Abstract

What is the relationship between the electoral success of populist radical right parties (PRRPs) and public attitudes toward immigration? Previous research suggests that PRRP success can lead to more negative attitudes due to the breaking down of antiprejudice norms and more prominent anti-immigration party cues. However, we argue that greater PRRP success could have a positive relationship with immigration attitudes, reflecting negative partisanship, polarization, and a desire to reemphasize antiprejudice norms, which we call a “reverse backlash effect.” Using the best available electoral and public opinion data across the last thirty years in twenty-four European countries, our TSCS analyses show the predominance of such “reverse backlash effects” across several operationalizations of PRRP success. Our argument has important consequences for the understanding of possible PRRP effects on public opinion, as well as attitudinal formation via party cueing and social norms more generally.

Introduction

In recent decades, populist radical right parties (PRRPs) have secured increasing electoral strength across Europe and beyond. While there has been significant research on how such parties cue widespread anti-immigration sentiments to improve their electoral fortunes, it is still unclear whether they actually change these very sentiments. The so-called “immigration backlash” is usually conceptualized as a populist response to proimmigration policy advancements or increasing immigration (Kustov 2023), but one can also imagine an analogous “reverse backlash” to populist electoral success in terms of improving public attitudes toward immigration, especially given the increasing polarization on the issue. The recent prominent examples of such a dynamic are the seeming proimmigration reactions of the US public to the election of Donald Trump (Collingwood et al. 2018) or the UK public in the aftermath of Brexit (Schwartz et al. 2021). In this paper, we test whether these notable cases of “reverse backlash” can be generalized to a larger set of the populist radical right successes across countries.

What is the relationship between the electoral success of PRRPs and public attitudes toward immigration? Intuitively, we might expect the increased success of populist radical right parties to have a negative effect on public attitudes to immigration via two related mechanisms: the increased ability of PRRPs to cue anti-immigration attitudes in the general population via greater resources, attention, and legitimacy (Zaller 1992; Hellwig and Kweon 2016; Harteveld, Kokkonen, and Dahlberg 2017; Vrânceanu and Lachat 2021) and the breaking of social stigmas related to open expression of anti-immigration sentiment (Rooduijn et al. 2016). Moreover, Abou-Chadi and Krause (2020; Van Spanje 2010) show that when radical right parties enter parliament, it moves mainstream parties toward more anti-immigration positions (meaning that at the aggregate level the growth of PRRPs may also lead to anti-immigration sentiment via other parties’ cueing efforts). At the same time, Valentim (2021) shows that PRRP entry to parliament erodes social norms so that their voters see their views as legitimized and are more willing to publicly express their intention to vote for such parties.

However, empirically, despite the rise of PRRPs over the last decades, attitudes to immigration have been shown to be stable over the long term (Kustov, Laaker, and Reller 2021). Moreover, as the electoral breakthroughs and successes of PRRPs became more widespread and acute across Europe in the aftermath of the 2015–2016 so-called “migration crisis,” there was not and has still not been a correspondent negative turn in attitudes to immigration. Indeed, according to some sources of data and as measured by certain questions, attitudes to immigration have become more positive in several European countries (Dennison and Geddes 2019; Banai, Votta, and Seitz 2022).

In this research note, we argue and empirically demonstrate that PRRP electoral success is associated with greater positivity in overall public attitudes via what we dub a “reverse backlash effect.” If present, this relationship might reflect the desire among the majority of citizens to reemphasize antixenophobic social norms and stigma toward PRRPs’ key policies (e.g., Mendes and Dennison 2021), as well as express negative partisanship toward such parties (e.g., Meléndez and Kaltwasser 2021). It may also reflect a reduction in ambivalence toward the topic of immigration via higher issue salience and polarization more generally, with a majority of those who were ambivalent moving toward proimmigration positions (Lindstam, Mader, and Schoen 2021). Finally, this “reverse backlash effect” may also be conceptualized as a form of thermostatic public opinion. Rather than the thermostat measuring feeling toward particular policies (Wlezien 1995), it would be toward the direction and state of the party system and societal norms regarding what constitutes an acceptable political opinion.

Findings related to similar albeit less generalizable events lend plausibility to our central proposition. Scholars (Schwartz et al. 2021; Solodoch 2021) have shown that the UK’s vote to leave the EU (“Brexit”) led to an increase in self-reported positivity toward immigrants, theorized as resulting from satisfaction that immigration “control” had been “restored” and a desire to distance oneself from accusations of xenophobia and racism following the controversial referendum campaign. The latter point is grounded in the Motivation to Control Prejudice theory, which posits that “individuals are averse to breaking antiprejudice norms and will deliberately seek to control actions, expressions or thoughts that can be deemed to violate these norms” (Fazio et al. 1986; Devine 1989; Devine et al. 1991; Czopp and Monteith 2003; Dovidio and Gaertner 2004; Blinder, Ford, and Ivarsflaten 2013; Schwartz et al. 2021, p. 1169). Crucially, the Motivation to Control Prejudice theoretically requires continuous self-reflection about one’s own views to external events to control and check manifestations of prejudice to avoid social sanction (Monteith et al. 2002; Blinder, Ford, and Ivarsflaten 2013). As such, the widely discussed and controversial rise in radical right support in Europe in the 2010s may plausibly have—like “Brexit”—triggered a psychological imperative for many individuals to redouble their support for immigration. Indeed, Blinder, Ford, and Ivarsflaten (2013, p. 845) argue that politicians or parties with “clear racist or fascist reputations” can be expected to “activate the antiprejudice norm” while the controversial leaders of PRRPs make individuals “aware that a norm is at stake, before they take the cognitive effort to control prejudice and adjust their response in accordance with it” (Harteveld, Kokkonen, and Dahlberg 2017, p. 372).

Finally, we may also expect the increased success of PRRPs to have no effect on public attitudes to immigration. Such attitudes have been shown to be stable at the aggregate level (Kustov, Laaker, and Reller 2021), theoretically the result of attitudes being formed in early life and reflecting deep-seated psychological predispositions and national and education socializing experiences (Kauff et al. 2013; Dinesen, Klemmensen, and Nørgaard 2016; García-Faroldi 2017; Dennison, Davidov, and Seddig 2020; McLaren and Paterson 2020; McLaren, Neundorf, and Paterson 2021). Therefore, attitudinal changes that do occur happen gradually at the aggregate level due to generational replacement, as those socialized in more precipitous conditions for immigration sympathy (e.g., heterogenous societies, “universalist” tertiary education) enter adulthood.

We test these three possible alternatives regarding the possible relationship of PRRP success with anti-immigration attitudes (positive, negative, or null) using the standard fixed-effects TSCS specifications and the best available electoral and public opinion data across the last several decades in Europe. To capture PRRP success, we look at the vote and the seat shares won by the populist right as defined by the Popu-List or the Timbro Index data. To capture immigration attitudes, we aggregate the relevant public opinion items from major sources.

Data and Methods

Since the electoral success of PRRPs reflects the preferences and priorities of voters alongside other unobserved factors, any cross-sectional associations (or the lack thereof) are likely subject to endogeneity concerns, including reverse causality and omitted variable bias. The standard econometric solution to these problems is to produce plausible causal estimates by utilizing panel data with unit fixed-effects regression models, which can account for unobserved country-specific time-invariant confounding factors—such as historic, “constant” PRRP success and other fixed structural, historic, and long-term factors—under a few reasonable assumptions.1 The main identifying assumption of such an approach here is that immigration attitudes would have developed similarly in countries with and without the observed PRRP success had it not happened. As such, our approach cannot solve all potential sources of endogeneity, such as omitted variables that account for both PRRP success and immigration attitudes or reverse causality—though theoretically we believe it is unlikely that PRRPs can capitalize on proimmigration attitudes.

To test for the potential reverse backlash, we have thus gathered a TSCS dataset linking the best available electoral and public opinion data at the country-year level across twenty-four European countries (1989–2017).2 As for our main dependent variable, we construct an aggregate country-year measure of public anti-immigration attitudes comprised from the two most comprehensive data sources on the topic. We primarily rely on the “immigration conservatism” index compiled by Caughey, O’Grady, and Warshaw (2019), which aggregates major cross-national public opinion survey questions on immigration using an item response theory model. We then standardize and average it with the similar “immigration mood” index created by Claassen and McLaren (2021), which uses a slightly different aggregation methodology and a range of country-year cases (r = 0.81). Our results are robust to the use of these indices separately (see Supplementary Material tables S2 and S3).

As for our independent variables, we primarily rely on the Popu-List project combined with ParlGov data, which provides a comprehensive measure of the share of votes and seats won by the “far-right populist” parties in European national legislatures in each country-year since the last elections. These are exactly the parties that have been either explicitly anti-immigration or otherwise attractive to the anti-immigration electorate (Rooduijn et al. 2019). As a robustness check, we also look at the conceptually similar vote share of “right-wing populist” parties as coded by the Timbro Authoritarian Populism Index (Heinö 2016; r = 0.92). Some specifications also include common (lagged) control variables, including the share of immigrant population, unemployment rate (log), and GDP per capita (log). All major dependent and independent variables have been standardized to vary from 0 to 1. All coefficient estimates are reported using heteroskedasticity-robust standard errors.3 For more details on variables and their construction, see the Supplementary Material.

Results and Analysis

First, we consider the general relationship between the PRRP success and anti-immigration attitudes across countries and time. In line with the conventional wisdom, the naïve cross-sectional correlation between these variables in the pooled data is consistently positive and varies from 0.06 to 0.25 depending on the operationalization (see Supplementary Material figure S1).

We then plot the historical trajectories of PRRP success and anti-immigration attitudes within each European country (see figure 1). As can be seen, despite their positive cross-sectional correlation and their conflation in the popular discourse, these dependent and independent variables rarely go together across time regardless of their operationalization. If anything, there seems to be a negative correlation between these two phenomena within most of the countries.

The trajectories of PRRP success and anti-immigration attitudes across Europe (1989–2017). Higher values on the y-axis correspond to higher PRRP success or anti-immigration attitudes. Dashed lines indicate various measures of PRRP success. Solid lines indicate various measures of anti-immigration attitudes. For variable descriptions, see the Supplementary Material.
Figure 1.

The trajectories of PRRP success and anti-immigration attitudes across Europe (1989–2017). Higher values on the y-axis correspond to higher PRRP success or anti-immigration attitudes. Dashed lines indicate various measures of PRRP success. Solid lines indicate various measures of anti-immigration attitudes. For variable descriptions, see the Supplementary Material.

We thus further take advantage of the time-series cross-sectional data to fit a set of fixed-effects linear regressions to answer the following question: “As PRR success changes within a certain country, how does it relate to the changes in aggregate immigration attitudes over time?”

Table 1 summarizes our main empirical results. As can be seen, the success of PRRPs has a consistent negative relationship with anti-immigration attitudes. This is true regardless of PRRP success operationalization or a particular model specification. In terms of substantive size of this relationship, a one standard deviation increase in the seat and vote share of PRRPs is associated with a 0.16 and 0.2 standard deviation increase of proimmigration sentiments within countries, respectively. Overall, this evidence is consistent with the hypothesized reverse backlash effects of PRRP success, at least in the aggregate.

Table 1.

Populist right electoral success and anti-immigration attitudes. The table shows the relationship between PRRP success and anti-immigration attitudes. For variable descriptions, see the Supplementary Material. Heteroskedasticity-robust standard errors are given in parentheses.

Anti-immigration attitudes
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)
Far-right populist (seat share)−0.226−0.158−0.135
(0.043)(0.043)(0.046)
Far-right populist (vote share)−0.301−0.233−0.208
(0.048)(0.047)(0.050)
Right-wing populist (vote share)−0.266−0.167−0.136
(0.042)(0.041)(0.042)

Country FEYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
ControlsNoYesYesNoYesYesNoYesYes
Year FENoNoYesNoNoYesNoNoYes
Observations681660660681660660666660660
Adjusted R20.7940.8240.8210.7980.8260.8230.7930.8240.821
Anti-immigration attitudes
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)
Far-right populist (seat share)−0.226−0.158−0.135
(0.043)(0.043)(0.046)
Far-right populist (vote share)−0.301−0.233−0.208
(0.048)(0.047)(0.050)
Right-wing populist (vote share)−0.266−0.167−0.136
(0.042)(0.041)(0.042)

Country FEYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
ControlsNoYesYesNoYesYesNoYesYes
Year FENoNoYesNoNoYesNoNoYes
Observations681660660681660660666660660
Adjusted R20.7940.8240.8210.7980.8260.8230.7930.8240.821
Table 1.

Populist right electoral success and anti-immigration attitudes. The table shows the relationship between PRRP success and anti-immigration attitudes. For variable descriptions, see the Supplementary Material. Heteroskedasticity-robust standard errors are given in parentheses.

Anti-immigration attitudes
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)
Far-right populist (seat share)−0.226−0.158−0.135
(0.043)(0.043)(0.046)
Far-right populist (vote share)−0.301−0.233−0.208
(0.048)(0.047)(0.050)
Right-wing populist (vote share)−0.266−0.167−0.136
(0.042)(0.041)(0.042)

Country FEYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
ControlsNoYesYesNoYesYesNoYesYes
Year FENoNoYesNoNoYesNoNoYes
Observations681660660681660660666660660
Adjusted R20.7940.8240.8210.7980.8260.8230.7930.8240.821
Anti-immigration attitudes
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)
Far-right populist (seat share)−0.226−0.158−0.135
(0.043)(0.043)(0.046)
Far-right populist (vote share)−0.301−0.233−0.208
(0.048)(0.047)(0.050)
Right-wing populist (vote share)−0.266−0.167−0.136
(0.042)(0.041)(0.042)

Country FEYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
ControlsNoYesYesNoYesYesNoYesYes
Year FENoNoYesNoNoYesNoNoYes
Observations681660660681660660666660660
Adjusted R20.7940.8240.8210.7980.8260.8230.7930.8240.821

To further check the robustness of these results, we conduct several additional empirical tests with little change in the substantive finding. First, we replicate the specifications from table 1/Supplementary Material table S2 using the “economic conservatism index” from Caughey, O’Grady, and Warshaw (2019) as a placebo test, which expectedly shows no relationship with the PRRP success (see Supplementary Material table S4). Second, given that populist voting is not observed every year, we replicate our specifications at the more empirically appropriate, but less statistically powered, country-election level (see Supplementary Material table S5).

Discussion

How does the success of populist parties relate to voters’ immigration views? Given that right-wing populists use anti-immigration rhetoric in their political campaigns, it is tempting to think that, if anything, they can only make the electorate more anti-immigration when they emerge, gain support, and come to power. After all, in theory the rise of PRRPs should give them greater capacity to cue supporters and the general public (Zaller 1992; Hellwig and Kweon 2016; Harteveld, Kokkonen, and Dahlberg 2017; Vrânceanu and Lachat 2021) as well as an already observed (Rooduijn, Van der Brug, and De Lange 2016; Valentim 2021) reduction in stigma toward self-reporting support for such parties, which could theoretically also affect self-reporting antipathy or opposition toward immigration.

However, populism is polarizing (Bischof and Wagner 2019; Dai and Kustov 2022). While radical right views and parties carry significant social stigma and evoke negative partisanship (Meléndez and Kaltwasser 2021; Mendes and Dennison 2021), it is also possible that there may be a reverse backlash among voters who want to distance themselves personally from such parties and views. Indeed, voters may wish to halt the progress of such parties, their views, and their increased acceptability, or—particularly among moderates—see the rise of such parties as a signal that enough has been done to voice concerns about (perhaps selected aspects of) immigration policies or realities. Furthermore, the rise of such parties may lead to greater cognition about the issue of immigration and thus a reduction in attitudinal ambivalence (Lindstam, Mader, and Schoen 2021), with a majority of those who were ambivalent leaning toward proimmigration positions, particularly relative to the now higher-profile PRRPs.

In this paper, we tried to use the best available data to examine which possibility is more likely. While it is possible that both effects can be happening at the same time, the evidence is more consistent with the “reverse backlash effect.” That is, when PRRPs become increasingly successful, a significant proportion of the electorate “thermostatically” reacts to these developments by adjusting their stated preferences toward immigration in a positive direction. Given the general stability of immigration attitudes, however, it is important not to overstate the magnitude of these possible effects. Still, the available evidence strongly suggests that the populist success does not make the electorate more anti-immigration in the aggregate.

Of course, our study is not without limitations. In terms of endogeneity, one can still reasonably worry about reverse causality and omitted variable bias. As such, our findings should be regarded as associational rather than causal. Although we cannot rule out other confounding factors, theoretically there are few reasons to believe that the relationship reflects more positive attitudes to immigration causing greater PRRP success. In fact, the extent to which PRRPs rely on negative immigration attitudes when they come to power would only bias the regression estimates against finding any contemporaneous “reverse backlash effects,” contrary to what we find. Furthermore, the PRRP success may have an impact only on some voters, and some of these impacts may even cancel each other out in the aggregate (e.g., when PRRP success increases or decreases anti-immigration attitudes among PRRP and non-PRRP voters). Notably, if the greater positivity toward immigration were equal among PRRP and non-PRRP voters, it would undermine the stigma mechanism, and instead support the thermostatic interpretation. Future research may thus benefit from using individual-level longitudinal data to explore the potential heterogeneous effects on different types of voters and the related mechanisms in more detail, as well as exploring the more credible identification strategies to isolate the causal effects of PRRP success. Finally, our current dataset ends in 2017, after which the rise of PRRPs has, in many countries, stopped or even gone into reverse while public attitudes to immigration have tended to gradually become more positive (Banai, Votta, and Seitz 2022). This suggests that, if tested again in the future with the use of more recent data, the stipulated relationship may be stronger or, at the very least, is unlikely to disappear.

These limitations notwithstanding, it is important to know that the relationship between PRRP success and anti-immigration attitudes is in fact negative across the broadest sample of electoral contexts. This descriptive finding has several implications. First, social stigmas may mediate the effects of PRRP cueing on attitudes, not only precluding immigration backlash but also potentially reversing it. Second, just because the key determinants of attitudes to immigration—notably early life socialization and psychological predispositions—are “deep-seated” and have a constant component, that does not mean that they are not dynamic and subject to mediating exogenous factors. Third, the policy preferences of political supply (i.e., parties) have repeatedly been shown to be primarily affected by the policy preferences of political demand (i.e., voters), rather than vice versa. We offer a case whereby political demand seems to be related to political supply, albeit not in the manner that the political supply might prefer. Finally, although the electoral breakthroughs of PRRPs were for many countries a radical change from postwar or posttransition party systems, which was distressing or even traumatic for many individuals, it does not seem that social norms and stigmas around xenophobic attitudes and radical right parties were entirely “broken” by these events. Instead, it seems that they survived and found new forms of expression in most citizens who reject such norms.

Footnotes

1

The common two-way fixed-effects models have been recently criticized in the literature for their unclear counterfactual interpretation (Kropko and Kubinec 2020; Imai and Kim 2021). We thus use the simpler unit FEs as our starting specification but also report two-way FEs as a robustness check.

2

Information on data sources and corresponding response rates is included in the SI.

3

Given that the PRRP success is “assigned” at the level of countries, we also apply the more conservative country-clustered standard errors across all specification with no change in the substantive results (see table S1).

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material may be found in the online version of this article: https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfad052.

Funding

This research was made possible by the generous support of the Leverhulme Trust [ECF-2021-342 to J.D.].

Data Availability

Replication data and documentation are available at https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/EPIQHH.

References

Abou-Chadi
Tarik
,
Krause
Werner
.
2020
. “
The Causal Effect of Radical Right Success on Mainstream Parties’ Policy Positions: A Regression Discontinuity Approach
.”
British Journal of Political Science
 
50
:
829
47
.

Banai
Adam
,
Votta
Francesco
,
Seitz
Rainer
.
2022
. “
The Polls—Trends: Trends in Public Opinion toward Immigration among EU Member States
.”
Public Opinion Quarterly
 
86
:
191
215
.

Bischof
Daniel
,
Wagner
Markus
.
2019
. “
Do Voters Polarize When Radical Parties Enter Parliament?
American Journal of Political Science
 
63
:
888
904
.

Blinder
Scott
,
Ford
Robert
,
Ivarsflaten
Elisabeth
.
2013
. “
The Better Angels of Our Nature: How the Antiprejudice Norm Affects Policy and Party Preferences in Great Britain and Germany
.”
American Journal of Political Science
 
57
:
841
57
.

Caughey
Devin
,
O’Grady
Tom
,
Warshaw
Christopher
.
2019
. “
Policy Ideology in European Mass Publics, 1981–2016
.”
American Political Science Review
 
113
:
674
93
.

Claassen
Christopher
,
McLaren
Lauren
.
2021
. “
Does Immigration Produce a Public Backlash or Public Acceptance? Time-Series, Cross-Sectional Evidence from Thirty European Democracies
.”
British Journal of Political Science
 
52
:
1013
31
.

Collingwood
Loren
,
Lajevardi
Nazita
,
Oskooii
Kassra A. R.
 
2018
. “
A change of heart? Why individual-level public opinion shifted against Trump's “Muslim Ban”
.”
Political Behavior
 
40
:
1035
72
.

Czopp
Alexander M.
,
Monteith
Margo J.
 
2003
. “
Confronting Prejudice (Literally): Reactions to Confrontations of Racial and Gender Bias
.”
Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin
 
29
:
532
44
.

Dai
Yaoyao
,
Kustov
Alexander
.
2022
. “
When Do Politicians Use Populist Rhetoric? Populism as a Campaign Gamble
.”
Political Communication
 
39
:
383
404
.

Dennison
James
,
Davidov
Eldad
,
Seddig
Daniel
.
2020
. “
Explaining Voting in the UK’s 2016 EU Referendum: Basic Human Values, Attitudes to Immigration
.”
European Identity and Trust in Politicians." Social Science Research
 
92
:
102476
.

Dennison
James
,
Geddes
Andrew
.
2019
. “
A Rising Tide? The Salience of Immigration and the Rise of Anti-Immigration Political Parties in Western Europe
.”
The Political Quarterly
 
90
:
107
16
.

Devine
Patricia G.
 
1989
. “
Stereotypes and Prejudices: Their Automatic and Controlled Components
.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
 
56
:
5
18
.

Devine
Patricia G.
,
Monteith
Margo J.
,
Zuwerink
Julia R.
,
Elliot
Andrew J.
 
1991
. “
Prejudice with and without Compunction
.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
 
60
:
817
30
.

Dinesen
Peter Thisted
,
Klemmensen
Robert
,
Nørgaard
Asbjørn Sonne
.
2016
. “
Personality and Immigration Attitudes
.”
Political Psychology
 
37
:
55
72
.

Dovidio
John F.
,
Gaertner
Samuel L.
 
2004
. “
Aversive Racism
.”
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology
 
36
:
4
56
.

Fazio
R. H.
,
Sanbonmatsu
D. M.
,
Powell
M. C.
,
Kardes
F. R.
 
1986
. “
On the Automatic Activation of Attitudes
.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
 
50
:
229
38
.

García-Faroldi
Luana.
 
2017
. “
Determinants of Attitudes towards Immigration: Testing the Influence of Interculturalism, Group Threat Theory and National Contexts in Time of Crisis
.”
International Migration
 
55
:
10
22
.

Harteveld
Eelco
,
Kokkonen
Andrej
,
Dahlberg
Stefan
.
2017
. “
Adapting to Party Lines: The Effect of Party Affiliation on Attitudes to Immigration
.”
West European Politics
 
40
:
1177
97
.

Heinö
Andreas Johansson.
 
2016
. Timbro Authoritarian Populism Index. Timbro. https://timbro.se/allmant/timbro-authoritarian-populism-index/.

Hellwig
Timothy
,
Kweon
Younghoon
.
2016
. “
Taking Cues on Multidimensional Issues: The Case of Attitudes toward Immigration
.”
West European Politics
 
39
:
710
30
.

Imai
Kosuke
,
Kim
In Song
.
2021
. “
On the Use of Two-Way Fixed Effects Regression Models for Causal Inference with Panel Data
.”
Political Analysis
 
29
:
405
15
.

Kauff
Mathias
,
Asbrock
Frank
,
Thörner
Stefan
,
Wagner
Ulrich
.
2013
. “
Side Effects of Multiculturalism: The Interaction Effect of a Multicultural Ideology and Authoritarianism on Prejudice and Diversity Beliefs
.”
Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin
 
39
:
305
20
.

Kropko
Jonathan
,
Kubinec
Robert
.
2020
. “
Interpretation and Identification of Within-Unit and Cross-Sectional Variation in Panel Data Models
.”
PloS One
 
15
:
e0231349
.

Kustov
Alexander
,
Laaker
Denise
,
Reller
Connor
.
2021
. “
The Stability of Immigration Attitudes: Evidence and Implications
.”
The Journal of Politics
 
83
:
1478
94
.

Kustov
Alexander.
 
2023
. “
Testing the Backlash Argument: Voter Responses to (Pro-) Immigration Reforms
.”
Journal of European Public Policy
 
30
:
1183
203
.

Lindstam
Emma
,
Mader
Matthias
,
Schoen
Harald
.
2021
. “
Conceptions of National Identity and Ambivalence towards Immigration
.”
British Journal of Political Science
 
51
:
93
114
.

McLaren
Lauren
,
Neundorf
Anja
,
Paterson
Ian
.
2021
. “
Diversity and Perceptions of Immigration: How the Past Influences the Present
.”
Political Studies
 
69
:
725
47
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0032321720922774.

McLaren
Lauren
,
Paterson
Ian
.
2020
. “
Generational Change and Attitudes to Immigration
.”
Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies
 
46
:
665
82
.

Meléndez
Carlos
,
Kaltwasser
Cristóbal Rovira
.
2021
. “
Negative Partisanship towards the Populist Radical Right and Democratic Resilience in Western Europe
.”
Democratization
 
28
:
949
69
. https://doi.org/10.1080/13510347.2021.1883002.

Mendes
Mariana S.
,
Dennison
James
.
2021
. “
Explaining the Emergence of the Radical Right in Spain and Portugal: Salience, Stigma, and Supply
.”
West European Politics
 
44
:
752
75
. https://doi.org/10.1080/01402382.2020.1777504.

Monteith
Margo J.
,
Ashburn-Nardo
Leslie
,
Voils
Corrine I
,
Czopp
Alexander M.
 
2002
. “
Putting the Brakes on Prejudice: On the Development and Operation of Cues for Control
.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
 
83
:
1029
50
.

Rooduijn
Matthijs
,
Van der Brug
Wouter
,
De Lange
Sarah L.
 
2016
. “
Expressing or Fuelling Discontent? The Relationship between Populist Voting and Political Discontent
.”
Electoral Studies
 
43
:
32
40
.

Rooduijn
Matthijs
,
Van Kessel
Stijn
,
Froio
Caterina
,
Pirro
Andrea L. P.
,
De Lange
Sarah L.
,
Halikiopoulou
Daphne
,
Lewis
Paul
,
Mudde
Cas
,
Taggart
Paul
.
2019
. The PopuList: An Overview of Populist, Far Right, Far Left and Eurosceptic Parties in Europe. https://www.popu-list.org.

Schwartz
Corey
,
Simon
Markus
,
Hudson
David
,
Van-Heerde-Hudson
Jennifer
.
2021
. “
A Populist Paradox? How Brexit Softened Anti-Immigrant Attitudes
.”
British Journal of Political Science
 
51
:
1160
80
.

Solodoch
Omer.
 
2021
. “
Regaining Control? The Political Impact of Policy Responses to Refugee Crises
.”
International Organization
 
75
:
735
68
.

Valentim
Vicente.
 
2021
. “
Parliamentary Representation and the Normalization of Radical Right Support
.”
Comparative Political Studies
 
54
:
2475
511
.

Van Spanje
Joost.
 
2010
. “
Contagious Parties: Anti-immigration Parties and their Impact on Other Parties’ Immigration Stances in Contemporary Europe
.”
Party Politics
 
16
:
563
86
.

Vrânceanu
Ana
,
Lachat
Romain
.
2021
. “
Do Parties Influence Public Opinion on Immigration? Evidence from Europe
.”
Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties
 
31
:
1
2
.

Wlezien
Christopher.
 
1995
. “
The Public as Thermostat: Dynamics of Preferences for Spending
.”
American Journal of Political Science
 
39
:
981
1000
. https://doi.org/10.2307/2111666.

Zaller
John R.
 
1992
.
The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion
.
Cambridge
:
Cambridge University Press
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Supplementary data