Abstract

Objectives

Identifying sociocultural correlates of neurocognitive dysfunction among Hispanics/Latinos, and their underlying biological pathways, is crucial for understanding disparities in Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. We examined cross-sectional associations between stress and neurocognition, and the role that metabolic syndrome (MetS) and systemic inflammation might play in these associations.

Method

Participants included 3,045 adults aged 45–75 (56% female, education 0–20+ years, 86% Spanish-speaking, 23% U.S.-born), enrolled in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos and its Sociocultural Ancillary Study. Global neurocognition was the primary outcome and operationalized as the average of the z scores of measures of learning and memory, word fluency, and processing speed. Stress measures included self-report assessments of stress appraisal (perceived and acculturative stress) and exposure to chronic and traumatic stressors. MetS was defined via established criteria including waist circumference, high blood pressure, elevated triglycerides, fasting plasma glucose, and high levels of high-density lipoprotein cholesterol. Systemic inflammation was represented by high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (hs-CRP).

Results

Separate survey multivariable linear regression models adjusting for covariates showed that higher perceived (b = −0.004, SE = 0.002, p < .05) and acculturative stress (b = −0.004, SE = 0.001, p < .0001) were significantly associated with worse global neurocognition, while lifetime exposure to traumatic stressors was associated with better global neurocognition (b = 0.034, SE = 0.009, p < .001). Neither MetS nor hs-CRP were notable pathways in the association between stress and neurocognition; rather, they were both independently associated with worse neurocognition in models including stress measures (ps < .05).

Discussion

These cross-sectional analyses suggest that stress appraisal, MetS, and systemic inflammation may be targets to reduce neurocognitive dysfunction among Hispanics/Latinos.

Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (ADRDs) are a major public health issue, particularly among Hispanics/Latinos, who may be at increased risk for mild cognitive impairment and ADRDs (González et al., 2015; Gurland et al., 1999; Tang et al., 2001). With the growing population of Hispanics/Latinos reaching older age, the number of Hispanics/Latinos with Alzheimer’s disease is expected to increase to 3.5 million by 2060—a growth of 832% relative to 2012—representing the largest increase in ADRDs of any racial/ethnic group in the United States (USC Edward R. Roybal Institute on Aging and the LatinosAgainstAlzheimer’s Network, 2016). While the factors underlying these Hispanic/Latino disparities in ADRDs are not well understood, they do not appear to include known genetic risks for ADRD (Farrer et al., 1997). Better understanding of key sociocultural factors underlying neurocognitive function among older Hispanics/Latinos, and the biological pathways contributing to these associations, may facilitate targeted culturally relevant interventions aimed at preventing neurocognitive disorders in this group.

Among the many potential sociocultural factors impacting neurocognitive function in older Hispanics/Latinos, stress may be an important one to consider. Some Hispanics/Latinos face stressors related to socioeconomic disadvantages, limited English language use, place of birth, and experiences with discrimination. Notably, stress has been linked to increased risk for ADRDs and poor neurocognitive function among older persons (Lupien et al., 2009; Machado et al., 2014). Most of the research in this area has been in primarily non-Hispanic samples, and the scant research among Hispanics/Latinos has yielded mixed findings, possibly due to varying methodological approaches across studies, including different stress assessments (Muñoz et al., 2021; Nguyen et al., 2012; ,Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019). Stress is a complex multidimensional construct, with measures differing in whether they capture the experience of challenging environmental events (e.g., trauma, life events), or the appraisal of these events (e.g., perceived stress), as well as dimensions of time and severity (Gallo, Roesch, et al., 2014; Monroe, 2008). In the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos (HCHS/SOL) Muñoz and colleagues (2021) examined whether measures of chronic stress exposure and acculturative stress (i.e., the psychosocial strain experienced as a result of the process of adapting to cultural norms that differ from one’s own) were associated with individual neurocognitive tests among middle- to older-aged Hispanics/Latinos (Muñoz et al., 2021). Chronic stress was associated with worse verbal learning, though this association was no longer significant after adjusting for a range of mental (depression and anxiety) and physical health symptoms (i.e., presence of hypertension, coronary heart disease/angina, stroke/transient ischemic attack, diabetes, and heart failure; and body mass index classification). Acculturative stress was linked with worse performance on several neurocognitive tests, with most associations remaining significant after similar adjustments. While not directly examined, some of these findings support the notion that a marker of cardiovascular disease (CVD) risk, such as metabolic syndrome (MetS), might play an important role in the association between stress and neurocognition among Hispanics/Latinos. The increased prevalence of MetS and its components in this group (Heiss et al., 2014) further highlights its relevance to understanding disparities in neurocognitive outcomes among Hispanics/Latinos.

Several lines of research indicate that stress may influence neurocognition through both direct and indirect pathways. Stress has been linked to CVD (Dimsdale, 2008; Steptoe & Kivimäki, 2013) and chronic inflammation (Hänsel et al., 2010; Shivpuri et al., 2012), which in turn have been associated with poor neurocognitive outcomes (Heppner et al., 2015; Siervo et al., 2014), underscoring the role of these factors as potentially important underlying mechanisms in the link between stress and ADRD risk (Bisht et al., 2018; Justice, 2018). A small number of studies have directly examined the mediating role of metabolic and inflammation markers in the association of stress and cognition (Vitaliano et al., 2005; Zahodne, Kraal, et al., 2019). For example, Zhadone and colleagues (2019) found that systemic inflammation (as assessed by high-sensitivity C-reactive protein [hs-CRP]) partially mediated the association between discrimination and memory cross-sectionally in a diverse group of persons aged 51+, but did not mediate longitudinal association between discrimination and cognitive decline, while Vitalino and colleagues (2005) showed that an index of metabolic risk partially mediated differences in longitudinal neurocognitive decline between caregivers and noncaregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease.

Within Hispanics/Latinos, stress has been linked to increased presence of CVD risk factors and inflammation (Gallo, Roesch, et al., 2014; Isasi et al., 2015; McCurley et al., 2015), as well as with adverse health behaviors linked to CVD such as poor diet, sedentary behaviors, and smoking (Gallo, Roesch, et al., 2014; Isasi et al., 2015). In turn, CVD risk, including composite measures such as MetS, has been associated with worse neurocognitive function (González et al., 2018; Lamar et al., 2019; Tarraf et al., 2020; Yaffe et al., 2007). Yet, investigation of biological pathways, including MetS and systemic inflammation, linking stress and neurocognition among Hispanics/Latinos is scant.

Building upon previous work in HCHS/SOL (González et al., 2018; Muñoz et al., 2021), the main purpose of the current study was to examine the roles that MetS and systemic inflammation may play in associations between stress (appraisal and exposure) and neurocognition among middle- and older-aged Hispanics/Latinos. In order to do so, we first examined associations between several key stress constructs relevant to Hispanic/Latino health (i.e., perceived stress, acculturative stress, and exposure to ongoing chronic stressors and traumatic events) with neurocognitive function (global and domain specific) in a large and well-characterized cohort of middle- and older-aged Hispanics/Latinos from the HCHS/SOL study. The stress measures that were the focus of this study represent both measures of stress appraisal (perceived and acculturative stress) and exposure to experiences that are typically considered challenging (life events/trauma), and have been linked to worse neurocognition and/or MetS indicators within Hispanics/Latinos (Gallo, Roesch, et al., 2014; Muñoz et al., 2021). For those stress measures that were significantly associated with neurocognition, we investigated whether MetS and systemic inflammation (assessed via serum hs-CRP) helped explain the association between stress and neurocognition via pathway analyses. We hypothesized that higher stress, particularly the appraisal of stress, would be associated with worse neurocognitive function, and with increased MetS and inflammation. Furthermore, accounting for MetS and inflammation would significantly attenuate the association between increased stress and decreased neurocognition among middle- and older-aged Hispanics/Latinos. We also investigated whether the experience of stress might interact with the presence of MetS and inflammation to impact neurocognition. Given the biological importance of age and sex (Farrer et al., 1997) in the development of neurocognitive disorders, a secondary aim of the study was to examine whether age and sex might modify the associations between stress and neurocognition. As we strive to improve our understanding of risk factors for neurocognitive problems in Hispanics/Latinos, it is important to consider whether such factors differ by sex or with increasing age.

Method

Participants

The HCHS/SOL is a multisite (Bronx, NY; Chicago, IL; Miami, FL; and San Diego, CA), prospective cohort study (Visit 1 [V1]; 2008–2011 total unweighted n = 16,415) of CVD among diverse Hispanics/Latinos aged 18–74 years at V1 (Sorlie et al., 2010). Current analyses included HCHS/SOL participants 45–74 years old, who participated in its Sociocultural Ancillary Study (SCAS) and had neurocognitive data available. Study details are available in previously published reports (Gallo, Penedo, et al., 2014; González et al., 2015; Sorlie et al., 2010). Briefly, all HCHS/SOL participants who consented to being contacted for future research and were willing and able to attend a separate visit within 9 months of HCHS/SOL V1 were eligible for SCAS. During the SCAS visit, which for most participants occurred within 4 months of HCHS/SOL V1, participants completed multiple measures of stress and other sociocultural factors (Gallo, Penedo, et al., 2014). SCAS enrolled 5,313 eligible HCHS/SOL participants, of which 3,282 completed neurocognitive testing during HCHS/SOL V1. For this analysis, we excluded 15 participants in HCHS/SOL younger than 45 years old and 120 participants who self-reported histories of stroke or transient ischemic attack. We also excluded 102 participants who were missing data on covariates of interest (see section below). The unweighted analytical sample size was n = 3,045.

Materials and Procedures

Neurocognition was assessed via the Brief-Spanish English Verbal Learning Test (B-SEVLT (González et al., 2002), the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT)- Letters F and A (Benton & Hamsher, 1989), and Digit Symbol Substitution test (Wechsler, 1981). Participants completed the neurocognitive assessment battery in their preferred language (Spanish or English). Individual continuous neurocognitive tests scores were transformed into z scores derived from the entire sample of participants who completed neurocognitive testing during HCHS/SOL V1. Global neurocognition was the primary outcome and operationalized as the average of the z scores using four neurocognitive measures, including learning (sum of three learning trials on the B-SEVLT), memory (recall trial of the B-SEVLT), word fluency (sum of scores on letters F and A on the COWAT), and processing speed (Digit Symbol Substitution test total score).

Participants completed various self-reported measures of stress. Perceived stress was assessed via the Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen et al., 1983), a 10-item Likert-type measure of the degree to which situations in one’s life over the past month are appraised as stressful (range 0–40), and higher scores indicate higher perceived stress. Acculturative stress was assessed using the Hispanic Stress Inventory (Cervantes et al., 1990), which measures stress associated with the process of living and integrating in a new culture. This 17-item scale asks about specific events over the last 3 months (e.g., “Because I am Latino I have had difficulty finding the type of work I want”), and degree of worry/tension associated with each event using a 5-point scale (from [1] “Not at all worried/tense” to [5] “Extremely worried/tense”). Higher total scores (range 0–85) indicate more acculturative stress. Current chronic stress was measured via the Chronic Stress Burden Scale (Bromberger & Matthews, 1996), an eight-item measure which captures the ongoing presence of life stressors (e.g., Have you had a serious ongoing health problem?) in several domains (e.g., health, work, finances, personal relationships). Total scores (range: 0–8) represent the total number of current stressors lasting at least 6 months. Exposure to traumatic events (e.g., robberies, natural disasters, sexual and physical assault) was assessed via the Traumatic Stress Schedule (Norris, 1990), a 10-item measure that asks about exposure to traumatic life events (e.g., “Did anyone ever beat you up or attack you?”). Scores range from 0 to 10 and reflect lifetime and past-year exposure to these events.

MetS was operationalized using International Diabetes Federation (2006). Accordingly, MetS was defined by the presence of abdominal obesity (waist circumference >94 cm in men and >80 cm in women) and the presence of two or more of the following: (a) triglycerides ≥150 mg/dL or treatment for lipid abnormality, (b) high-density lipoprotein (HDL)-cholesterol <40 mg/dL in men and <50 mg/dL in women or specified treatment for lipid abnormality, (c) blood pressure ≥130/85 mmHg or use of antihypertension medications, and (d) fasting glucose ≥100 mg/dL, or previous diagnosis of diabetes or use of antidiabetic medications. In secondary analyses, we investigated individual components of MetS and incorporated pulse pressure. Inflammation was assessed via hs-CRP, which was measured continuously and categorically using standard clinical thresholds, that is, ≤3 mg/L as normal and >3 mg/L as high (Pearson et al., 2003).

Covariates included the following demographic characteristics: age in years, years of education, sex, place of birth (U.S. or U.S. territory vs foreign-born), Hispanic/Latino heritage background, and language preference. In sensitivity models, we also controlled for study site and two mental health measures assessing symptoms of depression and anxiety. Depression was measured via the 10-item Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (Andresen et al., 1994), which assesses the frequency of depressive symptoms in the past week, with higher scores indicating higher depression (range 0–30). Anxiety was measured via the 10-item Trait Anxiety Subscale of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger et al., 1983), with scores ranging from 10 to 40, and higher scores indicating greater anxiety.

Statistical Analyses

We generated descriptive statistics to characterize our target population overall, and by MetS (yes/no) and inflammation status (normal and high). Survey adjusted t tests were used to test differences in continuous measures and survey adjusted chi-squared tests for categorical variables. In order to investigate the roles that MetS and systemic inflammation may play in explaining associations between stress and neurocognition among Hispanics/Latinos, we first fit survey linear models to independently examine the associations between each of the stress exposures and global neurocognition (measured in z score units), following a four-step approach: (a) adjustment for demographics (age, sex, years of education, Hispanic/Latino background, place of birth, and language preference); (b) adjustment for demographics and MetS only; (c) adjustment for demographics and hs-CRP only; and (d) adjustment for demographics, MetS, and hs-CRP simultaneously. Unstandardized regression coefficients and standard errors were generated and presented in tables. Standardized beta coefficients can be obtained by multiplying the reported coefficients by the standard deviation of the stress measures of interest as presented in Table 1. Sensitivity analyses included adjustments for study site and symptoms of depression and anxiety. This process was repeated for all secondary outcomes to examine neurocognitive domain-specific associations between the stress measures and neurocognition and the degree of attenuation through MetS and inflammation adjustments. All outcomes were modeled using survey linear regression. Additionally, we modeled the primary outcome (global neurocognition) as a function of the individual MetS factors, as well as pulse pressure to investigate the independent impact of individual components. For stress measures that were significantly associated with global neurocognition and biomarkers (MetS and hs-CRP), we then fit structural equation models (SEMs) to estimate direct, indirect (through hs-CRP and MetS status), and total effects for the associations between stress measures and global neurocognition. We followed standard recommendations for fitting the models, and derived direct, indirect, and total effects and their SEs within an SEM framework. We used Robust Maximum Likelihood estimators to account for potential deviation from normality assumptions in the outcomes. SEM modeling was conducted using MPLUS version 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017), which allows testing of complex indirect effects models using noncontinuous mediators (binary measures in our case) and could accommodate the complex design features of the data (Muthén et al., 2017). Theoretical and applied treatments of these methods have been published elsewhere (Hayes, 2017). To investigate whether MetS and hs-CRP modified the association between stress and cognition, we refit fully adjusted survey linear regression models on global neurocognition, including interaction terms of stress by MetS (yes/no) and hs-CRP (high/normal). Lastly, to investigate whether age or sex modified the association between stress and neurocognition, we refit fully adjusted survey linear regression models on global neurocognition, including interaction terms of stress by age (younger: 45–64 and older: 65+) and sex (male and female).

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos Target Population Overall, and by Metabolic Syndrome (MetS) and High-Sensitivity C-Reactive Protein (hs-CRP)

Overall sampleaMetSahs-CRPa,b
NoYesp valueNormalHighp value
Demographics
 Age56.60 (9.86)54.70 (9.09)58.30 (10.05)<.00156.40 (9.65)56.89 (10.16).299
 Years of education11.27 (5.50)11.79 (5.48)10.81 (5.45)<.00111.34 (5.58)11.16 (5.37).418
 Female sex, % (SE)56.33 (1.30)55.01 (1.96)57.51 (1.72).33548.96 (1.77)67.20 (1.83)<.001
 Language preference (% [SE] Spanish)85.62 (1.58)83.56 (2.52)87.45 (1.42).10484.83 (1.88)86.78 (2.26).468
 U.S.-born, % (SE)22.61 (1.72)22.22 (2.58)22.96 (1.84).79420.80 (1.98)25.27 (2.45).103
 Latino background, % (SE).354.010
  Mexican29.30 (2.24)30.30 (2.90)28.41 (2.33)32.77 (2.59)24.21 (2.33)
  Cuban28.42 (2.60)26.76 (2.91)29.91 (2.83)26.43 (2.66)31.37 (3.30)
  Puerto Rican18.38 (1.61)17.74 (2.46)18.96 (1.66)16.70 (1.83)20.86 (2.43)
  Dominican10.21 (0.99)11.58 (1.34)9.00 (1.09)10.08 (1.12)10.42 (1.29)
  Central American6.34 (0.67)5.66 (0.74)6.95 (0.90)6.21 (0.82)6.53 (0.83)
  South American6.15 (0.56)6.70 (0.85)5.67 (0.72)6.59 (0.74)5.51 (0.75)
  More than one heritage1.18 (0.21)1.27 (0.32)1.10 (0.27)1.22 (0.31)1.11 (0.27)
Cognition, z scores
 Global neurocognition0.00 (0.77)0.12 (0.74)−0.11 (0.77)<.0010.04 (0.76)−0.06 (0.78).004
 B-SEVLT sum−0.00 (1.00)0.12 (0.99)−0.11 (0.99)<.0010.02 (1.01)−0.04 (0.98).155
 B-SEVLT recall0.00 (1.00)0.11 (0.96)−0.09 (1.02)<.0010.05 (0.97)−0.07 (1.03).011
 Word fluency0.01 (1.01)0.11 (1.02)−0.09 (0.99)<.0010.06 (0.99)−0.08 (1.03).002
 Digit Symbol Substitution0.02 (0.97)0.16 (0.96)−0.10 (0.95)<.0010.05 (0.95)−0.02 (0.98).111
Stress measures
 Total perceived stress14.42 (8.61)14.32 (8.40)14.51 (8.80).61114.09 (8.46)14.90 (8.81).026
 Total acculturative stress12.88 (16.14)13.25 (16.44)12.55 (15.86).32112.64 (16.41)13.23 (15.72).391
 Chronic stress total2.03 (2.02)1.99 (1.96)2.07 (2.06).3871.95 (2.00)2.16 (2.03).018
 Traumatic stress
  Lifetime total2.15 (2.14)2.31 (2.19)2.00 (2.07).0132.22 (2.12)2.04 (2.14).100
  Past year0.14 (0.55)0.15 (0.59)0.13 (0.50).5370.13 (0.52)0.14 (0.58).742
MetS, % (SE)52.83 (1.36)44.50 (1.86)65.09 (1.96)<.001
 Abdominal obesity86.06 (0.84)80.47 (1.27)94.30 (0.86)<.001
 Hypertension58.38 (1.33)54.91 (1.83)63.49 (1.86).001
 High fasting glucose43.87 (1.53)40.63 (1.89)48.65 (2.08).002
 Low HDL-cholesterol38.95 (1.24)32.23 (1.62)48.83 (2.06)<.001
 High triglycerides37.07 (1.25)35.05 (1.65)40.05 (1.89).047
Inflammation
 hs-CRP (continuous)4.54 (9.37)3.79 (9.97)5.21 (8.72)<.0011.41 (0.94)12.72 (9.04)<.001
 hs-CRP, % (SE) high40.45 (1.38)29.93 (2.08)49.83 (1.68)<.001
Overall sampleaMetSahs-CRPa,b
NoYesp valueNormalHighp value
Demographics
 Age56.60 (9.86)54.70 (9.09)58.30 (10.05)<.00156.40 (9.65)56.89 (10.16).299
 Years of education11.27 (5.50)11.79 (5.48)10.81 (5.45)<.00111.34 (5.58)11.16 (5.37).418
 Female sex, % (SE)56.33 (1.30)55.01 (1.96)57.51 (1.72).33548.96 (1.77)67.20 (1.83)<.001
 Language preference (% [SE] Spanish)85.62 (1.58)83.56 (2.52)87.45 (1.42).10484.83 (1.88)86.78 (2.26).468
 U.S.-born, % (SE)22.61 (1.72)22.22 (2.58)22.96 (1.84).79420.80 (1.98)25.27 (2.45).103
 Latino background, % (SE).354.010
  Mexican29.30 (2.24)30.30 (2.90)28.41 (2.33)32.77 (2.59)24.21 (2.33)
  Cuban28.42 (2.60)26.76 (2.91)29.91 (2.83)26.43 (2.66)31.37 (3.30)
  Puerto Rican18.38 (1.61)17.74 (2.46)18.96 (1.66)16.70 (1.83)20.86 (2.43)
  Dominican10.21 (0.99)11.58 (1.34)9.00 (1.09)10.08 (1.12)10.42 (1.29)
  Central American6.34 (0.67)5.66 (0.74)6.95 (0.90)6.21 (0.82)6.53 (0.83)
  South American6.15 (0.56)6.70 (0.85)5.67 (0.72)6.59 (0.74)5.51 (0.75)
  More than one heritage1.18 (0.21)1.27 (0.32)1.10 (0.27)1.22 (0.31)1.11 (0.27)
Cognition, z scores
 Global neurocognition0.00 (0.77)0.12 (0.74)−0.11 (0.77)<.0010.04 (0.76)−0.06 (0.78).004
 B-SEVLT sum−0.00 (1.00)0.12 (0.99)−0.11 (0.99)<.0010.02 (1.01)−0.04 (0.98).155
 B-SEVLT recall0.00 (1.00)0.11 (0.96)−0.09 (1.02)<.0010.05 (0.97)−0.07 (1.03).011
 Word fluency0.01 (1.01)0.11 (1.02)−0.09 (0.99)<.0010.06 (0.99)−0.08 (1.03).002
 Digit Symbol Substitution0.02 (0.97)0.16 (0.96)−0.10 (0.95)<.0010.05 (0.95)−0.02 (0.98).111
Stress measures
 Total perceived stress14.42 (8.61)14.32 (8.40)14.51 (8.80).61114.09 (8.46)14.90 (8.81).026
 Total acculturative stress12.88 (16.14)13.25 (16.44)12.55 (15.86).32112.64 (16.41)13.23 (15.72).391
 Chronic stress total2.03 (2.02)1.99 (1.96)2.07 (2.06).3871.95 (2.00)2.16 (2.03).018
 Traumatic stress
  Lifetime total2.15 (2.14)2.31 (2.19)2.00 (2.07).0132.22 (2.12)2.04 (2.14).100
  Past year0.14 (0.55)0.15 (0.59)0.13 (0.50).5370.13 (0.52)0.14 (0.58).742
MetS, % (SE)52.83 (1.36)44.50 (1.86)65.09 (1.96)<.001
 Abdominal obesity86.06 (0.84)80.47 (1.27)94.30 (0.86)<.001
 Hypertension58.38 (1.33)54.91 (1.83)63.49 (1.86).001
 High fasting glucose43.87 (1.53)40.63 (1.89)48.65 (2.08).002
 Low HDL-cholesterol38.95 (1.24)32.23 (1.62)48.83 (2.06)<.001
 High triglycerides37.07 (1.25)35.05 (1.65)40.05 (1.89).047
Inflammation
 hs-CRP (continuous)4.54 (9.37)3.79 (9.97)5.21 (8.72)<.0011.41 (0.94)12.72 (9.04)<.001
 hs-CRP, % (SE) high40.45 (1.38)29.93 (2.08)49.83 (1.68)<.001

Notes: B-SEVLT = Brief-Spanish English Verbal Learning Test; HDL-cholesterol = high-density lipoprotein cholesterol. Sum: sum of three learning trials. p Values are derived from chi-squared tests for categorical variables and t tests for continuous variables.

aValues represent means (SDs) unless otherwise noted. bNormal: ≤3 mg/L and high: >3 mg/L.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos Target Population Overall, and by Metabolic Syndrome (MetS) and High-Sensitivity C-Reactive Protein (hs-CRP)

Overall sampleaMetSahs-CRPa,b
NoYesp valueNormalHighp value
Demographics
 Age56.60 (9.86)54.70 (9.09)58.30 (10.05)<.00156.40 (9.65)56.89 (10.16).299
 Years of education11.27 (5.50)11.79 (5.48)10.81 (5.45)<.00111.34 (5.58)11.16 (5.37).418
 Female sex, % (SE)56.33 (1.30)55.01 (1.96)57.51 (1.72).33548.96 (1.77)67.20 (1.83)<.001
 Language preference (% [SE] Spanish)85.62 (1.58)83.56 (2.52)87.45 (1.42).10484.83 (1.88)86.78 (2.26).468
 U.S.-born, % (SE)22.61 (1.72)22.22 (2.58)22.96 (1.84).79420.80 (1.98)25.27 (2.45).103
 Latino background, % (SE).354.010
  Mexican29.30 (2.24)30.30 (2.90)28.41 (2.33)32.77 (2.59)24.21 (2.33)
  Cuban28.42 (2.60)26.76 (2.91)29.91 (2.83)26.43 (2.66)31.37 (3.30)
  Puerto Rican18.38 (1.61)17.74 (2.46)18.96 (1.66)16.70 (1.83)20.86 (2.43)
  Dominican10.21 (0.99)11.58 (1.34)9.00 (1.09)10.08 (1.12)10.42 (1.29)
  Central American6.34 (0.67)5.66 (0.74)6.95 (0.90)6.21 (0.82)6.53 (0.83)
  South American6.15 (0.56)6.70 (0.85)5.67 (0.72)6.59 (0.74)5.51 (0.75)
  More than one heritage1.18 (0.21)1.27 (0.32)1.10 (0.27)1.22 (0.31)1.11 (0.27)
Cognition, z scores
 Global neurocognition0.00 (0.77)0.12 (0.74)−0.11 (0.77)<.0010.04 (0.76)−0.06 (0.78).004
 B-SEVLT sum−0.00 (1.00)0.12 (0.99)−0.11 (0.99)<.0010.02 (1.01)−0.04 (0.98).155
 B-SEVLT recall0.00 (1.00)0.11 (0.96)−0.09 (1.02)<.0010.05 (0.97)−0.07 (1.03).011
 Word fluency0.01 (1.01)0.11 (1.02)−0.09 (0.99)<.0010.06 (0.99)−0.08 (1.03).002
 Digit Symbol Substitution0.02 (0.97)0.16 (0.96)−0.10 (0.95)<.0010.05 (0.95)−0.02 (0.98).111
Stress measures
 Total perceived stress14.42 (8.61)14.32 (8.40)14.51 (8.80).61114.09 (8.46)14.90 (8.81).026
 Total acculturative stress12.88 (16.14)13.25 (16.44)12.55 (15.86).32112.64 (16.41)13.23 (15.72).391
 Chronic stress total2.03 (2.02)1.99 (1.96)2.07 (2.06).3871.95 (2.00)2.16 (2.03).018
 Traumatic stress
  Lifetime total2.15 (2.14)2.31 (2.19)2.00 (2.07).0132.22 (2.12)2.04 (2.14).100
  Past year0.14 (0.55)0.15 (0.59)0.13 (0.50).5370.13 (0.52)0.14 (0.58).742
MetS, % (SE)52.83 (1.36)44.50 (1.86)65.09 (1.96)<.001
 Abdominal obesity86.06 (0.84)80.47 (1.27)94.30 (0.86)<.001
 Hypertension58.38 (1.33)54.91 (1.83)63.49 (1.86).001
 High fasting glucose43.87 (1.53)40.63 (1.89)48.65 (2.08).002
 Low HDL-cholesterol38.95 (1.24)32.23 (1.62)48.83 (2.06)<.001
 High triglycerides37.07 (1.25)35.05 (1.65)40.05 (1.89).047
Inflammation
 hs-CRP (continuous)4.54 (9.37)3.79 (9.97)5.21 (8.72)<.0011.41 (0.94)12.72 (9.04)<.001
 hs-CRP, % (SE) high40.45 (1.38)29.93 (2.08)49.83 (1.68)<.001
Overall sampleaMetSahs-CRPa,b
NoYesp valueNormalHighp value
Demographics
 Age56.60 (9.86)54.70 (9.09)58.30 (10.05)<.00156.40 (9.65)56.89 (10.16).299
 Years of education11.27 (5.50)11.79 (5.48)10.81 (5.45)<.00111.34 (5.58)11.16 (5.37).418
 Female sex, % (SE)56.33 (1.30)55.01 (1.96)57.51 (1.72).33548.96 (1.77)67.20 (1.83)<.001
 Language preference (% [SE] Spanish)85.62 (1.58)83.56 (2.52)87.45 (1.42).10484.83 (1.88)86.78 (2.26).468
 U.S.-born, % (SE)22.61 (1.72)22.22 (2.58)22.96 (1.84).79420.80 (1.98)25.27 (2.45).103
 Latino background, % (SE).354.010
  Mexican29.30 (2.24)30.30 (2.90)28.41 (2.33)32.77 (2.59)24.21 (2.33)
  Cuban28.42 (2.60)26.76 (2.91)29.91 (2.83)26.43 (2.66)31.37 (3.30)
  Puerto Rican18.38 (1.61)17.74 (2.46)18.96 (1.66)16.70 (1.83)20.86 (2.43)
  Dominican10.21 (0.99)11.58 (1.34)9.00 (1.09)10.08 (1.12)10.42 (1.29)
  Central American6.34 (0.67)5.66 (0.74)6.95 (0.90)6.21 (0.82)6.53 (0.83)
  South American6.15 (0.56)6.70 (0.85)5.67 (0.72)6.59 (0.74)5.51 (0.75)
  More than one heritage1.18 (0.21)1.27 (0.32)1.10 (0.27)1.22 (0.31)1.11 (0.27)
Cognition, z scores
 Global neurocognition0.00 (0.77)0.12 (0.74)−0.11 (0.77)<.0010.04 (0.76)−0.06 (0.78).004
 B-SEVLT sum−0.00 (1.00)0.12 (0.99)−0.11 (0.99)<.0010.02 (1.01)−0.04 (0.98).155
 B-SEVLT recall0.00 (1.00)0.11 (0.96)−0.09 (1.02)<.0010.05 (0.97)−0.07 (1.03).011
 Word fluency0.01 (1.01)0.11 (1.02)−0.09 (0.99)<.0010.06 (0.99)−0.08 (1.03).002
 Digit Symbol Substitution0.02 (0.97)0.16 (0.96)−0.10 (0.95)<.0010.05 (0.95)−0.02 (0.98).111
Stress measures
 Total perceived stress14.42 (8.61)14.32 (8.40)14.51 (8.80).61114.09 (8.46)14.90 (8.81).026
 Total acculturative stress12.88 (16.14)13.25 (16.44)12.55 (15.86).32112.64 (16.41)13.23 (15.72).391
 Chronic stress total2.03 (2.02)1.99 (1.96)2.07 (2.06).3871.95 (2.00)2.16 (2.03).018
 Traumatic stress
  Lifetime total2.15 (2.14)2.31 (2.19)2.00 (2.07).0132.22 (2.12)2.04 (2.14).100
  Past year0.14 (0.55)0.15 (0.59)0.13 (0.50).5370.13 (0.52)0.14 (0.58).742
MetS, % (SE)52.83 (1.36)44.50 (1.86)65.09 (1.96)<.001
 Abdominal obesity86.06 (0.84)80.47 (1.27)94.30 (0.86)<.001
 Hypertension58.38 (1.33)54.91 (1.83)63.49 (1.86).001
 High fasting glucose43.87 (1.53)40.63 (1.89)48.65 (2.08).002
 Low HDL-cholesterol38.95 (1.24)32.23 (1.62)48.83 (2.06)<.001
 High triglycerides37.07 (1.25)35.05 (1.65)40.05 (1.89).047
Inflammation
 hs-CRP (continuous)4.54 (9.37)3.79 (9.97)5.21 (8.72)<.0011.41 (0.94)12.72 (9.04)<.001
 hs-CRP, % (SE) high40.45 (1.38)29.93 (2.08)49.83 (1.68)<.001

Notes: B-SEVLT = Brief-Spanish English Verbal Learning Test; HDL-cholesterol = high-density lipoprotein cholesterol. Sum: sum of three learning trials. p Values are derived from chi-squared tests for categorical variables and t tests for continuous variables.

aValues represent means (SDs) unless otherwise noted. bNormal: ≤3 mg/L and high: >3 mg/L.

Results

Characteristics of the Study Sample

Table 1 shows descriptive characteristics of the study sample overall and by MetS and hs-CRP status. Age ranged from 45 to 74 years, and years of education from 0 to 20+. A little over half of the target population was female and most (86%) completed testing in Spanish. Regarding Hispanic/Latino background, over half of the target population was of Caribbean heritage (i.e., Cuban, Puerto Rican or Dominican), a third of Mexican heritage, and the remaining were of other or more than one heritage. Individuals meeting MetS criteria were older and less educated than those who did not meet criteria. Women were more likely than men to have elevated hs-CRP, and there were differences in hs-CRP based on Hispanic/Latino background. Global neurocognition was lower among participants with MetS and elevated hs-CRP. Perceived and chronic stress were higher among those with elevated hs-CRP, and lifetime traumatic stress was lower in those meeting MetS criteria, with no other significant associations of the stress measures with MetS and hs-CRP. Over half of the sample met criteria for MetS, with over three quarters of participants meeting criteria for abdominal obesity, and approximately 40% showed elevated hs-CRP. All MetS components were significantly associated with elevated hs-CRP. Descriptive characteristics comparing the target population (HCHS/SOL sample age 45+) with SCAS participation status are provided in Supplementary Table 1. Demographic characteristics, neurocognitive scores, and metabolic and inflammatory markers of participants in the HCHS/SOL parent study were not substantially different from those in the SCAS, except that participants in SCAS were significantly less likely to have high fasting glucose than participants in the parent study. Comparisons between the overall HCHS/SOL and SCAS samples have been reported previously (Gallo, Penedo, et al., 2014).

Associations of Stress Measures and Global Neurocognition

Separate analyses by stress measures (Table 2) showed that increased perceived stress and acculturative stress were each significantly associated with worse global neurocognition, and higher lifetime traumatic stress exposure was associated with better global neurocognition. The size of the effects of these associations was fairly small. Based on the standardized beta coefficients (obtained by multiplying the reported coefficients in Table 2 [M1] by the standard deviation of the stress measures of interest presented in Table 1), for every 1 SD increase in perceived stress and acculturative stress, global cognition decreased by 0.04 and 0.08 SDs, respectively, while for every 1 SD increase in lifetime traumatic stress, global cognition increased by 0.08 SD. These associations were statistically significant in both the model adjusting for demographics only (M1) and models adjusted also for MetS and/or hs-CRP (M1 + MetS, M1 + hs-CRP, M1 + MetS + hs-CRP), with both MetS and hs-CRP being significantly and independently associated with global neurocognition in these latter models. Traumatic stress over the past year and chronic stress were not significantly associated with global neurocognition, and thus were not considered further in analyses on global neurocognition.

Table 2.

Associations Between the Primary Stress Measures and Global Neurocognition, Considering Metabolic Syndrome (MetS) and High-Sensitivity C-Reactive Protein (hs-CRP) in These Associations, Among Participants in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos

Global neurocognition (B [SE])
M1M1 + MetSM1 + hs-CRPM1 + MetS + hs-CRP
Perceived stress−0.005* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)
 MetS (yes)−0.098*** (0.028)−0.088** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.070** (0.026)−0.053* (0.025)
Acculturative stress (total)−0.005*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)
 MetS (yes)−0.095*** (0.028)−0.086** (0.027)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.068** (0.025)−0.051* (0.024)
Chronic stress (total)0.000 (0.008)0.002 (0.008)0.001 (0.008)0.002 (0.008)
 MetS (yes)−0.103*** (0.028)−0.094*** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.068** (0.026)−0.050 (0.025)
Traumatic stress (lifetime)0.036*** (0.009)0.035*** (0.009)0.036*** (0.009)0.034*** (0.009)
 MetS (yes)−0.092*** (0.027)−0.082** (0.027)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.070** (0.024)−0.054* (0.024)
Traumatic stress (past year)0.033 (0.035)0.034 (0.036)0.036 (0.035)0.036 (0.036)
 MetS (yes)−0.101*** (0.028)−0.091** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.072** (0.025)−0.054* (0.025)
Global neurocognition (B [SE])
M1M1 + MetSM1 + hs-CRPM1 + MetS + hs-CRP
Perceived stress−0.005* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)
 MetS (yes)−0.098*** (0.028)−0.088** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.070** (0.026)−0.053* (0.025)
Acculturative stress (total)−0.005*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)
 MetS (yes)−0.095*** (0.028)−0.086** (0.027)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.068** (0.025)−0.051* (0.024)
Chronic stress (total)0.000 (0.008)0.002 (0.008)0.001 (0.008)0.002 (0.008)
 MetS (yes)−0.103*** (0.028)−0.094*** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.068** (0.026)−0.050 (0.025)
Traumatic stress (lifetime)0.036*** (0.009)0.035*** (0.009)0.036*** (0.009)0.034*** (0.009)
 MetS (yes)−0.092*** (0.027)−0.082** (0.027)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.070** (0.024)−0.054* (0.024)
Traumatic stress (past year)0.033 (0.035)0.034 (0.036)0.036 (0.035)0.036 (0.036)
 MetS (yes)−0.101*** (0.028)−0.091** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.072** (0.025)−0.054* (0.025)

Notes: Model 1 (M1): age, sex, education, and Hispanic/Latino background, language preference (Spanish/English), and place of birth; M1 + MetS: variables in M1 and MetS (reference: no); M1 + hs-CRP: variables in M1 and hs-CRP (reference: low); and M1 + MetS + hs-CRP: variables in M1, MetS, and hs-CRP.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. *

Table 2.

Associations Between the Primary Stress Measures and Global Neurocognition, Considering Metabolic Syndrome (MetS) and High-Sensitivity C-Reactive Protein (hs-CRP) in These Associations, Among Participants in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos

Global neurocognition (B [SE])
M1M1 + MetSM1 + hs-CRPM1 + MetS + hs-CRP
Perceived stress−0.005* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)
 MetS (yes)−0.098*** (0.028)−0.088** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.070** (0.026)−0.053* (0.025)
Acculturative stress (total)−0.005*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)
 MetS (yes)−0.095*** (0.028)−0.086** (0.027)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.068** (0.025)−0.051* (0.024)
Chronic stress (total)0.000 (0.008)0.002 (0.008)0.001 (0.008)0.002 (0.008)
 MetS (yes)−0.103*** (0.028)−0.094*** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.068** (0.026)−0.050 (0.025)
Traumatic stress (lifetime)0.036*** (0.009)0.035*** (0.009)0.036*** (0.009)0.034*** (0.009)
 MetS (yes)−0.092*** (0.027)−0.082** (0.027)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.070** (0.024)−0.054* (0.024)
Traumatic stress (past year)0.033 (0.035)0.034 (0.036)0.036 (0.035)0.036 (0.036)
 MetS (yes)−0.101*** (0.028)−0.091** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.072** (0.025)−0.054* (0.025)
Global neurocognition (B [SE])
M1M1 + MetSM1 + hs-CRPM1 + MetS + hs-CRP
Perceived stress−0.005* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)−0.004* (0.002)
 MetS (yes)−0.098*** (0.028)−0.088** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.070** (0.026)−0.053* (0.025)
Acculturative stress (total)−0.005*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)−0.004*** (0.001)
 MetS (yes)−0.095*** (0.028)−0.086** (0.027)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.068** (0.025)−0.051* (0.024)
Chronic stress (total)0.000 (0.008)0.002 (0.008)0.001 (0.008)0.002 (0.008)
 MetS (yes)−0.103*** (0.028)−0.094*** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.068** (0.026)−0.050 (0.025)
Traumatic stress (lifetime)0.036*** (0.009)0.035*** (0.009)0.036*** (0.009)0.034*** (0.009)
 MetS (yes)−0.092*** (0.027)−0.082** (0.027)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.070** (0.024)−0.054* (0.024)
Traumatic stress (past year)0.033 (0.035)0.034 (0.036)0.036 (0.035)0.036 (0.036)
 MetS (yes)−0.101*** (0.028)−0.091** (0.028)
 hs-CRP (high)−0.072** (0.025)−0.054* (0.025)

Notes: Model 1 (M1): age, sex, education, and Hispanic/Latino background, language preference (Spanish/English), and place of birth; M1 + MetS: variables in M1 and MetS (reference: no); M1 + hs-CRP: variables in M1 and hs-CRP (reference: low); and M1 + MetS + hs-CRP: variables in M1, MetS, and hs-CRP.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. *

Sensitivity analyses (Supplementary Table 2), adjusting for study site, depression, and anxiety (in addition to other previously considered demographics), yielded largely comparable findings, except perceived stress was no longer significantly associated with worse neurocognition. A series of models (Supplementary Table 3A and B) adjusting for individual MetS components separately (waist circumference, high blood pressure, fasting glucose, triglycerides, HDL-cholesterol) and pulse pressure, in addition to demographics (age, education, sex, Hispanic heritage, place of birth and language preference) showed comparable findings to similar models adjusting for the MetS composite (yes/no; Table 2, M1 + MetS).

Association of Stress Measures and Individual Neurocognitive Measures

Findings from models on individual neurocognitive measures (Supplementary Table 4) showed that higher perceived stress was associated with worse learning and memory, and higher acculturative stress was associated with worse neurocognition across domains. In contrast, higher chronic stress was significantly associated with better word fluency, and higher lifetime traumatic stress was associated with better word fluency and processing speed (assessed by the Digit Symbol Substitution test). There were no significant associations with exposure to traumatic stressors over the past year. Estimates of the effects of each stress measure on individual neurocognitive measures were comparable in models adjusted for demographics only and those adjusted for MetS and hs-CRP (in addition to demographics).

Indirect Effects of Stress Measures on Global Neurocognition via MetS and hs-CRP

Results from fully adjusted SEM analyses investigating the role of hs-CRP (Table 3) in explaining the negative association of perceived stress with global neurocognition (and adjusting for demographics) showed no substantive evidence of indirect effects for perceived stress via hs-CRP. While there was a statistically significant indirect effect, the size of this effect was very small (indirect effect: b < 0.000, SE < 0.000). Because acculturative stress was not significantly associated with MetS or hs-CRP (Table 1), comparable SEM analyses were not performed on this stress measure despite its significant association with worse global neurocognition (Table 2).

Table 3.

Model Examining the Association Between Perceived Stress and Global Neurocognition (z Score Units), Including Estimates of the Indirect Effects Testing Whether High-Sensitivity C-Reactive Protein (hs-CRP) Might Account for This Association in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos

Perceived stress
B (SE)p value
Global neurocognition
 hs-CRP−0.077 (0.025).003
 Stress indicator−0.004 (0.002).032
hs-CRP
 Stress indicator0.017 (0.007).018
Intercepts
 Global neurocognition0.152 (0.124).22
OR scale
OR (SE)p value
hs-CRP
 Stress indicator1.017 (0.007).019
B (SE)p value
 Indirect effect<0.000 (<0.000).033
 Direct effect−0.004 (0.002).032
 Total effect−0.005 (0.002).023
Perceived stress
B (SE)p value
Global neurocognition
 hs-CRP−0.077 (0.025).003
 Stress indicator−0.004 (0.002).032
hs-CRP
 Stress indicator0.017 (0.007).018
Intercepts
 Global neurocognition0.152 (0.124).22
OR scale
OR (SE)p value
hs-CRP
 Stress indicator1.017 (0.007).019
B (SE)p value
 Indirect effect<0.000 (<0.000).033
 Direct effect−0.004 (0.002).032
 Total effect−0.005 (0.002).023

Notes: OR = odds ratio. Model is adjusted for age, sex, education in years, Hispanic/Latino background, language preference (Spanish/English), and place of birth.

Table 3.

Model Examining the Association Between Perceived Stress and Global Neurocognition (z Score Units), Including Estimates of the Indirect Effects Testing Whether High-Sensitivity C-Reactive Protein (hs-CRP) Might Account for This Association in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos

Perceived stress
B (SE)p value
Global neurocognition
 hs-CRP−0.077 (0.025).003
 Stress indicator−0.004 (0.002).032
hs-CRP
 Stress indicator0.017 (0.007).018
Intercepts
 Global neurocognition0.152 (0.124).22
OR scale
OR (SE)p value
hs-CRP
 Stress indicator1.017 (0.007).019
B (SE)p value
 Indirect effect<0.000 (<0.000).033
 Direct effect−0.004 (0.002).032
 Total effect−0.005 (0.002).023
Perceived stress
B (SE)p value
Global neurocognition
 hs-CRP−0.077 (0.025).003
 Stress indicator−0.004 (0.002).032
hs-CRP
 Stress indicator0.017 (0.007).018
Intercepts
 Global neurocognition0.152 (0.124).22
OR scale
OR (SE)p value
hs-CRP
 Stress indicator1.017 (0.007).019
B (SE)p value
 Indirect effect<0.000 (<0.000).033
 Direct effect−0.004 (0.002).032
 Total effect−0.005 (0.002).023

Notes: OR = odds ratio. Model is adjusted for age, sex, education in years, Hispanic/Latino background, language preference (Spanish/English), and place of birth.

Interactions of Stress Measures With MetS and hs-CRP on Neurocognition

Findings (Figure 1a–c) from analyses investigating whether the presence of MetS modified the association between stress and global neurocognition, adjusting for demographics (age, sex, education, background, place of birth, and language preference), were not significant. Similar models investigating interactions with hs-CRP (Figure 1d and e) showed a significant interaction with acculturative stress (p = .01), such that its association with global neurocognition was stronger among those with normal hs-CRP (B = −0.006, SE = 0.001, p < .01) than those with high hs-CRP (B = −0.002, SE = 0.001, p = .28). Follow-up comparable analyses on individual neurocognitive measures (Supplementary Figure 1) with terms for the hs-CRP and acculturative stress interaction showed a significant interaction on learning (p = .04), indicating that the association between increased acculturative stress and worse learning was stronger among those with normal hs-CRP (B = −0.006, SE = 0.002, p < .01) than those with high hs-CRP (B = −0.001, SE = 0.002, p = .44).

Results from survey linear models examining modifications in associations between stress and global neurocognition by metabolic syndrome (MetS: yes/no) and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (hs-CRP: high/normal), adjusting for age, sex, education, background, language, and place of birth. Results from interaction terms for each of the models were as follows: (a) MetS × Perceived stress: B = 0.001, SE = 0.004, p = .86. (b) MetS × Acculturative stress: B = 0.003, SE = 0.002, p = .15. (c) MetS × Lifetime traumatic stress: B = 0.018, SE = 0.016, p = .26. (d) hs-CRP × Perceived stress: B = −0.001, SE = 0.004, p = .79. (e) hs-CRP × Acculturative stress: B = 0.005, SE = 0.002, p =.01. (f) hs-CRP × Lifetime traumatic stress: B = −0.001, SE = 0.016, p =.95.
Figure 1.

Results from survey linear models examining modifications in associations between stress and global neurocognition by metabolic syndrome (MetS: yes/no) and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (hs-CRP: high/normal), adjusting for age, sex, education, background, language, and place of birth. Results from interaction terms for each of the models were as follows: (a) MetS × Perceived stress: B = 0.001, SE = 0.004, p = .86. (b) MetS × Acculturative stress: B = 0.003, SE = 0.002, p = .15. (c) MetS × Lifetime traumatic stress: B = 0.018, SE = 0.016, p = .26. (d) hs-CRP × Perceived stress: B = −0.001, SE = 0.004, p = .79. (e) hs-CRP × Acculturative stress: B = 0.005, SE = 0.002, p =.01. (f) hs-CRP × Lifetime traumatic stress: B = −0.001, SE = 0.016, p =.95.

Interactions of Stress Measures With Age and Sex on Neurocognition

Results (Figure 2) from analyses investigating modifications in the associations between stress and global neurocognition by age (45–64 vs 65–74) and sex, adjusting for age (in sex modification), sex (in age modification), education, background, place of birth, language preference, and MetS and hs-CRP, showed no evidence for modifications (nonsignificant interactions). Similarly, interaction tests for individual neurocognitive measures showed no consistent evidence for modifications between the stress measures and neurocognition by age or sex (results not shown).

Results from survey linear models examining modifications in associations between stress and global neurocognition by age (45–64 vs 65–74) and sex, adjusting for age (in sex modifications), sex (in age modification), education, background, language, place of birth, and metabolic syndrome and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein Results from interaction terms for each of the models were as follows: (a) Age × Perceived stress: B = 0.01, SE = 0.01, p =.24. (b) Age × Acculturation stress: B = −0.001, SE = 0.00, p = .75. (c) Age × Lifetime traumatic stress: B = 0.01, SE = 0.03, p = .68. (d) Sex × Perceived stress: B = −0.00, SE = 0.01, p = .47. (e) Sex × Acculturation stress: B = 0.00, SE = 0.00, p =.88. (f) Sex × Lifetime traumatic stress: B = 0.02, SE = 0.02, p =.36.
Figure 2.

Results from survey linear models examining modifications in associations between stress and global neurocognition by age (45–64 vs 65–74) and sex, adjusting for age (in sex modifications), sex (in age modification), education, background, language, place of birth, and metabolic syndrome and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein Results from interaction terms for each of the models were as follows: (a) Age × Perceived stress: B = 0.01, SE = 0.01, p =.24. (b) Age × Acculturation stress: B = −0.001, SE = 0.00, p = .75. (c) Age × Lifetime traumatic stress: B = 0.01, SE = 0.03, p = .68. (d) Sex × Perceived stress: B = −0.00, SE = 0.01, p = .47. (e) Sex × Acculturation stress: B = 0.00, SE = 0.00, p =.88. (f) Sex × Lifetime traumatic stress: B = 0.02, SE = 0.02, p =.36.

Discussion

Prior studies have linked increased stress with worse neurocognitive outcomes, including increased risk for ADRDs (Bisht et al., 2018; Justice, 2018; Machado et al., 2014). The biological pathways underlying this association are likely varied, with CVD risk and systemic inflammation being two of the proposed mechanisms. The present study adds to the current literature by showing significant associations between several stress measures and neurocognition among Hispanics/Latinos living in the United States, and indicating that these associations do not differ significantly by sex or age in this group. Specifically, we found that increased perceived and acculturative stress were significantly associated with worse global neurocognition, while traumatic stress was associated with better global neurocognition. Inconsistent with our hypothesis, neither markers of CVD risk (MetS) or systemic inflammation (hs-CRP) were notable pathways in the association of stress and neurocognition. They were, however, both independently associated with worse neurocognition in models including the stress measures. Furthermore, the impact of stress on neurocognition was not significantly modified by MetS or hs-CRP, except that higher acculturative stress was associated with worse neurocognition among participants with normal hs-CRP, but not among those with elevated hs-CRP.

The present study builds on prior findings (Nguyen et al., 2012), including some based on the HCHS/SOL cohort (González et al., 2018; Muñoz et al., 2021), and shows that two measures of stress appraisal (acculturative and perceived stress) show a negative association with global neurocognition, that is independent of the effect of MetS and inflammation on neurocognition. Analyses of findings within individual neurocognitive tests showed that acculturative stress was associated with all neurocognitive measures assessed. The association of perceived stress with global neurocognition, however, appeared to be primarily driven by the domains of learning and memory, underscoring the relevance of stress to neurocognitive domains typically impacted early on in neurodegenerative processes, such as Alzheimer’s disease. Of note, the effect sizes of these negative associations between measures of perceived and acculturative stress with neurocognition were fairly small. This might be explained by the presence of other related “protective” factors that might buffer the deleterious impact of stress on neurocognition in this sample of Hispanics/Latinos. Recent findings showed positive psychosocial factors (e.g., purpose in life, social support) were associated with better neurocognition in HCHS/SOL (Estrella et al., 2021), and underscore the need to examine potentially interactive effects of stress with “protective” psychosocial factors on neurocognition in future studies.

In contrast to findings on stress appraisal measures (i.e., perceived and acculturative stress), lifetime exposure to traumatic stress was linked to better global neurocognitive function, and exposure to ongoing chronic stressors showed no significant associations with global neurocognition. Both of these latter stress measures were significantly linked to better performance on tests of word fluency, with lifetime traumatic stress also being linked to faster psychomotor speed. The seemingly disparate findings across measures of stress in their association with neurocognition might be partly explained by the fact that while perceived and acculturative stress instruments assessed participants’ self-appraisals of their experience of stress, the chronic stress and traumatic stress measures capture events that are typically considered challenging (e.g., trauma). It has long been posited that appraisal of events plays an important role in the impact of such events on health outcomes (Somerfield & McCrae, 2000), and it is the felt experience that accompanies such neurocognitive processes that drives the deleterious effect of stress on health (Monroe, 2008). This conceptualization helps explain why measures of perceived and acculturative stress would be linked to worse neurocognition, while measures of report of challenging events might not. Yet, it does not fully explain why self-report of exposure to traumatic events would be linked to better neurocognition. Considering the cross-sectional nature of our findings, the possibility that better neurocognitive function might lead to better recall of life events should be considered. However, present results suggesting that individuals with greater traumatic stress exposure are also less likely to meet MetS criteria concur with those from a prior study in this cohort showing an association of traumatic stress with decreased CVD risk factors (Gallo, Roesch, et al., 2014). Thus, these counterintuitive effects do not appear to be specific to neurocognitive processes.

Considering our findings regarding traumatic stress in the context of posttraumatic growth theory (Linley & Joseph, 2004) might help shed some light into their interpretation. Posttraumatic growth theory posits that individuals might rise to a higher level of functioning through the process of struggling with adversity and refers to these “positive changes” as “adversarial growth.” While this construct was not assessed in the present study, future longitudinal studies incorporating assessments of adversarial growth might better discern whether the experience of traumatic life events results in positive behavioral changes in some individuals in this cohort, which might lead to better health. This interpretation is in line with observed resilience within the Hispanic/Latino population living in the United States, which has been posited to contribute to the Hispanic health paradox.

Prior findings in HCHS/SOL have linked stress with CVD/metabolic risk factors (Gallo, Roesch, et al., 2014), and MetS with worse neurocognition (González et al., 2018), and showed that there was not a significant interaction between MetS and hs-CRP on neurocognition (González et al., 2018). Inconsistent with our hypothesis and with prior findings (Vitaliano et al., 2005; Zahodne, Kraal, et al., 2019), we found that MetS and hs-CRP did not notably explain the link between increased stress and worse neurocognition in the HCHS/SOL population. Instead, stress appraisal (perceived and acculturative stress), MetS, and hs-CRP were all independently associated with worse neurocognition. If supported by longitudinal research, these results indicate that interventions targeting stress appraisal might have added benefits to neurocognition among Hispanics/Latinos over and above the effect of interventions targeting MetS and hs-CRP. This notion is further supported by findings indicating that the association between stress and neurocognition was largely comparable among participants with and without MetS and high and normal hs-CRP. The single exception was that acculturative stress was associated with worse global neurocognition (and learning) among participants with normal hs-CRP, but not those with high hs-CRP. While these results need to be interpreted with caution, they might indicate that in the presence of high systemic inflammation, the impact of acculturative stress on neurocognition is reduced, as high systemic inflammation overrides the biological mechanisms driving the association of acculturative stress with neurocognition.

Future studies examining other markers of inflammation and other posited mechanisms may further elucidate the underlying biological pathways linking stress and neurocognition among Hispanics/Latinos. Some of these mechanisms include the role of microglia (Bisht et al., 2018), multisystemic physiological dysregulation, as captured by the concept of allostatic load (Juster et al., 2010), activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, which increases production of glucocorticoids (Justice, 2018; Lupien et al., 2009), and dysregulated sleep (Lucey, 2020), as well as mechanisms by which advancing neurological disease disrupts neural and endocrine circuits that mediate the stress response (Justice, 2018). Present findings showing that the association of stress and neurocognition was not significantly different by age or sex indicate that interventions aimed at reducing stress appraisal are likely to be beneficial across age and for both men and women in middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos.

Limitations of the present study include its cross-sectional design, which restricts our ability to ascribe directionality to our findings. Collection of SCAS stress data occurred up to 9 months after collection of neurocognitive and biological data, possibly explaining the generally small effects of stress on neurocognition, though most participants’ data were collected within 4 months of the HCHS/SOL baseline. While our cohort was quite diverse in its Hispanic/Latino heritage, a large proportion of participants were born outside the United States and although we account for place of birth in our analyses, caution is warranted in generalizing to U.S.-born Hispanics/Latinos. Future studies might explore whether our findings differ based on sociodemographic characteristics that vary among Hispanics/Latinos. While the neurocognitive battery used in the present study assessed a number of neurocognitive domains, it did so with a limited number of tests, and does not include measures of other neurocognitive domains which are impacted by aging and ADRDs (e.g., visuospatial abilities, executive function), and may be incorporated in future studies. Our assessment of MetS included a widely used composite based on established criteria. Yet, other markers of CVD risk burden, such as the Framingham Cardiovascular Risk Score and Global Vascular Risk Score(Lamar et al., 2019; Tarraf et al., 2020), have been linked to neurocognition in HCHS/SOL and their consideration in future studies might shed further light on the role of CVD risk in the association of stress and neurocognition.

The estimated large numbers of Hispanics/Latinos reaching older age and projected to develop ADRDs underscores the dire need for research into mechanisms driving neurocognitive impairment in this population. The appraisal of stress (rather than exposure to challenging events), MetS, and systemic inflammation had additive (independent) associations with neurocognition in this diverse cohort of Hispanics/Latinos. This highlights the potential importance of considering them all as potential targets in the development of prevention and intervention efforts to ameliorate neurocognitive dysfunction among Hispanics/Latinos.

Funding

The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos is a collaborative study supported by contracts from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute to the University of North Carolina (HHSN268201300001I/ N01-HC-65233), University of Miami (HHSN268201300004I/N01-HC-65234), Albert Einstein College of Medicine (HHSN268201300002I/N01-HC-65235), University of Illinois at Chicago (HHSN268201300003I/N01-HC-65236 Northwestern University), and San Diego State University (HHSN268201300005I/N01-HC-65237). The following institutes/centers/offices have contributed to the HCHS/ SOL through a transfer of funds to the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute: National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities, National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research, National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, and National Institutes of Health (NIH) Office of Dietary Supplements. The HCHS/SOL Socio-Cultural Ancillary Study was supported by grant number RC2HL101649. This work was also supported by the following NIH grants: R01AG048642, RF1AG054548, P30AG059299, DK111022-8786, and P30AG062429.

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the staff and participants of Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos (HCHS/SOL) for their important contributions. The contents of this report are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. Investigators website: http://www.cscc.unc.edu/hchs/. Please contact the corresponding author for analytic methods and data used in this manuscript. The conducted research was not preregistered.

References

Andresen
,
E. M.
,
Malmgren
,
J. A.
,
Carter
,
W. B.
, &
Patrick
,
D. L
. (
1994
).
Screening for depression in well older adults: Evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale)
.
American Journal of Preventive Medicine
,
10
(
2
),
77
84
. doi:10.1016/S0749-3797(18)30622-6

Benton
,
A. L.
, &
Hamsher
,
K
. (
1989
).
Multilingual aphasia examination
(2nd ed.).
AJA Associates
.

Bisht
,
K.
,
Sharma
,
K.
, &
Tremblay
,
M. È
. (
2018
).
Chronic stress as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease: Roles of microglia-mediated synaptic remodeling, inflammation, and oxidative stress
.
Neurobiology of Stress
,
9
,
9
21
. doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2018.05.003

Bromberger
,
J. T.
, &
Matthews
,
K. A
. (
1996
).
A longitudinal study of the effects of pessimism, trait anxiety, and life stress on depressive symptoms in middle-aged women
.
Psychology and Aging
,
11
(
2
),
207
213
. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.11.2.207

Cervantes
,
R. C.
,
Padilla
,
A. M.
, &
De Snyder
,
N. S
. (
1990
).
Reliability and validity of the Hispanic Stress Inventory
.
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences
,
12
(
1
),
76
82
. doi:10.1177/07399863900121004

Cohen
,
S.
,
Kamarck
,
T.
, &
Mermelstein
,
R
. (
1983
).
A global measure of perceived stress
.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior
,
24
(
4
),
385
396
. doi:10.2307/2136404

Dimsdale
,
J. E
. (
2008
).
Psychological stress and cardiovascular disease
.
Journal of the American College of Cardiology
,
51
(
13
),
1237
1246
. doi:10.1016/j.jacc.2007.12.024

Estrella
,
M. L.
,
Durazo-Arvizu
,
R. A.
,
Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Tarraf
,
W.
,
Isasi
,
C. R.
,
Perreira
,
K. M.
,
Zeng
,
D.
,
Marquine
,
M. J.
,
Lipton
,
R. B.
,
González
,
H. M.
,
Daviglus
,
M. L.
, &
Lamar
,
M
. (
2021
).
Psychosocial factors associated with cognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos: The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos and its Sociocultural Ancillary Study
.
Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease
,
79
(
1
),
433
449
. doi:10.3233/JAD-200612

Farrer
,
L. A.
,
Cupples
,
L. A.
,
Haines
,
J. L.
,
Hyman
,
B.
,
Kukull
,
W. A.
,
Mayeux
,
R.
,
Myers
,
R. H.
,
Pericak-Vance
,
M. A.
,
Risch
,
N.
, &
van Duijn
,
C. M
. (
1997
).
Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium
.
Journal of the American Medical Association
,
278
(
16
),
1349
1356
. doi:10.1001/jama.1997.03550160069041

Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Penedo
,
F. J.
,
Carnethon
,
M.
,
Isasi
,
C. R.
,
Sotres-Alvarez
,
D.
,
Malcarne
,
V. L.
,
Roesch
,
S. C.
,
Youngblood
,
M. E.
,
Daviglus
,
M. L.
,
Gonzalez
,
P.
, &
Talavera
,
G. T
. (
2014
).
The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos Sociocultural Ancillary Study: Sample, design, and procedures
.
Ethnicity & Disease
,
24
(
1
),
77
83
.

Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Roesch
,
S. C.
,
Fortmann
,
A. L.
,
Carnethon
,
M. R.
,
Penedo
,
F. J.
,
Perreira
,
K.
,
Birnbaum-Weitzman
,
O.
,
Wassertheil-Smoller
,
S.
,
Castañeda
,
S. F.
,
Talavera
,
G. A.
,
Sotres-Alvarez
,
D.
,
Daviglus
,
M. L.
,
Schneiderman
,
N.
, &
Isasi
,
C. R
. (
2014
).
Associations of chronic stress burden, perceived stress, and traumatic stress with cardiovascular disease prevalence and risk factors in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos Sociocultural Ancillary Study
.
Psychosomatic Medicine
,
76
(
6
),
468
475
. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000069

González
,
H. M.
,
Mungas
,
D.
, &
Haan
,
M. N
. (
2002
).
A verbal learning and memory test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-American adults
.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist
,
16
(
4
),
439
451
. doi:10.1076/clin.16.4.439.13908

González
,
H. M.
,
Tarraf
,
W.
,
Gouskova
,
N.
,
Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Penedo
,
F. J.
,
Davis
,
S. M.
,
Lipton
,
R. B.
,
Argüelles
,
W.
,
Choca
,
J. P.
,
Catellier
,
D. J.
, &
Mosley
,
T. H
. (
2015
).
Neurocognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanic/Latinos: Results from the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos
.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology
,
30
(
1
),
68
77
. doi:10.1093/arclin/acu066

González
,
H. M.
,
Tarraf
,
W.
,
Vásquez
,
P.
,
Sanderlin
,
A. H.
,
Rosenberg
,
N. I.
,
Davis
,
S.
,
Rodríguez
,
C. J.
,
Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Thyagarajan
,
B.
,
Daviglus
,
M.
,
Khambaty
,
T.
,
Cai
,
J.
, &
Schneiderman
,
N
. (
2018
).
Metabolic syndrome and neurocognition among diverse middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos: HCHS/SOL results
.
Diabetes Care
,
41
(
7
),
1501
1509
. doi:10.2337/dc17-1896

Gurland
,
B. J.
,
Wilder
,
D. E.
,
Lantigua
,
R.
,
Stern
,
Y.
,
Chen
,
J.
,
Killeffer
,
E. H.
, &
Mayeux
,
R
. (
1999
).
Rates of dementia in three ethnoracial groups
.
International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry
,
14
(
6
),
481
493
. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1166(199906)14:6<481::AID-GPS959>3.0.CO;2-5

Hänsel
,
A.
,
Hong
,
S.
,
Cámara
,
R. J.
, &
von Känel
,
R
. (
2010
).
Inflammation as a psychophysiological biomarker in chronic psychosocial stress
.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
,
35
(
1
),
115
121
. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.012

Hayes
,
A. F
. (
2017
).
Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach
.
Guilford Publications
.

Heiss
,
G.
,
Snyder
,
M. L.
,
Teng
,
Y.
,
Schneiderman
,
N.
,
Llabre
,
M. M.
,
Cowie
,
C.
,
Carnethon
,
M.
,
Kaplan
,
R.
,
Giachello
,
A.
,
Gallo
,
L.
,
Loehr
,
L.
, &
Avilés-Santa
,
L
. (
2014
).
Prevalence of metabolic syndrome among Hispanics/Latinos of diverse background: The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos
.
Diabetes Care
,
37
(
8
),
2391
2399
. doi:10.2337/dc13-2505

Heppner
,
F. L.
,
Ransohoff
,
R. M.
, &
Becher
,
B
. (
2015
).
Immune attack: The role of inflammation in Alzheimer disease
.
Nature Reviews. Neuroscience
,
16
(
6
),
358
372
. doi:10.1038/nrn3880

International Diabetes Federation.
(
2006
).
The IDF consensus worldwide definition of the metabolic syndrome
. https://www.idf.org/e-library/consensus-statements/60-idfconsensus-worldwide-definitionof-the-metabolic-syndrome.html

Isasi
,
C. R.
,
Parrinello
,
C. M.
,
Jung
,
M. M.
,
Carnethon
,
M. R.
,
Birnbaum-Weitzman
,
O.
,
Espinoza
,
R. A.
,
Penedo
,
F. J.
,
Perreira
,
K. M.
,
Schneiderman
,
N.
,
Sotres-Alvarez
,
D.
,
Van Horn
,
L.
, &
Gallo
,
L. C
. (
2015
).
Psychosocial stress is associated with obesity and diet quality in Hispanic/Latino adults
.
Annals of Epidemiology
,
25
(
2
),
84
89
. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2014.11.002

Juster
,
R. P.
,
McEwen
,
B. S.
, &
Lupien
,
S. J
. (
2010
).
Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition
.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
,
35
(
1
),
2
16
. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.002

Justice
,
N. J
. (
2018
).
The relationship between stress and Alzheimer’s disease
.
Neurobiology of Stress
,
8
,
127
133
. doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2018.04.002

Lamar
,
M.
,
Durazo-Arvizu
,
R. A.
,
Sachdeva
,
S.
,
Pirzada
,
A.
,
Perreira
,
K. M.
,
Rundek
,
T.
,
Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Grober
,
E.
,
DeCarli
,
C.
,
Lipton
,
R. B.
,
Tarraf
,
W.
,
González
,
H. M.
, &
Daviglus
,
M. L
. (
2019
).
Cardiovascular disease risk factor burden and cognition: Implications of ethnic diversity within the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos
.
PLoS One
,
14
(
4
),
e0215378
. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0215378

Linley
,
P. A.
, &
Joseph
,
S
. (
2004
).
Positive change following trauma and adversity: A review
.
Journal of Traumatic Stress
,
17
(
1
),
11
21
. doi:10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014671.27856.7e

Lucey
,
B. P
. (
2020
).
It’s complicated: The relationship between sleep and Alzheimer’s disease in humans
.
Neurobiology of Disease
,
144
,
105031
. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2020.105031

Lupien
,
S. J.
,
McEwen
,
B. S.
,
Gunnar
,
M. R.
, &
Heim
,
C
. (
2009
).
Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition
.
Nature Reviews. Neuroscience
,
10
(
6
),
434
445
. doi:10.1038/nrn2639

Machado
,
A.
,
Herrera
,
A. J.
,
de Pablos
,
R. M.
,
Espinosa-Oliva
,
A. M.
,
Sarmiento
,
M.
,
Ayala
,
A.
,
Venero
,
J. L.
,
Santiago
,
M.
,
Villarán
,
R. F.
,
Delgado-Cortés
,
M. J.
,
Argüelles
,
S.
, &
Cano
,
J
. (
2014
).
Chronic stress as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease
.
Reviews in the Neurosciences
,
25
(
6
),
785
804
. doi:10.1515/revneuro-2014-0035

McCurley
,
J. L.
,
Mills
,
P. J.
,
Roesch
,
S. C.
,
Carnethon
,
M.
,
Giacinto
,
R. E.
,
Isasi
,
C. R.
,
Teng
,
Y.
,
Sotres-Alvarez
,
D.
,
Llabre
,
M. M.
,
Penedo
,
F. J.
,
Schneiderman
,
N.
, &
Gallo
,
L. C
. (
2015
).
Chronic stress, inflammation, and glucose regulation in U.S. Hispanics from the HCHS/SOL Sociocultural Ancillary Study
.
Psychophysiology
,
52
(
8
),
1071
1079
. doi:10.1111/psyp.12430

Monroe
,
S. M
. (
2008
).
Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress
.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology
,
4
,
33
52
. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141207

Muñoz
,
E.
,
Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Hua
,
S.
,
Sliwinski
,
M. J.
,
Kaplan
,
R.
,
Lipton
,
R. B.
,
González
,
H. M.
,
Penedo
,
F. J.
,
Tarraf
,
W.
,
Daviglus
,
M. L.
,
Llabre
,
M. M.
, &
Isasi
,
C. R
. (
2021
).
Stress is associated with neurocognitive function in Hispanic/Latino adults: Results from HCHS/SOL Socio-Cultural Ancillary Study
.
The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences
,
76
(
4
),
e122
e128
. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbz144

Muthén
,
B. O.
,
Muthén
,
L. K.
, &
Asparouhov
,
T
. (
2017
).
Regression and mediation analysis using Mplus
.
Muthén & Muthén
.

Muthén
,
L. K.
, &
Muthén
,
B. O.
(
1998
–2017).
Mplus user’s guide
(8th ed.).
Muthén & Muthén
.

Nguyen
,
H. T.
,
Quandt
,
S. A.
,
Grzywacz
,
J. G.
,
Chen
,
H.
,
Galván
,
L.
,
Kitner-Triolo
,
M. H.
, &
Arcury
,
T. A
. (
2012
).
Stress and cognitive function in Latino farm workers
.
American Journal of Industrial Medicine
,
55
(
8
),
707
713
. doi:10.1002/ajim.22035

Norris
,
F. H
. (
1990
).
Screening for traumatic stress—A scale for use in the general-population
.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology
,
20
(
20
),
1704
1718
. doi:10.1111/J.1559-1816.1990.Tb01505.X

Pearson
,
T. A.
,
Mensah
,
G. A.
,
Alexander
,
R. W.
,
Anderson
,
J. L.
,
Cannon
,
R. O.
3rd
,
Criqui
,
M.
,
Fadl
,
Y. Y.
,
Fortmann
,
S. P.
,
Hong
,
Y.
,
Myers
,
G. L.
,
Rifai
,
N.
,
Smith
,
S. C.
Jr
,
Taubert
,
K.
,
Tracy
,
R. P.
, &
Vinicor
,
F.
;
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; American Heart Association.
(
2003
).
Markers of inflammation and cardiovascular disease: Application to clinical and public health practice: A statement for healthcare professionals from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American Heart Association
.
Circulation
,
107
(
3
),
499
511
. doi:10.1161/01.cir.0000052939.59093.45

Shivpuri
,
S.
,
Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Crouse
,
J. R.
, &
Allison
,
M. A
. (
2012
).
The association between chronic stress type and C-reactive protein in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis: Does gender make a difference?
Journal of Behavioral Medicine
,
35
(
1
),
74
85
. doi:10.1007/s10865-011-9345-5

Siervo
,
M.
,
Harrison
,
S. L.
,
Jagger
,
C.
,
Robinson
,
L.
, &
Stephan
,
B. C
. (
2014
).
Metabolic syndrome and longitudinal changes in cognitive function: A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease
,
41
(
1
),
151
161
. doi:10.3233/JAD-132279

Somerfield
,
M. R.
, &
McCrae
,
R. R
. (
2000
).
Stress and coping research. Methodological challenges, theoretical advances, and clinical applications
.
The American Psychologist
,
55
(
6
),
620
625
.

Sorlie
,
P. D.
,
Avilés-Santa
,
L. M.
,
Wassertheil-Smoller
,
S.
,
Kaplan
,
R. C.
,
Daviglus
,
M. L.
,
Giachello
,
A. L.
,
Schneiderman
,
N.
,
Raij
,
L.
,
Talavera
,
G.
,
Allison
,
M.
,
Lavange
,
L.
,
Chambless
,
L. E.
, &
Heiss
,
G
. (
2010
).
Design and implementation of the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos
.
Annals of Epidemiology
,
20
(
8
),
629
641
. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.03.015

Spielberger
,
C. D.
,
Gorsuch
,
R. L.
,
Lushene
,
R.
,
Vagg
,
P. R.
, &
Jacobs
,
G. A
. (
1983
).
Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory
.
Consulting Psychologists Press
.

Steptoe
,
A.
, &
Kivimäki
,
M
. (
2013
).
Stress and cardiovascular disease: An update on current knowledge
.
Annual Review of Public Health
,
34
,
337
354
. doi:10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031912-114452

Tang
,
M. X.
,
Cross
,
P.
,
Andrews
,
H.
,
Jacobs
,
D. M.
,
Small
,
S.
,
Bell
,
K.
,
Merchant
,
C.
,
Lantigua
,
R.
,
Costa
,
R.
,
Stern
,
Y.
, &
Mayeux
,
R
. (
2001
).
Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan
.
Neurology
,
56
(
1
),
49
56
. doi:10.1212/wnl.56.1.49

Tarraf
,
W.
,
Kaplan
,
R.
,
Daviglus
,
M.
,
Gallo
,
L. C.
,
Schneiderman
,
N.
,
Penedo
,
F. J.
,
Perreira
,
K. M.
,
Lamar
,
M.
,
Chai
,
A.
,
Vásquez
,
P. M.
, &
González
,
H. M
. (
2020
).
Cardiovascular risk and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos: Results from the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos (HCHS/SOL)
.
Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease
,
73
(
1
),
103
116
. doi:10.3233/JAD-190830

USC Edward R. Roybal Institute on Aging and the LatinosAgainstAlzheimer’s Network.
(
2016
).
Latinos and Alzheimer’s disease: New numbers behind the crisis.
https://www.usagainstalzheimers.org/sites/default/files/Latinos-and-AD_USC_UsA2-Impact-Report.pdf

Vitaliano
,
P. P.
,
Echeverria
,
D.
,
Yi
,
J.
,
Phillips
,
P. E.
,
Young
,
H.
, &
Siegler
,
I. C
. (
2005
).
Psychophysiological mediators of caregiver stress and differential cognitive decline
.
Psychology and Aging
,
20
(
3
),
402
411
. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.402

Wechsler
,
D
. (
1981
).
WAIS-R manual
.
Psychological Corporation
.

Yaffe
,
K.
,
Haan
,
M.
,
Blackwell
,
T.
,
Cherkasova
,
E.
,
Whitmer
,
R. A.
, &
West
,
N
. (
2007
).
Metabolic syndrome and cognitive decline in elderly Latinos: Findings from the Sacramento Area Latino Study of Aging study
.
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society
,
55
(
5
),
758
762
. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01139.x

Zahodne
,
L. B.
,
Kraal
,
A. Z.
,
Sharifian
,
N.
,
Zaheed
,
A. B.
, &
Sol
,
K
. (
2019
).
Inflammatory mechanisms underlying the effects of everyday discrimination on age-related memory decline
.
Brain, Behavior, and Immunity
,
75
,
149
154
. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2018.10.002

Zahodne
,
L. B.
,
Sol
,
K.
, &
Kraal
,
Z
. (
2019
).
Psychosocial pathways to racial/ethnic inequalities in late-life memory trajectories
.
The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences
,
74
(
3
),
409
418
. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbx113

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)
Decision Editor: Alyssa Gamaldo, PhD
Alyssa Gamaldo, PhD
Decision Editor
Search for other works by this author on: