Abstract

Chronic nonspecific low back pain (LBP) is a complex and multifaceted problem. The following Perspective piece tries to help make sense of this complexity by describing a model for the development and maintenance of persistent LBP that integrates modifiable factors across the biopsychosocial spectrum. The Fit-for-Purpose model posits the view that chronic nonspecific LBP represents a state in which the person in pain holds strong and relatively intransient internal models of an immutably damaged, fragile, and unhealthy back, and information that supports these models is more available and trustworthy than information that counters them. This Perspective proposes a corresponding treatment framework for persistent pain that aims to shift internal models of a fragile, damaged, unhealthy, and unchangeable self toward the formulation of the back as healthy, strong, adaptable, and fit for purpose and to provide the system with precise and trustworthy evidence that supports this supposition while minimizing information that works against it.

Introduction

Chronic nonspecific low back pain (CNSLBP) is a common and complex problem with considerable personal and societal consequences.1–3 Prognosis is unfavorable, with recovery occurring in less than one-half of people with CNSLBP over a 1-year period.4 Despite a vast range of potential treatment approaches,5 an optimal form of care has not yet been identified. A large number of cross-sectional studies have detected countless potential targets for management across the biopsychosocial spectrum,2 and it is commonly suggested that effective care needs to target multiple drivers to the clinical condition.6 Combining interventions from different specialties into a multimodal treatment plan has been shown to be more effective than single therapies or usual care,7 and multimodal care is an approach endorsed by clinical practice guidelines,8 though improvements in outcome remain modest. Optimization of multimodal care and further improvements in treatment outcomes may come from a richer understanding of the interaction between modifiable contributing factors that exist across the biopsychosocial spectrum and how these issues coalesce to shape the chronic pain experience and trajectory.

Here we present the Fit-for-Purpose Model (FFPM),9 which shares some characteristics with other approaches to understanding and managing CNSLBP but extends them by integrating cognitive and behavioral factors with modifiable neuroimmune processes. Similar to the Fear-Avoidance,10,11 Health Belief,12 and Common Sense13 models, the FFPM recognizes the importance of interactions between beliefs and behaviors in persistent pain. Also similar to those models, the FFPM has not suddenly come into being at a “single, defined moment” (eg, Leventhal et al.13,p937) but has undergone iterative development as new discoveries have been made and new interventions tested. What sets the FFPM apart from other models is the integration of and weighting given to foundational cognitions about “how pain and healing work”; the impact on information processing, cognitions, and clinical symptoms of a plastic and adaptive neuroimmune system14–16; and incorporation of contemporary models of learning, not least predictive processing,17–19 into explaining the persistence of pain and disability. Finally, we have developed a coherent, staged, complex treatment approach aimed at secondary and tertiary prevention for people with CNSLBP based on our model, which is currently being tested in a clinical trial.20 This Perspective Piece describes the theoretical underpinnings of the FFPM and the rationale behind each component of the complex care package. Ultimately, our aim is to provide a reasoning-based framework that will help clinicians make sense of the complexity of CNSLBP and thus provide contemporary, coherent, biopsychosocial informed management.

Theoretical Underpinnings

Noxious input from the body is likely an important part of many peoples’ pain experience, but pain does not reflect a simple readout of nociceptor activity.21 Current theory suggests that pain might best be considered an actively constructed experience based on multiple sources of information and reflects both conscious and non-conscious assessment that one’s body is under threat and in need of protection.19,22

This standpoint is consistent with current concepts of perception more generally. Where once the brain was viewed as a passive processor of sensory information, it is now considered a dynamic organ of inference that makes sense of the world by actively generating hypotheses regarding the sensory inputs the individual encounters and then tests these hypotheses against sensory evidence.23 What is perceived is the system’s final estimate of the most likely cause of sensory input derived from both prior beliefs about the body and the world and current sensory information from the body and the world.17 With regard to pain, the individual builds models of the body based on prior information from and about the body and uses these models to predict the causes of ongoing sensory information from the body, contingent on the context in which the individual is situated. Under this paradigm, pain then represents the conscious expression of a final estimate, from the integration of these information sources, that the sensory stream represents an abnormal somatic event in which there is a threat to bodily integrity and protective behavior would mitigate that threat.18,19 In this way psychological and social factors are stitched into the fundamental neurobiological processes that underpin the emergence of a conscious experience we recognize as pain.

The FFPM leans heavily on the notion of pain emerging as the result of a dynamic interplay between stored information and information transformed by, and encoded within, sensory processing systems. Particularly, the FFPM posits the view that CNSLBP represents a state in which the individual holds relatively intransient internal models of an immutably damaged, fragile, and unhealthy back and information from the body (and the world) that supports these models is more available and trustworthy than information that counters them. The following section explores how this self-reinforcing state could arise and lead to maintenance of an ongoing pain state.

Development of CNSLBP

A Cognitive Model of an Immutably Damaged, Fragile, and Unhealthy Self Begins to Emerge

Every persistent pain state has an onset, and there are characteristics of the experiences of the individual with acute low back pain (LBP) that are particularly relevant to the FFPM. Most important are those factors that start to shape beliefs that the back is damaged, fragile, and unhealthy and that this state is resistant to change. At a societal level, it seems that LBP is viewed primarily as a result of injury to somatic structures within the back,24,25 generally as a response to mechanical overload.26 We contend, alongside others, that much of the societal messaging about back care, particularly in the work place, reinforces ideas that “backs are fragile and easily injured”24 and that back pain is a particularly intransient problem.27 This has implications for the individual trying to make sense of their problem based on their own internal resources but also shapes the information they would obtain from their family, their social networks, and their world more broadly. That acute LBP is commonly characterized by movement-evoked pain and often eased by rest and inactivity makes this model of pain as a marker of damage intuitively sensible as well. So both the lived experience of the person in pain and their social and informational environment contribute to the formulation of a damaged, unhealthy, and load-intolerant self.

Seeking professional help for the problem may also enhance this narrative. A number of diagnostic models for spinal pain reference tissue injury and damage as primary contributors to the pain state28,29 and reinforce this through the provision of treatment strategies that promote unloading and volitional protection of the spine,30–32 including messaging that protecting “your fragile back” may be required long term.33 Furthermore, though not recommended,34,35 imaging of the spine is still common for people receiving care for acute nonspecific LBP.36 The high rate of positive imaging findings in the general population37 makes it highly likely that people exposed to spinal imaging will be provided with information that appears consistent with stored impressions of damage or structural insufficiency. Moreover, explanatory models for symptoms that reference imaging findings not only reinforce damage but they also augment this impression with implied irreversibility and a pathway of progressive deterioration.38

Specific clinical characteristics are also likely to drive a strong formulation of an unalterable, unhealthy self. There has been considerable research into identifying factors associated with the transition to chronicity in those with recent-onset LBP.39–42 A number of clinical characteristics consistently identified by this research effort are likely to reinforce beliefs that the back is damaged, fragile, and unhealthy. Known indicators of poor prognosis such as high pain intensity,43–45 symptom persistence and/or recurrence,46,47 and the presence of other health issues39,48–50 potentially impede recovery as they serve as reinforcers of the notion that the individual is significantly injured and unhealthy. Psychological factors that hinder recovery such as negative affect,39,41,51–53 pessimism about future outcome,43,54 and health-related anxiety51,55 are also plausible reinforcers of this account, particularly with reference to capacity for reversibility and positive change.

The majority of people experiencing an episode of acute LBP recover reasonably quickly.4 The FFPM contends that those who do not are those in whom social, psychological, experiential, and clinical factors shape particularly strong meta-cognitive and cognitive models of the back as being immutably damaged, fragile, and unhealthy. For these people, the process of trying to make sense of their LBP experience early in an episode leads to an understanding of the problem in which the back is appraised as being fragile and not fit for purpose and viewed as being under threat and in need of protection.

Initial Responses to the View That the Back Is Not Fit for Purpose and in Need of Protection

The model we are proposing brings 3 learning-related mechanisms into play. First, from a predictive processing perspective, as the back is moved and loaded multiple streams of sensory information from the back are generated, and, even in normal circumstances, this likely includes nociceptive input.56 Information that is expected, based on predictive models of the response of the back to moving and loading, is given more weighting and is therefore more likely to contribute to perception.57 In the pain-expectant individual, nociceptive input will be particularly weighted and will therefore have more influence on determining if the sensory stream accompanying movement is perceived as representing a harmful, abnormal somatic event. Critically, this increased weighting of nociceptive input is potentially accompanied by decreased weighting of non-nociceptive somatosensory information from the back, the various proprioceptive streams that are associated with back movement, and loading. Second, at a meta-cognitive level, viewing the back as easily injured and pain as a marker of tissue damage will impact the various complex and intertwined processes that are involved in making sense of what is perceived. In the individual who believes the back is not fit for purpose, pain with action is not only more likely, but pain is potentially interpreted in a more catastrophic way. The abnormal somatic event that pain signifies is not seen as minor or transient but taken to represent significant harm or damage to the body11,58 and an experience more likely to foster distress and disablement. Third, as implicated in the fear avoidance model, concerns about the decreased capacity of the back to cope safely with movement and loading, and viewing pain as a marker of tissue damage, may drive avoidance of some tasks11 and contribute to changing the way the spine is moved and controlled during other tasks, particularly the promotion of control strategies that increase rigidity59–62 and decrease movement variability.63–66 Actions that promote poverty of movement and inflexibility of the movement repertoire potentially change how the back is represented cortically67 and the information available from the back with movement and loading.

These Responses Induce Functional Changes That Drive Perceptual Models of a Damaged Fragile and Unhealthy Self

Ongoing changes in action, attention, and appraisal of sensory information have potential functional consequences for the back and brain of people in pain. The negative effects of inactivity and unloading on the musculoskeletal system are well documented68,69 and are recognized as important contributors to ongoing pain in approaches such as the fear-avoidance model.11 More recently, substantial evidence has also emerged highlighting significant changes within the central nervous system in people with persistent pain. Multiple studies have documented neurochemical,70 morphological,16,71 and organisational14,16,71,72 changes in the brains of people with CNSLBP, and many of these changes are evident in networks involved in attention, sensory processing, and motor planning and function.73

Functional changes effecting the back and brain may impact the person with LBP in a number of ways. The factors that we think are most important in maintaining the chronic pain state and that are supported by the current literature are (1) changes in musculoskeletal health such that the back is less fit and load tolerant,74–76 (2) increased efficiency within nociceptive networks such that there is augmentation of noxious information from the back,77,78 (3) decreased efficiency within proprioceptive and tactile networks such that there is diminution of non-noxious somatosensory information,79–88 and (4) disruption of brain grounded sensory and motor neural representations of the back.67,89–95

Disruption of neurally encoded representations of the back and diminution of non-noxious somatosensory information from the back will degrade motor control of the back and likely impacts self-perception of the back such that the back starts to feel foreign, peculiar, disconnected, and unfit. Qualitative investigations support this, noting that people with LBP perceive the back as fragile and vulnerable25,27,96 and feel a sense of alienation and rejection of the back.97–99 When quantitatively evaluated, people with LBP represent the back differently when asked to draw how the back feels to them100,101 and endorse questionnaire items associated with altered perceptual awareness of the back.101–104

Emerging Perceptual Models Reinforce Cognitive Models of a Damaged Fragile and Unhealthy Self

Central to our approach is the potential for self-sustaining interactions between cognitive models, or how we think about the body in pain, and perceptual models, how the body in pain feels to the individual. A body initially conceptualized as damaged, fragile, and unhealthy increasingly feels damaged, fragile, and unhealthy. The view that the back is not fit for purpose and in need of protection is thus reinforced by information from the back, and we contend that this state can become somewhat self-sustaining. The back is more strongly conceptualized as vulnerable and damaged and the associated behavioral responses further facilitated as the back deconditions, spinal motor control is degraded, self-perception is disrupted, and information that supports the supposition of a damaged, fragile, and unhealthy self is facilitated and more strongly weighted while information that counters this becomes less available and considered less trustworthy (see Fig. 1).

The Fit-For-Purpose Model (FFPM) of chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP).
Figure 1

The Fit-For-Purpose Model (FFPM) of chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP).

Relevance of the FFPM to Treatment of People With CNSLBP

The FFPM not only provides a novel perspective to help patients and clinicians understand CNSLBP, it also offers a framework to guide treatment and help integrate various common and contemporary practices into a coherent, graded, rehabilitation framework. Management strategies aligned with the FFPM aim to shift meta-cognitive, cognitive, and bodily related internal models that are consistent with a fragile, damaged, unhealthy, and unchangeable self toward the formulation of the back as healthy, strong, fit for purpose, and able to adaptively respond to progressive movement and loading. Information sources from the body as well as information sources external to the person can be used to drive change. A coherent treatment approach that aims to foster fitness for purpose can be organized according to 4 therapeutic targets that seek to help the person understand it is safe and helpful to move, refine neural representations so the back feels safe to move, load the back to promote positive tissue adaptations, and allow the person the experience safety with movement and consolidate safety under load through targeted self-management strategies.

Understand It Is Safe and Helpful to Move

To foster fitness for purpose, we contend that it is important that people with CNSLBP are provided with a less threatening and more hopeful understanding of their problem and a conceptualization of the drivers to the condition that make explicit both the safety and value of movement and loading. This may encompass the benefits that movement and activity impart on the health and load tolerance of the musculoskeletal system and the dynamic sensitivity of the body’s protective systems as well as the opportunity that movement and loading provide for learning about the back, exploring its capabilities, and influencing how it is represented cortically. Pain science education, which accommodates multiple “ways of knowing” and multiple types of knowledge,105 is a key intervention in promoting this understanding. This type of education emerged 2 decades ago in response to the failure of conventional back pain education and cognitive therapies to impact problematic and change-resistant conceptualizations of a damaged, fragile, and unhealthy self.22,106,107 Modern pain science education is based primarily on a constructivist model108 and encompasses a wide range of educational interventions aimed to improve an individual’s understanding of “how pain works,” including the distinction between nociception and pain and the clinical implications of this distinction; the dynamic sensitivity of nociceptors and other components of a wider “pain system”; the multifactorial processes that underpin the experience of pain and its persistence; and the ever-present tendency of biological systems to adapt to the demands placed on them, a concept recently coined as “bioplasticity.”109,110

A central objective of pain science education is to shift an individual’s conceptualization of pain from being simply a marker of tissue damage or disease to being a dynamic protective feeling that is influenced by multiple, largely reversible factors from across the biopsychosocial spectrum that vary across temporal scales ranging from moments to years.22 Pain science education has both generic learning objectives (eg, Moseley and Butler105) and learning objectives tailored to the individual. Individual learning objectives reflect specific misconceptions about the condition and individual factors that might shape the view that the back is not fit for purpose. Generic learning objectives, or “target concepts,”105 are based on contemporary pain science and consumer perspectives on recovery (eg, Leake et al111), so they change over time as new discoveries are made.112 Pain science education is foundational to our intervention because of the imperative for the individual to have the required procedural and conditional knowledge113 to make optimal collaborative decisions throughout the program and beyond. Inherent in this procedural and conditional knowledge is the above-mentioned concept of bioplasticity as it applies to both positive adaptation of body tissue and the development and reversibility of “pain system hypersensitivity.” Therefore, individually specific evidence of positive adaptation of tissue—for example via available radiological reports114 or experiences of soft tissue healing—helps to promote deep understanding of why movement and loading are critical for full recovery. The FFPM considers scientifically based beliefs about pain, loading, movement, and functional reversibility of the biological systems that underpin pain are potentially critical if one is to engage in subsequent aspects of the treatment package.

Refine Neural Representations of the Body So the Back Feels Safe to Move

There is a growing body of evidence to show that chronic pain is associated with progressive disruptions in bodily awareness and processing of bodily information.104,115–117 Disruptions of bodily awareness can be distressing and provide clear evidence to the individual experiencing them that the body is not fit for purpose and contribute to degraded motor control of the spine. Clinical investigation of these disruptions relies on measures such as tactile acuity118–120 and motor imagery performance,121 and laboratory investigation relies on brain imaging90,122 and electromyography.123 Performance in both clinical and laboratory tests seems to relate to perceptual and motor disruptions,100 and neural representation training normalizes performance (eg, Moseley and Flor120). The impact of these interventions when applied in isolation on outcomes such as pain and disability is small. However, normalizing these disruptions through neural representation training,124 such that the body begins to feel safe to move, may serve to reinforce the central educational message that the back is safe to move and help facilitate engagement in subsequent functional activities that move and load the back. Our inclusion of neural representation training to normalize the processes that subserve “how our body feels to us” is grounded in an interpretation of embodied cognition that emphasizes that cognitive processes are influenced by the body,125 particularly that information from the body shapes our views about the capacity of the body (eg, Zadra et al126). With respect to training, we propose that having a back that feels safe to move encompasses both how the back feels before and during movement. The aims, then, of neural representation training are to influence bodily-related neural representations that mold the internal models of health and load tolerance of the back—factors that shape the prediction of pain with movement and loading—and to utilize strategies that seek to increase the availability and precision of ongoing non-nociceptive somatosensory information from the back while the back is moved and loaded.

To meet these aims, neural representation training should involve both precision focused sensory and motor strategies. Sensory precision training requires a combination of repeated stimuli delivered to the back along with goal-orientated attention to each stimulus using techniques such as asking the patient to describe the location of stimulation or discern different types of stimulation.120 There is some evidence of an analgesic effect of sensory precision training in phantom limb pain127 and in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS).128 In CRPS, that sensory stimulation alone has no effect128 and that performance and analgesia are augmented by visual enhancement of touch and spatial attention129 point to cortical adaptations underpinning the effect. Preliminary data from participants with CNSLBP suggest sensory acuity improves with sensory discrimination training,130,131 and these improvements are associated with small reductions in pain intensity and disability.132 Moreover, data from other persistent pain conditions suggest that sensory precision training not only improves pain and sensory acuity but is also associated with normalization of cortical stimulus response profiles assessed using brain imaging approaches.127,133

A possible starting point for motor precision training is motor imagery training, which typically progresses from implicit to explicit motor imagery and involves a graded engagement of neural pathways that subserve movement planning and execution without moving the physical body.134,135 Implicit motor imagery commences with left/right judgement training in which a decision is made on whether a model viewed in an image has their back turned to the left or the right.136 Performance on this type of task is thought to offer insight into the integrity of proprioceptive representations of the pictured body part137 (but see Alazmi et al138 for 1 contrasting result), so it is plausible that training targeted at improving speed and accuracy in left/right judgements might update and improve cortical representations of the back.139 Preliminary data from people with CRPS offer some support for this idea,140–143 and there is a strong theoretical basis for proposing that implicit motor imagery induces rapid changes in cortical processing through subthreshold inhibitory pathways between pre-motor and primary motor areas (see Moseley et al135 for an expanded discussion). Movement observation, closely watching others move, is another approach to implicit motor imagery that has both theoretical and empirical support144 and has the advantage of being able to integrate contextual and functional characteristics that are individually tailored to a patient’s fears and goals.

To further drive activation of cortical pathways involved in movement, implicit motor imagery can be progressed to explicit motor imagery, where the individual imagines performing a movement or task in the first person. This can be augmented by having the individual simultaneously observe and mentally embody videos of people performing tasks of increasing complexity and personal relevance. Clinical trials in CRPS and phantom limb pain patients demonstrate the importance of the order of components (implicit followed by explicit) in progressive motor imagery training145 as well as possible clinical benefits.134

Progression from pre-movement strategies to movement of the physical body aims to further impact on cortical representations of the back and facilitate the availability and trustworthiness of non-noxious somatosensory information associated with back movement. Exercises that focus on mindful body mastery, such as learning how to perform precise and localized movements of the lumbar spine and hips, present an opportunity to achieve this aim. For example, learning to delineate the lumbar spine from the thorax and hips and precisely demarcate movement between these adjacent areas146,147 or learning to independently activate anatomically adjacent trunk muscles148 satisfies the requirements of a task that, theoretically, should improve precision of non-nociceptive information and sharpen cortical representations. Empirical support is sparse, but preliminary data suggest that specific trunk muscle training may shift motor cortex processing in people with back pain.149 Making goal attainment contingent on attention to specific movement characteristics, which will increase the weighting given to non-nociceptive data and improve the precision of cortical processing of that data, may be an important element of such approaches. Relevant here are the many common movement strategies employed by clinicians in the treatment of LBP. For example, performing a “hip stretch” could be reframed as a useful way of learning to delineate movement in the hip from movement in the back. Movement in this context, both active and passive, is seen not as a way of modifying tissue mechanics but as a way of sampling information from the body, learning about the body, updating internal models of the body, and manipulating the weighting given to sensory inputs within the context of potentially powerful cues of safety and health.

Load the Back to Promote Positive Tissue Adaptation and Experience Safety With Movement

As stated earlier, the experience of pain with movement is a very valid reason for concluding that the back is damaged, fragile, and unhealthy, and it is important to provide information to counter this view. Once the person understands that it is safe to move, observes both the disruption in somatosensory and motor processing and the resolution of these disruptions with training, and potentially starts to feel safe to move, then the treatment program can progress to functional movement training and loading. Here, the objectives are to (1) load the body and thus trigger positive adaptation towards greater load-bearing capacity of the tissues; and (2) through skilled movement coaching, provide movement and loading experiences that involve less pain than predicted and thus update internal models associated with how fit for purpose the back is. Experiencing that the back is safe to move and load builds on exposure based approaches to managing CNSLBP informed by the fear-avoidance model150–154 such that task selection is a shared decision-making process based on the person’s goals and employs behavioral experiments designed to violate prior expectations of the effect of the task on the back. In addition to these concepts, we would advocate skillful movement coaching and task modification in which the therapist and patient work together to identify strategies that minimize symptom provocation with action and thereby provide clear evidence of a healthy, load-tolerant self and safety with function, as well as demonstrate the potential for reversibility of movement sensitivity.

The epistemic nature of movement is at the forefront of this type of exercise prescription with a focus on the individual learning about their body, how to move and complete functional tasks with decreased provocation, the impact of context on sensitivity, and the reversible nature of their sensitivity state–their so-called “protect by pain zone.”109 We think this is best facilitated by a precision-focused and feedback-enriched approach to movement training and coaching in which participants progress from part practice to whole task practice of functionally relevant tasks while attention is directed towards non-nociceptive aspects of task performance through visual,155 tactile, proprioceptive, and even auditory sources of information. Several treatment models have been proposed that seek to decrease movement sensitivity through reducing unhelpful protective behaviors such as breath holding and muscle co-activation156 or provocative lumbar movement patterns such as disrupted load sharing.157–159 These approaches provide valuable skills to help the clinician enact the ideas presented here. Strategies to consider could also extend beyond movement quality or loading patterns and seek to utilize cues from across psychological and social domains that infer safety (see, eg, “the Protectometer” tool109). Emerging methods such as virtual reality-based training may also facilitate this step.160,161

RESOLVE: a progressive complex care package based on the Fit-For-Purpose Model (FFPM).
Figure 2

RESOLVE: a progressive complex care package based on the Fit-For-Purpose Model (FFPM).

Consolidate Safety Under Load

The long-term goal of treating CNSLBP according to the FFPM is resilience under the movement and loading conditions to which the individual is likely to be exposed. Thus, progression may continue to work-hardening contexts, traditional aerobic-based exercises, or general strength training according to the individual patient’s personal goals and values. That movement and loading of tissues triggers adaptations in those tissues and their functional engagement is well established,162 but there seems to be only weak relationships between peripheral musculoskeletal changes and clinical status.163 What little evidence is available suggests that traditional physical training may benefit people with back pain by decreasing fear and increasing confidence.164 Our approach would support a synergistic relationship between changes in the body and changes in beliefs. Positive adaptations within the tissues of the back may well contribute information that updates internal models toward a strong, healthy, and load-tolerant back. Simultaneously, the experience of undertaking intense physical training without significant symptom exacerbation provides strong evidence in support of this view and that the back is fit for purpose, even under large loads. That is, strength and fitness training provide countless potential opportunities to update meta-cognitive, cognitive, and proprioceptive internal models about the structural integrity and resilience of the back under given demands. This calls for expertly prescribed and appropriately dosed exercise in which priority is given to optimizing movement confidence rather than dosing regimens designed to simply optimize peripheral tissue adaptations.165 In this way, the FFPM represents a clear shift from previous models that recognize only peripheral adaptations in response to training166 or only meta-cognitive adaptations.154 A final therapeutic target of the FFPM is empowering the individual with the understanding and skills to self-manage and maintain optimal physical and psychological health under future challenges.

Treatment Summary

Figure 2 outlines a complex care package based on the FFPM currently being tested in a large sham-controlled explanatory clinical trial (“RESOLVE”20). The key aim of care is to reinforce fitness for purpose of the back by helping the individual: UNDERSTAND that it is safe and helpful to move; REFINE the neural representations of the body so the back FEELS safe to move; LOAD the back to promote positive tissue adaptation and experience safety with movement; and CONSOLIDATE the concept of safety under load through general exercise prescription and work-hardening programs orientated toward patient-derived goals. Each stage reinforces previous stages. Our, as yet untested, prediction is that optimal outcomes will be realized by treatment approaches that integrate these features in a synergistic manner. The FFPM has been the key theoretical guide to develop this complex care package, and we hope the model facilitates other health care professionals to make sense of those treatments they perceive as helpful and to implement them in synergistic ways that align with contemporary understanding of how pain and recovery work. We do not see this complex care package as a one-size-fits-all approach but an overarching blueprint for care in which there is significant scope for individualization in terms of the weighting of attention devoted to each of the steps, what is emphasized in each step, and how the aims of each step are best achieved for the individual. Note that all phases are individualized toward patient-derived fears, misconceptions, behaviors, and goals. Finally, tailoring of care should be optimized by assessing knowledge about “how pain works,”167 beliefs about the fitness for purpose of the back,168 back-specific body representations and awareness,104 motor imagery performance,121 sensory precision,169 and movement precision.146 These assessments can help shape and refine care as well as be used to evaluate the process of care.170

CNSLBP is a complex problem that is best understood through a biopsychosocial lens, particularly one that incorporates a contemporary understanding of the neurobiology of the pain experience. The FFPM regards CNSLBP as a problem of internal meta-cognitive, cognitive, and proprioceptive models that hold that the back is not fit for purpose. Based on this premise, we propose a theoretically justified, coherent, progressive complex care approach that targets modifiable factors to help shift internal models of a fragile, damaged, unhealthy, and unchangeable self towards the formulation of the back as healthy, strong, adaptable, and fit for purpose.

Key Points

  • CNSLBP represents a dynamic interplay between beliefs about the body in pain, neural representations that impact on how the painful area feels and moves, the information available as the back is moved and loaded, and the capacity of the musculoskeletal system to tolerate load.

  • Multimodal care for the person with back pain should seek to foster fitness for purpose by helping the person reconceptualize pain so they understand it is safe and helpful to move, targeting neural representations so the back feels safe to move, employing graded movement coaching to promote positive tissue adaptations and the experience of safety with movement and consolidate these through targeted self-management.

Author Contributions

Concept/idea/research design: B.M. Wand, A.G. Cashin, J.H. McAuley, M.K. Bagg, G.L. Moseley

Writing: B.M. Wand, A.G. Cashin, J.H. McAuley, M.K. Bagg, G.L. Moseley

Project management: B.M. Wand, A.G. Cashin

Consultation (including review of manuscript before submitting): B.M. Wand, M.K. Bagg, G.M. Orange, G.L. Moseley

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr Neil O’Connell from Brunel University for his comments on the manuscript and Associate Professor William Gibson and Dr Mervyn Travers from the University of Notre Dame Australia for their contribution and insightful observations on predictive processing and pain.

Funding

No specific grant was received for this study from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Disclosures

The authors completed the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest and reported no conflicts of interest.

B.M. Wand has received fees to speak about pain neuroscience and low back pain rehabilitation and is an investigator on the RESOLVE trial, which was funded by the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia, ID1087045. A.G. Cashin was supported by the University of New South Wales Prince of Wales Clinical School Postgraduate Research Scholarship and a NeuRA PhD Candidature Supplementary Scholarship. M.K. Bagg was supported by a NeuRA PhD Candidature Scholarship and was supported during this work by an Australian Research Training Program Scholarship and a UNSW Research Excellence Award. M.K. Bagg received conference travel support from the Chiropractor’s Association of Australia to speak about pain neuroscience and rehabilitation and Memorial University of Newfoundland to speak about engagement with research evidence. J.H. McAuley is supported by a Leadership Investigator Grant from the NHMRC of Australia; has received other research funding from the Australian Commonwealth National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) and the Australian Medical Research Future Fund (MRFF); and is an investigator on the RESOLVE trial. G.L. Moseley is supported by a Leadership Investigator grant from the NHMRC of Australia (ID 1178444), has received other research funding from NHMRC, the Australian Medical Research Future Fund (MRFF), and is an investigator on the RESOLVE trial (NHMRC ID1087045), other Australian and overseas governments, and not for profit agencies. G.L. Moseley also has received support from Reality Health; ConnectHealth UK; Seqirus; Kaiser Permanente; Workers’ Compensation Boards in Australia, Europe, and North America; AIA Australia; the International Olympic Committee; Western Australia Cricket Association; Arsenal Football Club; Brighton and Hove Albion Football Club; and Melbourne Football Club. Professional and scientific bodies have reimbursed G.L. Moseley for travel costs related to presentation of research on pain at scientific conferences/symposia. G.L. Moseley has received speaker fees for lectures on pain and rehabilitation and receives book royalties from NOIgroup Publications and Dancing Giraffe Press, including key texts involved in the education component of the FFPM.

References

1.

Cholewicki
 
J
,
Popovich
 
JM
,
Aminpour
 
P
,
Gray
 
SA
,
Lee
 
AS
,
Hodges
 
PW
.
Development of a collaborative model of low back pain: report from the 2017 NASS consensus meeting
.
Spine J
.
2019
;
19
:
1029
1040
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spinee.2018.11.014.

2.

Hartvigsen
 
J
,
Hancock
 
MJ
,
Kongsted
 
A
 et al.  
What low back pain is and why we need to pay attention
.
Lancet
.
2018
;
391
:
2356
2367
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30480-X.

3.

Maher
 
C
,
Underwood
 
M
,
Buchbinder
 
R
.
Non-specific low back pain
.
Lancet
.
2017
;
389
:
736
747
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30970-9.

4.

Menezes Costa
 
LDC
,
Maher
 
CG
,
Hancock
 
MJ
,
McAuley
 
JH
,
Herbert
 
RD
,
Costa
 
LOP
.
The prognosis of acute and persistent low-back pain: a meta-analysis
.
Can Med Assoc J
.
2012
;
184
:
E613
E624
. https://doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.111271.

5.

Haldeman
 
S
,
Dagenais
 
S
.
A supermarket approach to the evidence-informed management of chronic low back pain
.
Spine J
.
2008
;
8
:
1
7
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spinee.2007.10.009.

6.

Bagg
 
MK
,
McAuley
 
JH
,
Moseley
 
GL
,
Wand
 
BM
.
Recent data from radiofrequency denervation trials further emphasise that treating nociception is not the same as treating pain
.
Br J Sports Med
.
2018
;
53
:
841
842
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2017-098510.

7.

Kamper
 
SJ
,
Apeldoorn
 
AT
,
Chiarotto
 
A
 et al.  
Multidisciplinary biopsychosocial rehabilitation for chronic low back pain
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev
.
2014
;
9
:
CD000963
. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD000963.pub3.

8.

Bernstein
 
IA
,
Malik
 
Q
,
Carville
 
S
,
Ward
 
S
.
Low back pain and sciatica: summary of NICE guidance
.
BMJ
.
2017
;
356
:
i6748
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.i6748.

9.

Wand
,
B.M.
 Chronic lower back pain: a maladaptive perceptions model. In:
NOI Neurodynamics and the Neuromatrix Conference
.
Adelaide, Australia
:
April, 2012
.

10.

Leeuw
 
M
,
Goossens
 
MEJB
,
Linton
 
SJ
,
Crombez
 
G
,
Boersma
 
K
,
Vlaeyen
 
JWS
.
The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: current state of scientific evidence
.
J Behav Med
.
2007
;
30
:
77
94
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-006-9085-0.

11.

Vlaeyen
 
JWS
,
Linton
 
SJ
.
Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art
.
Pain
.
2000
;
85
:
317
332
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00242-0.

12.

Jones
 
CL
,
Jensen
 
JD
,
Scherr
 
CL
,
Brown
 
NR
,
Christy
 
K
,
Weaver
 
J
.
The health belief model as an explanatory framework in communication research: exploring parallel, serial, and moderated mediation
.
Health Commun
.
2015
;
30
:
566
576
. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2013.873363.

13.

Leventhal
 
H
,
Phillips
 
LA
,
Burns
 
E
.
The common-sense model of self-regulation (CSM): a dynamic framework for understanding illness self-management
.
J Behav Med
.
2016
;
39
:
935
946
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-016-9782-2.

14.

Goossens
 
N
,
Rummens
 
S
,
Janssens
 
L
,
Caeyenberghs
 
K
,
Brumagne
 
S
.
Association between sensorimotor impairments and functional brain changes in patients with low back pain
.
Am J Phys Med Rehabil
.
2018
;
97
:
200
211
. https://doi.org/10.1097/PHM.0000000000000859.

15.

Malfliet
 
A
,
Coppieters
 
I
,
Van Wilgen
 
P
 et al.  
Brain changes associated with cognitive and emotional factors in chronic pain: a systematic review
.
Eur J Pain (United Kingdom)
.
2017
;
21
:
769
786
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.1003.

16.

Ng
 
SK
,
Urquhart
 
DM
,
Fitzgerald
 
PB
,
Cicuttini
 
FM
,
Hussain
 
SM
,
Fitzgibbon
 
BM
.
The relationship between structural and functional brain changes and altered emotion and cognition in chronic low back pain
.
Clin J Pain
.
2017
;
34
:
237
261
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000534.

17.

Edwards
 
MJ
,
Adams
 
RA
,
Brown
 
H
,
Pareés
 
I
,
Friston
 
KJ
.
A Bayesian account of “hysteria.”
 
Brain
.
2012
;
135
:
3495
3512
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws129.

18.

Ongaro
 
G
,
Kaptchuk
 
TJ
.
Symptom perception, placebo effects, and the Bayesian brain
.
Pain
.
2019
;
160
:
1
4
. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001367.

19.

Tabor
 
A
,
Thacker
 
MA
,
Moseley
 
GL
,
Körding
 
KP
.
Pain: a statistical account
.
PLoS Comput Biol
.
2017
;
13
:
e1005142
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005142.

20.

Bagg
 
MK
,
Hübscher
 
M
,
Rabey
 
M
 et al.  
The RESOLVE Trial for people with chronic low back pain: protocol for a randomised clinical trial
.
J Physiother
.
2017
;
63
:
47
48
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphys.2016.11.001.

21.

Wall
 
PD
,
McMahon
 
SB
.
The relationship of perceived pain to afferent nerve impulses
.
Trends Neurosci
.
1986
;
9
:
254
255
. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(86)90070-6.

22.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Butler
 
DS
.
Fifteen years of explaining pain: the past, present, and future
.
J Pain
.
2015a
;
16
:
807
813
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.005.

23.

Picard
 
F
,
Friston
 
K
.
Predictions, perception, and a sense of self
.
Neurology
.
2014
;
83
:
1112
1118
. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000000798.

24.

Darlow
 
B
.
Beliefs about back pain: the confluence of client, clinician and community
.
Int J Osteopath Med
.
2016
;
20
:
53
61
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijosm.2016.01.005.

25.

Darlow
 
B
,
Dean
 
S
,
Perry
 
M
,
Mathieson
 
F
,
Baxter
 
GD
,
Dowell
 
A
.
Easy to harm, hard to heal
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2015
;
40
:
842
850
. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0000000000000901.

26.

Setchell
 
J
,
Costa
 
N
,
Ferreira
 
M
,
Makovey
 
J
,
Nielsen
 
M
,
Hodges
 
PW
.
Individuals’ explanations for their persistent or recurrent low back pain: a cross-sectional survey
.
BMC Musculoskelet Disord
.
2017
;
18
:
466
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12891-017-1831-7.

27.

Bunzli
 
S
,
Watkins
 
R
,
Smith
 
A
,
Schütze
 
R
,
O’Sullivan
 
P
.
Lives on hold: a qualitative synthesis exploring the experience of chronic low-back pain
.
Clin J Pain
.
2013
;
29
:
907
916
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e31827a6dd8.

28.

Ford
 
JJ
,
Hahne
 
AJ
,
Surkitt
 
LD
 et al.  
Individualised physiotherapy as an adjunct to guideline-based advice for low back disorders in primary care: a randomised controlled trial
.
Br J Sports Med
.
2016
;
50
:
237
245
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2015-095058.

29.

Petersen
 
T
,
Olsen
 
S
,
Laslett
 
M
 et al.  
Inter-tester reliability of a new diagnostic classification system for patients with non-specific low back pain
.
Aust J Physiother
.
2004
;
50
:
85
94
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0004-9514(14)60100-8.

30.

May
 
S
,
Donelson
 
R
.
Evidence-informed management of chronic low back pain with the McKenzie method
.
Spine J
.
2008
;
8
:
134
141
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spinee.2007.10.017.

31.

Parreira
 
P
,
Heymans
 
MW
,
van
 
Tulder
 
MW
 et al.  
Back schools for chronic non-specific low back pain
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev
.
2017
;
8
:
CD011674
. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD011674.pub2.

32.

Tariq
 
RA
,
George
 
JS
,
Ampat
 
G
,
Toney-Butler
 
TJ
.
Back Safety
. Tampa, FL:
StatPearls Publishing
;
2021
.

33.

Verbeek
 
JH
,
Martimo
 
KP
,
Kuijer
 
PPFM
,
Karppinen
 
J
,
Viikari-Juntura
 
E
,
Takala
 
EP
.
Proper manual handling techniques to prevent low back pain, a Cochrane systematic review
.
Work
.
2012
;
41
:
2299
2301
. https://doi.org/10.3233/WOR-2012-0455-2299.

34.

Hall
 
A
,
Aubrey-Bassler
 
K
,
Thorne
 
B
,
Maher
 
CG
.
Do not routinely offer imaging for uncomplicated low back pain
.
BMJ
.
2021
;
372
:
n291
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n291.

35.

Oliveira
 
CB
,
Maher
 
CG
,
Pinto
 
RZ
 et al.  
Clinical practice guidelines for the management of non-specific low back pain in primary care: an updated overview
.
Eur Spine J
.
2018
;
27
:
2791
2803
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-018-5673-2.

36.

Kamper
 
SJ
,
Logan
 
G
,
Copsey
 
B
 et al.  
What is usual care for low back pain? A systematic review of health care provided to patients with low back pain in family practice and emergency departments
.
Pain
.
2020
;
161
:
694
702
. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001751.

37.

Brinjikji
 
W
,
Luetmer
 
PH
,
Comstock
 
B
 et al.  
Systematic literature review of imaging features of spinal degeneration in asymptomatic populations
.
Am J Neuroradiol
.
2015
;
36
:
811
816
. https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A4173.

38.

Karran
 
EL
,
Medalian
 
Y
,
Hillier
 
SL
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
The impact of choosing words carefully: an online investigation into imaging reporting strategies and best practice care for low back pain
.
PeerJ
.
2017
;
5
:
e4151
. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.4151.

39.

Chou
 
R
,
Shekelle
 
P
.
Will this patient develop persistent disabling low back pain?
 
JAMA-J Am Med Assoc
.
2010
;
303
:
1295
1302
. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2010.344.

40.

Kent
 
PM
,
Keating
 
JL
.
Can we predict poor recovery from recent-onset nonspecific low back pain? A systematic review
.
Man Ther
.
2008
;
13
:
12
28
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2007.05.009.

41.

Pincus
 
T
,
Burton
 
AK
,
Vogel
 
S
,
Field
 
AP
.
A systematic review of psychological factors as predictors of chronicity/disability in prospective cohorts of low back pain
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2002
;
27
:
E109
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00007632-200203010-00017.

42.

Steenstra
 
IA
,
Verbeek
 
JH
,
Heymans
 
MW
,
Bongers
 
PM
.
Prognostic factors for duration of sick leave in patients sick listed with acute low back pain: a systematic review of the literature
.
Occup Environ Med
.
2005
;
62
:
851
860
. https://doi.org/10.1136/oem.2004.015842.

43.

Campbell
 
P
,
Foster
 
NE
,
Thomas
 
E
,
Dunn
 
KM
.
Prognostic indicators of low back pain in primary care: five-year prospective study
.
J Pain
.
2013
;
14
:
873
883
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2013.03.013.

44.

Green
 
DJ
,
Lewis
 
M
,
Mansell
 
G
 et al.  
Clinical course and prognostic factors across different musculoskeletal pain sites: a secondary analysis of individual patient data from randomised clinical trials
.
Eur J Pain
.
2018
;
22
:
1057
1070
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.1190.

45.

Grotle
 
M
,
Foster
 
NE
,
Dunn
 
KM
,
Croft
 
P
.
Are prognostic indicators for poor outcome different for acute and chronic low back pain consulters in primary care?
 
Pain
.
2010
;
151
:
790
797
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.014.

46.

da
 
Silva
 
T
,
Mills
 
K
,
Brown
 
BT
 et al.  
Recurrence of low back pain is common: a prospective inception cohort study
.
J Physiother
.
2019
;
65
:
159
165
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphys.2019.04.010.

47.

da
 
Silva
 
T
,
Mills
 
K
,
Brown
 
BT
,
Herbert
 
RD
,
Maher
 
CG
,
Hancock
 
MJ
.
Risk of recurrence of low back pain: a systematic review
.
J Orthop Sport Phys Ther
.
2017
;
47
:
305
313
. https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2017.7415.

48.

Gore
 
M
,
Sadosky
 
A
,
Stacey
 
BR
,
Tai
 
KS
,
Leslie
 
D
.
The burden of chronic low back pain: clinical comorbidities, treatment patterns, and health care costs in usual care settings
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2012
;
37
:
E668
E677
. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0b013e318241e5de.

49.

Ramanathan
 
S
,
Hibbert
 
P
,
Wiles
 
L
,
Maher
 
CG
,
Runciman
 
W
.
What is the association between the presence of comorbidities and the appropriateness of care for low back pain? A population-based medical record review study 11 Medical and Health Sciences 1117 Public Health and Health Services
.
BMC Musculoskelet Disord
.
2018
;
19
:
391
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12891-018-2316-z.

50.

van der
 
Zee-Neuen
 
A
,
Putrik
 
P
,
Ramiro
 
S
 et al.  
Impact of chronic diseases and multimorbidity on health and health care costs: the additional role of musculoskeletal disorders
.
Arthritis Care Res
.
2016
;
68
:
1823
1831
. https://doi.org/10.1002/acr.22913.

51.

Linton
 
SJ
.
A review of psychological risk factors in back and neck pain
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2000
;
25
:
1148
1156
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00007632-200005010-00017.

52.

Nicholas
 
MK
,
Linton
 
SJ
,
Watson
 
PJ
,
Main
 
CJ
.
Early identification and management of psychological risk factors (“yellow flags”) in patients with low back pain: a reappraisal
.
Phys Ther.
 
2011
;
91
:
737
753
. doi: https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20100224.

53.

Pinheiro
 
MB
,
Ferreira
 
ML
,
Refshauge
 
K
 et al.  
Symptoms of depression as a prognostic factor for low back pain: a systematic review
.
Spine J
.
2016
;
16
:
105
116
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spinee.2015.10.037.

54.

Myers
 
SS
,
Phillips
 
RS
,
Davis
 
RB
 et al.  
Patient expectations as predictors of outcome in patients with acute low back pain
.
J Gen Intern Med
.
2007
;
23
:
148
153
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-007-0460-5.

55.

Vowles
 
KE
,
Zvolensky
 
MJ
,
Gross
 
RT
,
Sperry
 
JA
.
Pain-related anxiety in the prediction of chronic low-back pain distress
.
J Behav Med
.
2004
;
27
:
77
89
. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBM.0000013645.40613.42.

56.

Baliki
 
MN
,
Apkarian
 
AV
.
Nociception, pain, negative moods, and behavior selection
.
Neuron
.
2015
;
87
:
474
491
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.005.

57.

Friston
 
K
.
The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory?
 
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2010
;
11
:
127
138
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787.

58.

Den Hollander
 
M
,
Goossens
 
M
,
De Jong
 
J
 et al.  
Expose or protect? A randomized controlled trial of exposure in vivo vs pain-contingent treatment as usual in patients with complex regional pain syndrome type 1
.
Pain
.
2016
;
157
:
2318
2329
. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000651.

59.

Geisser
 
ME
,
Haig
 
AJ
,
Wallbom
 
AS
,
Wiggert
 
EA
.
Pain-related fear, lumbar flexion, and dynamic emg among persons with chronic musculoskeletal low back pain
.
Clin J Pain
.
2004
;
20
:
61
69
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00002508-200403000-00001.

60.

Karayannis
 
NV
,
Smeets
 
RJEM
,
van den
 
Hoorn
 
W
,
Hodges
 
PW
.
Fear of movement is related to trunk stiffness in low back pain
.
PLoS One
.
2013
;
8
:
e67779
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0067779.

61.

Lamoth
 
CJC
,
Meijer
 
OG
,
Daffertshofer
 
A
,
Wuisman
 
PIJM
,
Beek
 
PJ
.
Effects of chronic low back pain on trunk coordination and back muscle activity during walking: changes in motor control
.
Eur Spine J
.
2006
;
15
:
23
40
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-004-0825-y.

62.

Lamoth
 
CJC
,
Meijer
 
OG
,
Wuisman
 
PIJM
,
van
 
Dieën
 
JH
,
Levin
 
MF
,
Beek
 
PJ
.
Pelvis-thorax coordination in the transverse plane during walking in persons with nonspecific low back pain
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2002
;
27
:
E92
E99
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00007632-200202150-00016.

63.

Abboud
 
J
,
Nougarou
 
F
,
Pagé
 
I
,
Cantin
 
V
,
Massicotte
 
D
,
Descarreaux
 
M
.
Trunk motor variability in patients with non-specific chronic low back pain
.
Eur J Appl Physiol
.
2014
;
114
:
2645
2654
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-014-2985-8.

64.

Ebrahimi
 
S
,
Kamali
 
F
,
Razeghi
 
M
,
Haghpanah
 
SA
.
Comparison of the trunk-pelvis and lower extremities sagittal plane inter-segmental coordination and variability during walking in persons with and without chronic low back pain
.
Hum Mov Sci
.
2017
;
52
:
55
66
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2017.01.004.

65.

Falla
 
D
,
Gizzi
 
L
,
Tschapek
 
M
,
Erlenwein
 
J
,
Petzke
 
F
.
Reduced task-induced variations in the distribution of activity across back muscle regions in individuals with low back pain
.
Pain
.
2014
;
155
:
944
953
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2014.01.027.

66.

Gizzi
 
L
,
Röhrle
 
O
,
Petzke
 
F
,
Falla
 
D
.
People with low back pain show reduced movement complexity during their most active daily tasks
.
Eur J Pain (United Kingdom)
.
2019
;
23
:
410
418
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.1318.

67.

Tsao
 
H
,
Danneels
 
LA
,
Hodges
 
PW
.
ISSLS prize winner: smudging the motor brain in young adults with recurrent low back pain
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2011
;
36
:
1721
1727
. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0b013e31821c4267.

68.

Kehler
 
DS
,
Theou
 
O
,
Rockwood
 
K
.
Bed rest and accelerated aging in relation to the musculoskeletal and cardiovascular systems and frailty biomarkers: a review
.
Exp Gerontol
.
2019
;
124
:
110643
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exger.2019.110643.

69.

Ramsey
 
KA
,
Rojer
 
AGM
,
D’Andrea
 
L
 et al.  
The association of objectively measured physical activity and sedentary behavior with skeletal muscle strength and muscle power in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Ageing Res Rev
.
2021
;
67
:
101266
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2021.101266.

70.

Zhao
 
X
,
Xu
 
M
,
Jorgenson
 
K
,
Kong
 
J
.
Neurochemical changes in patients with chronic low back pain detected by proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy: a systematic review
.
Neuroimage Clin
.
2017
;
13
:
33
38
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.11.006.

71.

Kregel
 
J
,
Meeus
 
M
,
Malfliet
 
A
 et al.  
Structural and functional brain abnormalities in chronic low back pain: a systematic review
.
Semin Arthritis Rheum
.
2015
;
45
:
29
237
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.semarthrit.2015.05.002.

72.

Schouppe
 
S
,
Van Oosterwijck
 
S
,
Danneels
 
L
,
Van Damme
 
S
,
Van Oosterwijck
 
J
.
Are functional brain alterations present in low back pain? A systematic review of EEG studies
.
J Pain
.
2020
;
21
:
25
43
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2019.06.010.

73.

Wand
 
BM
,
Parkitny
 
L
,
O’Connell
 
NE
 et al.  
Cortical changes in chronic low back pain: current state of the art and implications for clinical practice
.
Man Ther
.
2011
;
16
:
15
20
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2010.06.008.

74.

Briggs
 
AM
,
Straker
 
LM
,
Burnett
 
AF
,
Wark
 
JD
.
Chronic low back pain is associated with reduced vertebral bone mineral measures in community-dwelling adults
.
BMC Musculoskelet Disord
.
2012
;
13
:
49
. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2474-13-49.

75.

Noonan
 
AM
,
Brown
 
SHM
.
Paraspinal muscle pathophysiology associated with low back pain and spine degenerative disorders
.
JOR Spine
.
2021
;
4
:
e1171
. https://doi.org/10.1002/jsp2.1171.

76.

Steele
 
J
,
Bruce-Low
 
S
,
Smith
 
D
.
A reappraisal of the deconditioning hypothesis in low back pain: review of evidence from a triumvirate of research methods on specific lumbar extensor deconditioning
.
Curr Med Res Opin
.
2014
;
30
:
865
911
. https://doi.org/10.1185/03007995.2013.875465.

77.

Den Bandt
 
HL
,
Paulis
 
WD
,
Beckweé
 
D
,
Ickmans
 
K
,
Nijs
 
J
,
Voogt
 
L
.
Pain mechanisms in low back pain: a systematic review with meta-analysis of mechanical quantitative sensory testing outcomes in people with nonspecific low back pain
.
J Orthop Sports Phys Ther
.
2019
;
49
:
698
715
. https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2019.8876.

78.

McPhee
 
ME
,
Vaegter
 
HB
,
Graven-Nielsen
 
T
.
Alterations in pronociceptive and antinociceptive mechanisms in patients with low back pain: a systematic review with meta-analysis
.
Pain
.
2020
;
161
:
464
475
. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001737.

79.

Adamczyk
 
W
,
Luedtke
 
K
,
Saulicz
 
E
.
Lumbar tactile acuity in patients with low back pain and healthy controls
.
Clin J Pain
.
2017
;
34
:
1
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000499.

80.

Brumagne
 
S
,
Cordo
 
P
,
Verschueren
 
S
.
Proprioceptive weighting changes in persons with low back pain and elderly persons during upright standing
.
Neurosci Lett
.
2004
;
366
:
63
66
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.05.013.

81.

Claeys
 
K
,
Brumagne
 
S
,
Dankaerts
 
W
,
Kiers
 
H
,
Janssens
 
L
.
Decreased variability in postural control strategies in young people with non-specific low back pain is associated with altered proprioceptive reweighting
.
Eur J Appl Physiol
.
2011
;
111
:
115
123
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-010-1637-x.

82.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Gallagher
 
L
,
Gallace
 
A
.
Neglect-like tactile dysfunction in chronic back pain
.
Neurology
.
2012c
;
79
:
327
332
. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e318260cba2.

83.

Puta
 
C
,
Schulz
 
B
,
Schoeler
 
S
 et al.  
Somatosensory abnormalities for painful and innocuous stimuli at the back and at a site distinct from the region of pain in chronic back pain patients
.
PLoS One
.
2013
;
8
:
e58885
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0058885.

84.

Rausch Osthoff
 
AK
,
Ernst
 
MJ
,
Rast
 
FM
 et al.  
Measuring lumbar reposition accuracy in patients with unspecific low back pain: systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2015
;
40
:
E97
E111
. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0000000000000677.

85.

Tong
 
MH
,
Mousavi
 
SJ
,
Kiers
 
H
,
Ferreira
 
P
,
Refshauge
 
K
,
van
 
Dieën
 
J
. Is
there a relationship between lumbar proprioception and low back pain? A systematic review with meta-analysis
.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
,
2017
;
98
:
120
136.e2
. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2016.05.016.

86.

Wand
 
BM
,
Keeves
 
J
,
Bourgoin
 
C
 et al.  
Mislocalization of sensory information in people with chronic low back pain: a preliminary investigation
.
Clin J Pain
.
2013
;
29
:
737
743
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e318274b320.

87.

Wang
 
H
,
Fischer
 
C
,
Chen
 
G
,
Weinsheimer
 
N
,
Gantz
 
S
,
Schiltenwolf
 
M
.
Does long-term opioid therapy reduce pain sensitivity of patients with chronic low back pain? Evidence from quantitative sensory testing
.
Pain Physician
.
2012
;
15
:
ES135
ES143
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2011.01276.x.

88.

Willigenburg
 
NW
,
Kingma
 
I
,
Hoozemans
 
MJM
,
van
 
Dieën
 
JH
.
Precision control of trunk movement in low back pain patients
.
Hum Mov Sci
.
2013
;
32
:
228
239
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2012.12.007.

89.

Elgueta-Cancino
 
E
,
Schabrun
 
S
,
Hodges
 
P
.
Is the organization of the primary motor cortex in low back pain related to pain, movement, and/or sensation?
 
Clin J Pain
.
2018
;
34
:
207
216
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000535.

90.

Flor
 
H
,
Braun
 
C
,
Elbert
 
T
,
Birbaumer
 
N
.
Extensive reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex in chronic back pain patients
.
Neurosci Lett
.
1997
;
224
:
5
8
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(97)13441-3.

91.

Goossens
 
N
,
Janssens
 
L
,
Brumagne
 
S
.
Changes in the organization of the secondary somatosensory cortex while processing lumbar proprioception and the relationship with sensorimotor control in low back pain
.
Clin J Pain
.
2019
;
35
:
394
406
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000692.

92.

Hotz-Boendermaker
 
S
,
Marcar
 
VL
,
Meier
 
ML
,
Boendermaker
 
B
,
Humphreys
 
BK
.
Reorganization in secondary somatosensory cortex in chronic low back pain patients
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2016
;
41
:
E667
E673
. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0000000000001348.

93.

Lloyd
 
D
,
Findlay
 
G
,
Roberts
 
N
,
Nurmikko
 
T
.
Differences in low back pain behavior are reflected in the cerebral response to tactile stimulation of the lower back
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2008
;
33
:
1372
1377
. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181734a8a.

94.

Schabrun
 
SM
,
Elgueta-Cancino
 
EL
,
Hodges
 
PW
.
Smudging of the motor cortex is related to the severity of low back pain
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
2017
;
42
:
1172
1178
. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0000000000000938.

95.

Tsao
 
H
,
Galea
 
MP
,
Hodges
 
PW
.
Reorganization of the motor cortex is associated with postural control deficits in recurrent low back pain
.
Brain
.
2008
;
131
:
2161
2171
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn154.

96.

Stenberg
 
G
,
Fjellman-Wiklund
 
A
,
Ahlgren
 
C
.
“I am afraid to make the damage worse” - fear of engaging in physical activity among patients with neck or back pain - a gender perspective
.
Scand J Caring Sci
.
2014
;
28
:
146
154
. https://doi.org/10.1111/scs.12043.

97.

Afrell
 
M
,
Biguet
 
G
,
Rudebeck
 
CE
.
Living with a body in pain—between acceptance and denial
.
Scand J Caring Sci
.
2007
;
21
:
291
296
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6712.2007.00475.x.

98.

Crowe
 
M
,
Whitehead
 
L
,
Gagan
 
MJ
,
Baxter
 
GD
,
Pankhurst
 
A
,
Valledor
 
V
.
Listening to the body and talking to myself - the impact of chronic lower back pain: a qualitative study
.
Int J Nurs Stud
.
2010
;
47
:
586
592
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2009.09.012.

99.

Osborn
 
M
,
Smith
 
JA
.
Living with a body separate from the self. The experience of the body in chronic benign low back pain: an interpretative phenomenological analysis
.
Scand J Caring Sci
.
2006
;
20
:
216
222
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6712.2006.00399.x.

100.

Moseley
 
LG
.
I can’t find it! Distorted body image and tactile dysfunction in patients with chronic back pain
.
Pain
.
2008
;
140
:
239
243
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2008.08.001.

101.

Nishigami
 
T
,
Mibu
 
A
,
Osumi
 
M
 et al.  
Are tactile acuity and clinical symptoms related to differences in perceived body image in patients with chronic nonspecific lower back pain?
 
Man Ther
.
2015
;
20
:
63
67
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2014.06.010.

102.

Ehrenbrusthoff
 
K
,
Ryan
 
CG
,
Grüneberg
 
C
,
Martin
 
DJ
.
A systematic review and meta-analysis of the reliability and validity of sensorimotor measurement instruments in people with chronic low back pain
.
Musculoskelet Sci Pract
.
2018
;
35
:
73
83
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msksp.2018.02.007.

103.

Janssens
 
L
,
Goossens
 
N
,
Wand
 
BM
,
Pijnenburg
 
M
,
Thys
 
T
,
Brumagne
 
S
.
The development of the Dutch version of the Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire
.
Musculoskelet Sci Pract
.
2017
;
32
:
84
91
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msksp.2017.09.003.

104.

Wand
 
BM
,
Catley
 
MJ
,
Rabey
 
MI
,
O’Sullivan
 
PB
,
O’Connell
 
NE
,
Smith
 
AJ
.
Disrupted self-perception in people with chronic low back pain. Further evaluation of the Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire
.
J Pain
.
2016
;
17
:
1001
1012
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.06.003.

105.

Moseley
 
G
,
Butler
 
D
.
Explain Pain Supercharged
.
Adelaide, Australia
:
Noi Group Publications
;
2017
.

106.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Nicholas
 
MK
,
Hodges
 
PW
.
A randomized controlled trial of intensive neurophysiology education in chronic low back pain
.
Clin J Pain
.
2004
;
20
:
324
330
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00002508-200409000-00007.

107.

Moseley
 
L
.
Combined physiotherapy and education is efficacious for chronic low back pain
.
Aust J Physiother
.
2002
;
48
:
297
302
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0004-9514(14)60169-0.

108.

Yager
 
RE
.
The constructivist learning model
.
Sci Teach
.
1991
;
58
:
52
57
.

109.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Butler
 
DS
.
The Explain Pain Handbook: Protectometer
. Adelaide, Australia:
Noigroup Publications
;
2015
.

110.

Moseley
 
LG.
 
It is not just the brain that changes itself—time to embrace bioplasticity
?
Interational Association for the Study of Pain
.
2022
. Accessed April 19, 2022. https://www.iasp-pain.org/publications/relief-news/article/time-to-embrace-bioplasticity/.

111.

Leake
 
HB
,
Moseley
 
GL
,
Stanton
 
TR
,
O’Hagan
 
ET
,
Heathcote
 
LC
.
What do patients value learning about pain? A mixed-methods survey on the relevance of target concepts after pain science education
.
Pain Publish Ah
.
2021
;
162
:
2558
2568
. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002244.

112.

Cashin
 
AG
,
Lee
 
H
,
Traeger
 
AC
 et al.  
Producing clinically meaningful reductions in disability: a causal mediation analysis of a patient education intervention
.
J Pain
.
2021
;
23
:
236
247
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2021.07.007.

113.

de
 
Jong
 
T
,
Ferguson-Hessler
 
MGM
.
Types and qualities of knowledge
.
Educ Psychol
.
1996
;
31
:
105
113
. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3102_2.

114.

Karran
 
EL
,
Hillier
 
SL
,
Yau
 
Y-H
,
McAuley
 
JH
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
A quasi-randomised, controlled, feasibility trial of GLITtER (Green Light Imaging Interpretation to Enhance Recovery)—a psychoeducational intervention for adults with low back pain attending secondary care
.
PeerJ
.
2018
;
6
:
e 4301
. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.4301.

115.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Gallace
 
A
,
Spence
 
C
.
Bodily illusions in health and disease: physiological and clinical perspectives and the concept of a cortical ‘body matrix
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2012b
;
36
:
34
46
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.013.

116.

Nishigami
 
T
,
Watanabe
 
A
,
Maitani
 
T
 et al.  
Development and validation of a shoulder-specific body-perception questionnaire in people with persistent shoulder pain
.
BMC Musculoskelet Disord
.
2021
;
22
:
98
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12891-021-03944-z.

117.

Nishigami
 
T
,
Mibu
 
A
,
Tanaka
 
K
 et al.  
Development and psychometric properties of knee-specific body-perception questionnaire in people with knee osteoarthritis: The Fremantle Knee Awareness Questionnaire
.
PLoS One
.
2017
;
12
:
e0179225
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0179225.

118.

Catley
 
MJ
,
O’Connell
 
NE
,
Berryman
 
C
,
Ayhan
 
FF
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
Is tactile acuity altered in people with chronic pain?
 
A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J Pain
.
2014
;
15
:
985
1000
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2014.06.009.

119.

Luomajoki
 
H
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
Tactile acuity and lumbopelvic motor control in patients with back pain and healthy controls
.
Br J Sports Med
.
2011
;
45
:
437
440
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2009.060731.

120.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Flor
 
H
.
Targeting cortical representations in the treatment of chronic pain
.
Neurorehabil Neural Repair
.
2012
;
26
:
646
652
. https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968311433209.

121.

Bowering
 
KJ
,
Butler
 
DS
,
Fulton
 
IJ
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
Motor imagery in people with a history of back pain, current back pain, both, or neither
.
Clin J Pain
.
2014
;
30
:
1
19
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000066.

122.

Chang
 
W-J
,
O’Connell
 
NE
,
Beckenkamp
 
PR
,
Alhassani
 
G
,
Liston
 
MB
,
Schabrun
 
SM
.
Altered primary motor cortex structure, organization, and function in chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J Pain
.
2018
;
19
:
341
359
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2017.10.007.

123.

Hodges
 
PW
,
Richardson
 
CA
.
Inefficient muscular stabilization of the lumbar spine associated with low back pain
.
Spine (Phila Pa 1976)
.
1996
;
21
:
2640
2650
. https://doi.org/10.1097/00007632-199611150-00014.

124.

Wallwork
 
SB
,
Bellan
 
V
,
Catley
 
MJ
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
Neural representations and the cortical body matrix: implications for sports medicine and future directions
.
Br J Sports Med
.
2015
;
50
:
990
996
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2015-095356.

125.

Goldinger
 
SD
,
Papesh
 
MH
,
Barnhart
 
AS
,
Hansen
 
WA
,
Hout
 
MC
.
The poverty of embodied cognition
.
Psychon Bull Rev
.
2016
;
23
:
959
978
. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0860-1.

126.

Zadra
 
JR
,
Weltman
 
AL
,
Proffitt
 
DR
.
Walkable distances are bioenergetically scaled
.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
.
2016
;
42
:
39
51
. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000107.

127.

Flor
 
H
,
Denke
 
C
,
Schaefer
 
M
,
Grüsser
 
S
.
Effect of sensory discrimination training on cortical reorganisation and phantom limb pain
.
Lancet
.
2001
;
357
:
1763
1764
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(00)04890-X.

128.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Zalucki
 
NM
,
Wiech
 
K
.
Tactile discrimination, but not tactile stimulation alone, reduces chronic limb pain
.
Pain
.
2008
;
137
:
600
608
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2007.10.021.

129.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Wiech
 
K
.
The effect of tactile discrimination training is enhanced when patients watch the reflected image of their unaffected limb during training
.
Pain
.
2009
;
144
:
314
319
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.04.030.

130.

Trapp
 
W
,
Weinberger
 
M
,
Erk
 
S
 et al.  
A brief intervention utilising visual feedback reduces pain and enhances tactile acuity in CLBP patients
.
J Back Musculoskelet Rehabil
.
2015
;
28
:
651
660
. https://doi.org/10.3233/BMR-140561.

131.

Wälti
 
P
,
Kool
 
J
,
Luomajoki
 
H
.
Short-term effect on pain and function of neurophysiological education and sensorimotor retraining compared to usual physiotherapy in patients with chronic or recurrent non-specific low back pain, a pilot randomized controlled trial rehabilitation, physic
.
BMC Musculoskelet Disord
.
2015
;
16
:
83
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12891-015-0533-2.

132.

Kälin
 
S
,
Rausch-Osthoff
 
A-K
,
Bauer
 
CM
.
What is the effect of sensory discrimination training on chronic losw back pain? A systematic review
.
BMC Musculoskelet Disord
.
2016
;
17
:
143
. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12891-016-0997-8.

133.

Pleger
 
B
,
Tegenthoff
 
M
,
Ragert
 
P
 et al.  
Sensorimotor returning in complex regional pain syndrome parallels pain reduction
.
Ann Neurol
.
2005
;
57
:
425
429
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.20394.

134.

Bowering
 
KJ
,
O’Connell
 
NE
,
Tabor
 
A
 et al.  
The effects of graded motor imagery and its components on chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J Pain
.
2013
;
14
:
3
13
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.09.007.

135.

Moseley
 
GL
,
Butler
 
DS
,
Beames
 
TB
,
Giles
 
TJ
.
The Graded Motor Imagery Handbook
.
Adelaide, Australia
:
Noigroup Publications
;
2012
.

136.

Bray
 
H
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
Disrupted working body schema of the trunk in people with back pain
.
Br J Sports Med
.
2011
;
45
:
168
173
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2009.061978.

137.

Parsons
 
LM
,
Fox
 
PT
,
Downs
 
JH
 et al.  
Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET
.
Nature
.
1995
;
375
:
54
58
. https://doi.org/10.1038/375054a0.

138.

Alazmi
 
L
,
Gadsby
 
GE
,
Heneghan
 
NR
,
Punt
 
TD
.
Do trunk-based left/right judgment tasks elicit motor imagery?
 
Musculoskelet Sci Pract.
 
2018
;
35
:
55
60
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msksp.2018.03.002.

139.

Priganc
 
VW
,
Stralka
 
SW
.
Graded motor imagery
.
J Hand Ther
.
2011
;
24
:
164
169
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jht.2010.11.002.

140.

Lotze
 
M
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
Clinical and neurophysiological effects of progressive movement imagery training for pathological pain
.
J Pain
.
2022
;
23
:
1480
1491
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2022.04.008.

141.

Strauss
 
S
,
Barby
 
S
,
Härtner
 
J
 et al.  
Graded motor imagery modifies movement pain, cortical excitability and sensorimotor function in complex regional pain syndrome
.
Brain Commun
.
2021
;
3
:
fcab216
. https://doi.org/10.1093/BRAINCOMMS/FCAB216.

142.

Strauss
 
S
,
Barby
 
S
,
Härtner
 
J
,
Neumann
 
N
,
Moseley
 
GL
,
Lotze
 
M
.
Modifications in fMRI representation of mental rotation following a 6 week graded motor imagery training in chronic CRPS patients
.
J Pain
.
2021a
;
22
:
680
691
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2020.12.003.

143.

Walz
 
AD
,
Usichenko
 
T
,
Moseley
 
GL
,
Lotze
 
M
.
Graded motor imagery and the impact on pain processing in a case of CRPS
.
Clin J Pain
.
2013
;
29
:
276
279
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e318250f4e8.

144.

Zhang
 
X
,
de
 
Beukelaar
 
TT
,
Possel
 
J
 et al.  
Movement observation improves early consolidation of motor memory
.
J Neurosci
.
2011
;
31
:
11515
11520
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6759-10.2011.

145.

Moseley
 
GL
.
Is successful rehabilitation of complex regional pain syndrome due to sustained attention to the affected limb? A randomised clinical trial
.
Pain
.
2005
;
114
:
54
61
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.11.024.

146.

Elgueta-Cancino
 
E
,
Schabrun
 
S
,
Danneels
 
L
,
Hodges
 
P
.
A clinical test of lumbopelvic control: development and reliability of a clinical test of dissociation of lumbopelvic and thoracolumbar motion
.
Man Ther
.
2014
;
19
:
418
424
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2014.03.009.

147.

Sahrmann
 
S
.
Diagnosis and Treatment of Movement Impairment Syndromes
.
St Louis
:
Mosby Inc
;
2021
.

148.

Macedo
 
LG
,
Latimer
 
J
,
Maher
 
CG
 et al.  
Effect of motor control exercises versus graded activity in patients with chronic nonspecific low back pain: a randomized controlled trial
.
Phys Ther
.
2012
;
92
:363–377. https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20110290.

149.

Tsao
 
H
,
Galea
 
MP
,
Hodges
 
PW
.
Driving plasticity in the motor cortex in recurrent low back pain
.
Eur J Pain
.
2010
;
14
:
832
839
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.01.001.

150.

Boersma
 
K
,
Linton
 
S
,
Overmeer
 
T
,
Jansson
 
M
,
Vlaeyen
 
J
,
De Jong
 
J
.
Lowering fear-avoidance and enhancing function through exposure in vivo: a multiple baseline study across six patients with back pain
.
Pain
.
2004
;
108
:
8
16
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2003.03.001.

151.

George
 
SZ
,
Zeppieri
 
G
,
Cere
 
AL
 et al.  
A randomized trial of behavioral physical therapy interventions for acute and sub-acute low back pain (NCT00373867)
.
Pain
.
2008
;
140
:
145
157
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2008.07.029.

152.

Leeuw
 
M
,
Goossens
 
MEJB
,
van
 
Breukelen
 
GJP
 et al.  
Exposure in vivo versus operant graded activity in chronic low back pain patients: results of a randomized controlled trial
.
Pain
.
2008
;
138
:
192
207
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2007.12.009.

153.

Linton
 
SJ
,
Boersma
 
K
,
Jansson
 
M
,
Overmeer
 
T
,
Lindblom
 
K
,
Vlaeyen
 
JWS
.
A randomized controlled trial of exposure in vivo for patients with spinal pain reporting fear of work-related activities
.
Eur J Pain
.
2008
;
12
:
722
730
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2007.11.001.

154.

Vlaeyen
 
JWS
,
De Jong
 
J
,
Geilen
 
M
,
Heuts
 
PHTG
,
Van Breukelen
 
G
.
Graded exposure in vivo in the treatment of pain-related fear: a replicated single-case experimental design in four patients with chronic low back pain
.
Behav Res Ther
.
2001
;
39
:
151
166
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00174-6.

155.

Wand
 
BM
,
Tulloch
 
VM
,
George
 
PJ
 et al.  
Seeing it helps: movement-related back pain is reduced by visualization of the back during movement
.
Clin J Pain
.
2012
;
28
:
602
608
. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e31823d480c.

156.

O’Sullivan
 
PB
,
Caneiro
 
JP
,
O’Keeffe
 
M
 et al.  
Cognitive functional therapy: an integrated behavioral approach for the targeted management of disabling low back pain
.
Phys Ther
.
2018
;
98
:
408
423
. https://doi.org/10.1093/ptj/pzy022.

157.

Marich
 
AV
,
Lanier
 
VM
,
Salsich
 
GB
,
Lang
 
CE
,
Van Dillen
 
LR
.
Immediate effects of a single session of motor skill training on the lumbar movement pattern during a functional activity in people with low back pain: a repeated-measures study
.
Phys Ther
.
2018
;
98
:
605
615
. https://doi.org/10.1093/ptj/pzy044.

158.

Sahrmann
 
S
,
Azevedo
 
DC
,
Van Dillen
 
L
.
Diagnosis and treatment of movement system impairment syndromes
.
Brazilian J Phys Ther
.
2017
;
21
:
391
399
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bjpt.2017.08.001.

159.

Van Dillen
 
LR
,
Lanier
 
VM
,
Steger-May
 
K
 et al.  
Effect of motor skill training in functional activities vs strength and flexibility exercise on function in people with chronic low back pain: a randomized clinical trial
.
JAMA Neurol
.
2021
;
78
:
385
395
. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.4821.

160.

Alemanno
 
F
,
Houdayer
 
E
,
Emedoli
 
D
 et al.  
Efficacy of virtual reality to reduce chronic low back pain: proof-of-concept of a non-pharmacological approach on pain, quality of life, neuropsychological and functional outcome
.
PLoS One
.
2019
;
14
:
e0216858
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216858.

161.

Harvie
 
DS
.
Immersive education for chronic condition self-management
.
Front Virtual Real
.
2021
;
2
:657761. https://doi.org/10.3389/frvir.2021.657761.

162.

Khan
 
KM
,
Scott
 
A
.
Mechanotherapy: how physical therapists’ prescription of exercise promotes tissue repair
.
Br J Sports Med
.
2009
;
43
:
247
252
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2008.054239.

163.

Steiger
 
F
,
Wirth
 
B
,
De Bruin
 
ED
,
Mannion
 
AF
.
Is a positive clinical outcome after exercise therapy for chronic non-specific low back pain contingent upon a corresponding improvement in the targeted aspect (s) of performance? A systematic review
.
Eur Spine J
.
2012
;
21
:
575
598
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-011-2045-6.

164.

Lee Mansell
 
G
,
McAuley
 
JH
,
Kamper
 
SJ
 et al.  
Causal mechanisms in the clinical course and treatment of back pain
.
Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol
.
2016
;
30
:
1074
1083
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.berh.2017.04.001.

165.

Cashin
 
AG
,
Booth
 
J
,
McAuley
 
JH
 et al.  
Making exercise count: considerations for the role of exercise in back pain treatment
.
Musculoskeletal Care
.
2021
;
20
:
259
270
. https://doi.org/10.1002/msc.1597.

166.

McGill
 
S
.
Low Back Disorders. Evidence-Based Prevention and Rehabilitation
. 3rd ed.
Champaign, IL
:
Human Kinetics
;
2015
.

167.

Catley
 
MJ
,
O’Connell
 
NE
,
Moseley
 
GL
.
How good is the neurophysiology of Pain Questionnaire? A Rasch analysis of psychometric properties
.
J Pain
.
2013
;
14
:
818
827
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2013.02.008.

168.

Travers
 
M
,
Gibson
 
W
,
Darlow
 
B
, et al.  My back is fragile and it is never getting better (The fremantle danger in Me Questionnaire). In:
Australian Physiotherapy Association Conference: Transform
.
Adelaide, Australia
: Australian Physiotherapy Association;
2019
.

169.

Cashin
 
AG
,
McAuley
 
JH
.
Measuring two-point discrimination threshold with a caliper
.
J. Physiother.
 
2017
;
63
:
186
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphys.2017.04.005.

170.

Cashin
 
AG
,
Lee
 
H
,
Bagg
 
MK
 et al.  
Investigating the mechanisms of graded sensorimotor precision training in adults with chronic nonspecific low back pain: protocol for a causal mediation analysis of the RESOLVE Trial
.
JMIR Res Protoc
.
2021
;
10
:
e26053
. https://doi.org/10.2196/26053.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)

Comments

0 Comments
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.