 (1.4)13.3 (1.1)15.6 (1.0)**14.5 (1.2)
 Group OP14.5 (1.4)14.6 (1.5)15.2 (1.4)13.8 (1.8)16.5 (1.7)*15.7 (1.5)
 Group PO14.4 (1.5)14.9 (1.6)14.9 (1.6)14.9 (1.5)17.4 (1.2)*16.8 (1.2)
POMS anxiety
 Group PP6.4 (1.4)4.5 (0.8)5.8 (1.0)5.7 (1.0)11.4 (1.5)**5.4 (1.0)
 Group OP7.4 (1.3)6.6 (1.3)5.8 (1.1)4.9 (1.0)14.8 (2.2)**7.8 (1.1)
 Group PO5.9 (0.8)6.0 (1.1)5.5 (0.7)5.6 (1.2)9.6 (1.7)**5.6 (0.8)
Heart rate
 Group PP65.2 (2.5)62.9 (2.2)65.3 (2.0)64.8 (2.4)65.9 (2.4)64.4 (2.0)
 Group OP70.0 (3.0)68.7 (3.2)66.1 (3.1)71.9 (2.2)68.1 (3.1)70.4 (3.4)
 Group PO65.2 (1.9)65.1 (2.1)62.8 (2.0)69.9 (1.8)66.6 (3.4)65.4 (2.6)
Blood pressure (MAP)
 Group PP92.5 (3.3)92.4 (2.7)90.1 (2.0)87.9 (2.0)98.7 (2.1)**89.1 (1.7)
 Group OP84.4 (1.7)88.0 (2.0)*88.0 (2.3)83.6 (1.8)91.1 (2.6)**84.4 (2.0)
 Group PO94.4 (3.8)94.6 (3.5)92.7 (2.9)92.8 (2.8)97.8 (2.9)#93.9 (3.0)

MAP = mean arterial pressure; POMS = Profile of Mood States; S-TCQ = Short Tobacco Craving Questionnaire; TSST = Trier Social Stress Test.

Significant increase as a result of the task. #p < .10, * p < .05, **p < .01.

Table 3.

Means (SEM) of Subjective and Cardiovascular Responses by Group and Task

GroupControlTSST
Pre-controlPost-controlPost-spray #2Pre-TSSTPost-TSSTPost-spray #2
TCQ expectancy
 Group PP12.9 (1.2)12.5 (1.3)11.5 (1.4)13.3 (1.1)15.6 (1.0)**14.5 (1.2)
 Group OP14.5 (1.4)14.6 (1.5)15.2 (1.4)13.8 (1.8)16.5 (1.7)*15.7 (1.5)
 Group PO14.4 (1.5)14.9 (1.6)14.9 (1.6)14.9 (1.5)17.4 (1.2)*16.8 (1.2)
POMS anxiety
 Group PP6.4 (1.4)4.5 (0.8)5.8 (1.0)5.7 (1.0)11.4 (1.5)**5.4 (1.0)
 Group OP7.4 (1.3)6.6 (1.3)5.8 (1.1)4.9 (1.0)14.8 (2.2)**7.8 (1.1)
 Group PO5.9 (0.8)6.0 (1.1)5.5 (0.7)5.6 (1.2)9.6 (1.7)**5.6 (0.8)
Heart rate
 Group PP65.2 (2.5)62.9 (2.2)65.3 (2.0)64.8 (2.4)65.9 (2.4)64.4 (2.0)
 Group OP70.0 (3.0)68.7 (3.2)66.1 (3.1)71.9 (2.2)68.1 (3.1)70.4 (3.4)
 Group PO65.2 (1.9)65.1 (2.1)62.8 (2.0)69.9 (1.8)66.6 (3.4)65.4 (2.6)
Blood pressure (MAP)
 Group PP92.5 (3.3)92.4 (2.7)90.1 (2.0)87.9 (2.0)98.7 (2.1)**89.1 (1.7)
 Group OP84.4 (1.7)88.0 (2.0)*88.0 (2.3)83.6 (1.8)91.1 (2.6)**84.4 (2.0)
 Group PO94.4 (3.8)94.6 (3.5)92.7 (2.9)92.8 (2.8)97.8 (2.9)#93.9 (3.0)
GroupControlTSST
Pre-controlPost-controlPost-spray #2Pre-TSSTPost-TSSTPost-spray #2
TCQ expectancy
 Group PP12.9 (1.2)12.5 (1.3)11.5 (1.4)13.3 (1.1)15.6 (1.0)**14.5 (1.2)
 Group OP14.5 (1.4)14.6 (1.5)15.2 (1.4)13.8 (1.8)16.5 (1.7)*15.7 (1.5)
 Group PO14.4 (1.5)14.9 (1.6)14.9 (1.6)14.9 (1.5)17.4 (1.2)*16.8 (1.2)
POMS anxiety
 Group PP6.4 (1.4)4.5 (0.8)5.8 (1.0)5.7 (1.0)11.4 (1.5)**5.4 (1.0)
 Group OP7.4 (1.3)6.6 (1.3)5.8 (1.1)4.9 (1.0)14.8 (2.2)**7.8 (1.1)
 Group PO5.9 (0.8)6.0 (1.1)5.5 (0.7)5.6 (1.2)9.6 (1.7)**5.6 (0.8)
Heart rate
 Group PP65.2 (2.5)62.9 (2.2)65.3 (2.0)64.8 (2.4)65.9 (2.4)64.4 (2.0)
 Group OP70.0 (3.0)68.7 (3.2)66.1 (3.1)71.9 (2.2)68.1 (3.1)70.4 (3.4)
 Group PO65.2 (1.9)65.1 (2.1)62.8 (2.0)69.9 (1.8)66.6 (3.4)65.4 (2.6)
Blood pressure (MAP)
 Group PP92.5 (3.3)92.4 (2.7)90.1 (2.0)87.9 (2.0)98.7 (2.1)**89.1 (1.7)
 Group OP84.4 (1.7)88.0 (2.0)*88.0 (2.3)83.6 (1.8)91.1 (2.6)**84.4 (2.0)
 Group PO94.4 (3.8)94.6 (3.5)92.7 (2.9)92.8 (2.8)97.8 (2.9)#93.9 (3.0)

MAP = mean arterial pressure; POMS = Profile of Mood States; S-TCQ = Short Tobacco Craving Questionnaire; TSST = Trier Social Stress Test.

Significant increase as a result of the task. #p < .10, * p < .05, **p < .01.

Effects of OT Administered Before the TSST

OT administered before the TSST did not decrease any of the responses to the stressor, including ratings of anxiety or cigarette craving, blood pressure, or cortisol levels. This was assessed by comparing the responses of the OP group to the PP group.

Effect of OT Administered After the TSST

OT administered shortly after the TSST had no significant effect on recovery from stress. This was determined by comparing responses of the PO and PP groups.

Discussion

In this study, we tested the effects of intranasal OT on physiological and subjective responses to the TSST and stress-induced cigarette craving in smokers. The TSST effectively increased blood pressure, cortisol, and subjective ratings of anxiety, and, as predicted, it increased cigarette craving. However, OT administration did not dampen any of the dimensions of stress measured, nor did it reduce stress-induced craving, whether it was administered before or after the TSST. If anything, participants who received OT before the TSST showed a trend toward further increased craving following the TSST relative to the other two groups (Figure 1), but this difference was not statistically significant. This null result was somewhat surprising, given the previous evidence that OT reduces responses to psychosocial stress. Heinrichs et al.17 reported that OT attenuated cortisol and anxiety responses to the TSST. Notably, that study showed that OT only significantly dampened cortisol responses to the TSST when combined with social support, which we did not include in our study. Another study found that OT reduced stress-induced dysphoria, but only in individuals with borderline personality disorder.38 In line with these findings, yet another group reported that OT reduced cortisol responses to stress in individuals with disordered emotion regulation.39 Thus, there is some evidence that the effects of OT are most pronounced in individuals with poor emotional regulation. In contrast, in the present study we excluded anyone with clinical symptoms of mood dysregulation, leaving open the possibility that the anxiolytic effects of OT are limited to individuals with negative affective symptoms.

The present findings can be considered in light of other studies on the effects of OT on either cue-induced or stress-induced craving for drugs. Although both drug cues and acute stress are known to heighten craving in humans and increase relapse-like behavior in animal models,40 they are thought to do so by separate mechanisms (eg, granular insular cortex activity for reinstatement by nicotine cues, and alpha-2-adreno-ceptors in central amygdala for reinstatement by acute stress). In the present study, we found that OT did not affect stress-induced cigarette craving, whereas in a previous study15 we reported that OT reduced cigarette craving induced by smoking-related cues. In addition, McRae-Clark et al.16 found that OT reduced stress-induced craving for cannabis among cannabis-dependent users. The explanation for these apparently discrepant findings is unclear. The three studies used comparable sample sizes. The precise conditions under which OT alters craving in response to drug-related cues, or stress, remain to be determined.

This study has several important limitations. First, our participants reported low to moderate levels of nicotine dependence despite being daily smokers. It is possible that OT would have more pronounced effects in more dependent smokers. Heavier smokers might report greater stress-induced craving, thus revealing an effect of OT. Second, we tested only one dose (40 IU) of OT. It is possible that higher, or lower, doses of OT may have differential effects. Third, our measure of OT effects before the stressor may have occurred too soon after the first spray to draw strong conclusions of the effects of OT independent of the TSST. These measurements were taken 20 min after the spray, and there is some evidence that plasma levels of OT peak 30–90 min after intranasal administration.31 It is not yet known whether the central effects occur at the same time as the increase in plasma levels. Fourth, we did not assess other individual difference factors that might moderate the effects of OT (eg, attachment style, experiences of childhood trauma). Finally, our relatively small number of subjects did not permit an analysis of the influence of sex. Future investigations may include a more direct comparison of the effects of OT on cue- and stress-induced craving, a larger sample size capable of examining sex differences, and a measure of plasma OT levels during the procedure.

Taken together, we found no evidence that intranasal OT reduces stress-induced craving in abstinent cigarette smokers with low to moderate nicotine dependence. Although we found that the TSST did increase cigarette craving, OT did not affect this or any other indices of stress (subjective, hormonal, cardiovascular). It remains to be determined whether OT affects stress-induced craving under other conditions, including those where OT alone has altered stress responses.

Funding

This work was supported by a Global Research Award for Nicotine Dependence (GRAND) from Pfizer, and by grants from NIDA (DA02812). KVH is currently supported by NIMH training grant (T32MH020065).

Declaration of Interests

HdW has received support unrelated to this study from the following sources: consulting fees from Marinius and Jazz Pharmaceuticals, gift of a study drug from Indivior, and support for a research study from Insys Therapeutics.

References

1.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration
.
Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2016 National Survey on Drug Use and Health
.
Cent Behav Health Stat Qual Subst Abuse Ment Health Serv Adm
.
2017
;
7
(
1
):
877
726
.

2.

Babb
S
,
Malarcher
A
,
Schauer
G
,
Asman
K
,
Jamal
A
.
Quitting smoking among adults—United States, 2000–2015
.
MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep
.
2017
;
65
(
52
):
1457
1464
.

3.

Fiore
MC
,
Jaén
CR
,
Baker
TB
, et al.
A clinical practice guideline for treating tobacco use and dependence: 2008 update: A U.S. public health service report
.
Am J Prev Med
.
2008
;
35
(
2
):
158
176
. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2008.04.009

4.

Piasecki
TM
.
Relapse to smoking
.
Clin Psychol Rev
.
2006
;
26
(
2
):
196
215
.

5.

Shiffman
S
,
Gwaltney
CJ
,
Balabanis
MH
, et al.
Immediate antecedents of cigarette smoking: an analysis from ecological momentary assessment
.
J Abnorm Psychol
.
2002
;
111
(
4
):
531
545
.

6.

Sinha
R
,
Li
CS
.
Imaging stress- and cue-induced drug and alcohol craving: association with relapse and clinical implications
.
Drug Alcohol Rev
.
2007
;
26
(
1
):
25
31
.

7.

McGregor
IS
,
Bowen
MT
.
Breaking the loop: oxytocin as a potential treatment for drug addiction
.
Horm Behav
.
2012
;
61
(
3
):
331
339
.

8.

Sarnyai
Z
.
Oxytocin as a potential mediator and modulator of drug addiction
.
Addict Biol
.
2011
;
16
(
2
):
199
201
.

9.

Lee
MR
,
Weerts
EM
.
Oxytocin for the treatment of drug and alcohol use disorders
.
Behav Pharmacol
.
2016
;
27
(
8
):
640
648
.

10.

Qi
J
,
Yang
JY
,
Wang
F
,
Zhao
YN
,
Song
M
,
Wu
CF
.
Effects of oxytocin on methamphetamine-induced conditioned place preference and the possible role of glutamatergic neurotransmission in the medial prefrontal cortex of mice in reinstatement
.
Neuropharmacology
.
2009
;
56
(
5
):
856
865
.

11.

Baracz
SJ
,
Rourke
PI
,
Pardey
MC
,
Hunt
GE
,
McGregor
IS
,
Cornish
JL
.
Oxytocin directly administered into the nucleus accumbens core or subthalamic nucleus attenuates methamphetamine-induced conditioned place preference
.
Behav Brain Res
.
2012
;
228
(
1
):
185
193
.

12.

Sarnyai
Z
,
Kovács
GL
.
Role of oxytocin in the neuroadaptation to drugs of abuse
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
1994
;
19
(
1
):
85
117
.

13.

Ibragimov
R
,
Kovács
GL
,
Szabó
G
,
Telegdy
G
.
Microinjection of oxytocin into limbic-mesolimbic brain structures disrupts heroin self-administration behavior: a receptor-mediated event
?
Life Sci
.
1987
;
41
(
10
):
1265
1271
.

14.

Carson
DS
,
Cornish
JL
,
Guastella
AJ
,
Hunt
GE
,
McGregor
IS
.
Oxytocin decreases methamphetamine self-administration, methamphetamine hyperactivity, and relapse to methamphetamine-seeking behaviour in rats
.
Neuropharmacology
.
2010
;
58
(
1
):
38
43
.

15.

Miller
MA
,
Bershad
A
,
King
AC
,
Lee
R
,
de Wit
H
.
Intranasal oxytocin dampens cue-elicited cigarette craving in daily smokers: a pilot study
.
Behav Pharmacol
.
2016
;
27
(
8
):
697
703
.

16.

McRae-Clark
AL
,
Baker
NL
,
Maria
MM
,
Brady
KT
.
Effect of oxytocin on craving and stress response in marijuana-dependent individuals: a pilot study
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
.
2013
;
228
(
4
):
623
631
.

17.

Heinrichs
M
,
Baumgartner
T
,
Kirschbaum
C
,
Ehlert
U
.
Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2003
;
54
(
12
):
1389
1398
.

18.

de Oliveira
DC
,
Zuardi
AW
,
Graeff
FG
,
Queiroz
RH
,
Crippa
JA
.
Anxiolytic-like effect of oxytocin in the simulated public speaking test
.
J Psychopharmacol
.
2012
;
26
(
4
):
497
504
.

19.

Tsuda
A
,
Steptoe
A
,
West
R
,
Fieldman
G
,
Kirschbaum
C
.
Cigarette smoking and psychophysiological stress responsiveness: effects of recent smoking and temporary abstinence
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
.
1996
;
126
(
3
):
226
233
.

20.

McKee
SA
,
Maciejewski
PK
,
Falba
T
,
Mazure
CM
.
Sex differences in the effects of stressful life events on changes in smoking status
.
Addiction
.
2003
;
98
(
6
):
847
855
.

21.

Buchmann
AF
,
Laucht
M
,
Schmid
B
,
Wiedemann
K
,
Mann
K
,
Zimmermann
US
.
Cigarette craving increases after a psychosocial stress test and is related to cortisol stress response but not to dependence scores in daily smokers
.
J Psychopharmacol
.
2010
;
24
(
2
):
247
255
.

22.

Childs
E
,
de Wit
H
.
Effects of acute psychosocial stress on cigarette craving and smoking
.
Nicotine Tob Res
.
2010
;
12
(
4
):
449
453
.

23.

McKee
SA
,
Sinha
R
,
Weinberger
AH
, et al.
Stress decreases the ability to resist smoking and potentiates smoking intensity and reward
.
J Psychopharmacol
.
2011
;
25
(
4
):
490
502
.

24.

Van Hedger
K
,
Kushner
MJ
,
Lee
R
,
de Wit
H
.
Oxytocin reduces cigarette consumption in daily smokers
.
Nicotine Tob Res
.
2018
; doi:10.1093/ntr/nty080

25.

Kirschbaum
C
,
Pirke
KM
,
Hellhammer
DH
.
The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’—a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting
.
Neuropsychobiology
.
1993
;
28
(
1-2
):
76
81
.

26.

APA
.
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
. 5th ed.
Washington, DC
:
American Psychiatric Publishing
;
2013
.

27.

Kirschbaum
C
,
Kudielka
BM
,
Gaab
J
,
Schommer
NC
,
Hellhammer
DH
.
Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis
.
Psychosom Med
.
1999
;
61
(
2
):
154
162
.

28.

Salonia
A
,
Nappi
RE
,
Pontillo
M
, et al.
Menstrual cycle-related changes in plasma oxytocin are relevant to normal sexual function in healthy women
.
Horm Behav
.
2005
;
47
(
2
):
164
169
.

29.

Heatherton
TF
,
Kozlowski
LT
,
Frecker
RC
,
Fagerström
KO
.
The fagerström test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the fagerström tolerance questionnaire
.
Br J Addict
.
1991
;
86
(
9
):
1119
1127
.

30.

Sandberg
A
,
Sköld
CM
,
Grunewald
J
,
Eklund
A
,
Wheelock
ÅM
.
Assessing recent smoking status by measuring exhaled carbon monoxide levels
.
PLoS One
.
2011
;
6
(
12
):
e28864
.

31.

Gossen
A
,
Hahn
A
,
Westphal
L
, et al.
Oxytocin plasma concentrations after single intranasal oxytocin administration—a study in healthy men
.
Neuropeptides
.
2012
;
46
(
5
):
211
215
.

32.

Childs
E
,
Bershad
AK
,
de Wit
H
.
Effects of d-amphetamine upon psychosocial stress responses
.
J Psychopharmacol
.
2016
;
30
(
7
):
608
615
.

33.

Bershad
AK
,
Miller
MA
,
de Wit
H
.
MDMA does not alter responses to the trier social stress test in humans
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
.
2017
;
234
(
14
):
2159
2166
.

34.

Bershad
AK
,
Miller
MA
,
Norman
GJ
,
de Wit
H
.
Effects of opioid- and non-opioid analgesics on responses to psychosocial stress in humans
.
Horm Behav
.
2018
;
102
:
41
47
.

35.

Heishman
SJ
,
Singleton
EG
,
Pickworth
WB
.
Reliability and validity of a Short Form of the Tobacco Craving Questionnaire
.
Nicotine Tob Res
.
2008
;
10
(
4
):
643
651
.

36.

Johanson
CE
,
Uhlenhuth
EH
.
Drug preference and mood in humans: d-amphetamine
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
.
1980
;
71
(
3
):
275
279
.

37.

Faul
F
,
Erdfelder
E
,
Lang
AG
,
Buchner
A
.
G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences
.
Behav Res Methods
.
2007
;
39
(
2
):
175
191
.

38.

Simeon
D
,
Bartz
J
,
Hamilton
H
, et al.
Oxytocin administration attenuates stress reactivity in borderline personality disorder: a pilot study
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
2011
;
36
(
9
):
1418
1421
.

39.

Quirin
M
,
Kuhl
J
,
Düsing
R
.
Oxytocin buffers cortisol responses to stress in individuals with impaired emotion regulation abilities
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
2011
;
36
(
6
):
898
904
.

40.

Shaham
Y
,
Shalev
U
,
Lu
L
,
de Wit
H
,
Stewart
J
.
The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
.
2003
;
168
(
1-2
):
3
20
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)

Comments

0 Comments
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.