-
PDF
- Split View
-
Views
-
Cite
Cite
Angela Fang, Bengi Baran, Clare C Beatty, Jennifer Mosley, Jamie D Feusner, K Luan Phan, Sabine Wilhelm, Dara S Manoach, Maladaptive self-focused attention and default mode network connectivity: a transdiagnostic investigation across social anxiety and body dysmorphic disorders, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Volume 17, Issue 7, July 2022, Pages 645–654, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab130
- Share Icon Share
Abstract
Maladaptive self-focused attention (SFA) is a bias toward internal thoughts, feelings and physical states. Despite its role as a core maintaining factor of symptoms in cognitive theories of social anxiety and body dysmorphic disorders (BDDs), studies have not examined its neural basis. In this study, we hypothesized that maladaptive SFA would be associated with hyperconnectivity in the default mode network (DMN) in self-focused patients with these disorders. Thirty patients with primary social anxiety disorder or primary BDD and 28 healthy individuals were eligible and scanned. Eligibility was determined by scoring greater than 1SD or below 1SD of the Public Self-Consciousness Scale normative mean, respectively, for each group. Seed-to-voxel functional connectivity was computed using a DMN posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) seed. There was no evidence of increased DMN functional connectivity in patients compared to controls. Patients (regardless of diagnosis) showed reduced functional connectivity of the PCC with several brain regions, including the bilateral superior parietal lobule (SPL), compared to controls, which was inversely correlated with maladaptive SFA but not associated with social anxiety, body dysmorphic, depression severity or rumination. Abnormal PCC-SPL connectivity may represent a transdiagnostic neural marker of SFA that reflects difficulty shifting between internal versus external attention.
Introduction
Self-focused attention (SFA) is a form of self-referential thought that falls along a dimension, with an adaptive ‘experiential’ mode of processing on one end and a maladaptive ‘analytical’ mode on the other (Watkins, 2004). Maladaptive SFA is a dispositional tendency to reflect excessively about one’s internal thoughts, feelings, beliefs and physical states that interferes with attention to the external environment (Ingram, 1990). Neurocognitive models of psychiatric disorders propose that attentional biases, such as maladaptive SFA, lead to negatively biased self-referential thoughts, which increase negative mood (e.g. anxiety, depression), and maladaptive behaviors (e.g. avoidance, safety seeking behaviors) (Ingram, 1990; Clark and Wells, 1995; Watkins, 2004). These attentional biases may reflect disrupted top-down attentional control or bottom-up attentional orienting mechanisms (Sylvester et al., 2012; Heeren et al., 2013). Transdiagnostically, maladaptive SFA conceptually overlaps with ruminations in depression (Watkins and Teasdale, 2004), obsessions in obsessive-compulsive disorder (Exner et al., 2009), worry in generalized anxiety disorder (Mor and Winquist, 2002), anxiety sensitivity in panic disorder (Schmidt et al., 1997) and somatic pre-occupations in eating disorders (Zucker et al., 2015). As research on SFA has been largely disorder-specific, studies have not tested whether maladaptive SFA extends across disorders, and the neural correlates of maladaptive SFA remain unknown. In this study, we tested whether maladaptive SFA would be associated with hyperconnectivity within the default mode network (DMN) across two self-focused psychiatric disorders.
Functional neuroimaging studies suggest that SFA is associated with activity in a large-scale, intrinsic brain network that mediates internal mentation: the DMN (Gentili et al., 2009; Grimm et al., 2009; Freton et al., 2014). The DMN comprises several brain regions that form an integrated system for self-related cognition, including the medial pre-frontal cortex (mPFC) and portions of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex (PCu/PCC), angular gyrus, lateral parietal cortex and medial temporal cortex (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Raichle et al., 2001; Buckner et al., 2008; Raichle, 2015; Margulies et al., 2016). Among these regions, the mPFC and the PCC display consistent patterns of activity across a variety of internally directed tasks and processes (Kelley et al., 2002; Moran et al., 2006; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2011). Brain regions within the DMN are commonly deactivated during tasks requiring external attention, a phenomenon known as task deactivation or task suppression (Gusnard and Raichle, 2001), which suggests that orienting attention between internal and external environments requires balance between the DMN and other networks (Fox et al., 2005).
A growing body of literature using task and resting state fMRI in ‘self-focused’ clinical and non-clinical populations implicate abnormalities in the DMN (Broyd et al., 2009; Kaiser et al., 2015; Gürsel et al., 2018). Task-based studies have shown both heightened activation, as well as impaired deactivation, of DMN regions. For example, one study that employed a task manipulating inward versus outward focus of attention while viewing video clips of a conversation partner found that maladaptive SFA was associated with increased activation in the mPFC, temporo-parietal junction and temporal pole, in a sample of individuals with high socially anxious symptoms (Boehme et al., 2015). In contrast, a study in body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) found reduced DMN deactivation in the dorsal mPFC during a house-viewing task, which may be associated with a reduced ability to disengage the DMN during the task due to self-referential thinking (Feusner et al., 2011). Impaired deactivation of the DMN during error processing has also been demonstrated in a study in obsessive-compulsive disorder (Agam et al., 2014), which may be suggestive of persistent self-referential processing after making errors. Studies examining resting state functional connectivity in patients with social anxiety disorder (SAD) have shown both increased and decreased functional connectivity within the DMN, in patients compared to controls (Liao et al., 2010; Qiu et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2015; Geiger et al., 2016), as well as one study reporting no group differences in a small sample of drug-naïve, non-comorbid individuals (Pannekoek et al., 2013). Although SAD is associated with high levels of maladaptive SFA (Hofmann, 2007), these studies did not explicitly link DMN connectivity with SFA. Furthermore, inconsistent findings may be due to methodological differences in resting state protocols and data processing procedures (especially in addressing motion-related confounds), as well as sample differences in symptoms and severity in psychiatric disorders. More research is needed to examine the extent to which maladaptive SFA is associated with DMN disruptions in clinical populations.
We addressed limitations of the prior literature by employing a novel sampling strategy and taking rigorous steps to account for motion-related confounds in functional connectivity. First, we recruited individuals based on self-reported levels of maladaptive SFA: patients with either SAD or BDD who scored above a cutoff on the Public Self-Consciousness Scale, a measure of trait SFA (Scheier and Carver, 1985), and healthy individuals who scored below the cutoff. We selected these two related disorders on the anxiety and obsessive-compulsive spectrum for the following reasons: (1) maladaptive SFA is proposed to be a major maintaining factor of symptoms in both disorders (Clark and Wells, 1995; Veale, 2004); (2) both disorders share a core fear of negative evaluation by others, which activates maladaptive SFA and results in social avoidance (Fang and Hofmann, 2010); and (3) a majority of the literature on SFA has focused on depression. These studies tend to emphasize the ruminative aspects of SFA in depression, as compared to the social presentation and self-consciousness aspects of SFA more relevant in anxiety and obsessive-compulsive related disorders (Clark and Wells, 1995; Watkins, 2004; Grimm et al., 2009). Second, we rigorously controlled for the confounding effects of head motion in functional connectivity studies by training participants to remain still during mock scan sessions and by procedures implemented during both data acquisition and post-processing (described below). We hypothesized that patients with maladaptive SFA (across both SAD and BDD diagnoses) would display greater resting state functional connectivity within the DMN, as compared to controls. We also predicted that connectivity within the DMN would correlate more strongly with self-reported trait maladaptive SFA (as compared to related constructs, such as depression severity and rumination) across both disorders, given the transdiagnostic nature of maladaptive SFA.
Methods and materials
Participants
Patients were treatment-seeking individuals recruited from outpatient psychiatry specialty clinics at the Massachusetts General Hospital and healthy individuals were recruited from the community through flyers and online advertisements. A total of 89 potentially eligible participants, who met prescreening criteria, consented to the study. Participants were recruited on the basis of their self-reported levels of trait maladaptive SFA, as measured by the Public Self-Consciousness Scale-Revised (SCS-R) (Scheier and Carver, 1985). Included patients scored ≥18 on this measure, whereas healthy controls were required to score ≤9, which reflects ± 1SD of the normative mean, respectively. There are seven items in this measure, scored on a 0 (not at all like me) to 3 (a lot like me) Likert scale. Example items include: I care a lot about how I present myself to others, I’m self-conscious about the way I look, and I usually worry about making a good impression. The Public SCS-R was given during pre-screening prior to enrollment, as well as during an in-person screening evaluation to confirm trait levels of SFA. Patients also had to meet cutoff scores reflecting at least moderate levels of SAD or BDD symptoms (≥50 on the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale [LSAS] (Liebowitz, 1987) for those with primary SAD, and ≥20 on the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale Modified for BDD [BDD-YBOCS] (Phillips et al., 1997) for those with primary BDD). Diagnoses were evaluated using the SCID-5 (First et al., 2015). Low-self-focused healthy individuals had no lifetime history of psychiatric disorders. Exclusion criteria for all participants included: (i) MR contraindications (e.g. claustrophobia, metal in body, pregnancy), (ii) history of head injury, neurological disorder or neurosurgical procedure, (iii) active suicidal or homicidal ideation, (iv) lifetime manic, hypomanic or psychotic episode, (v) past year alcohol or substance dependence or (vi) current or past cognitive behavioral therapy or formal mindfulness/meditation training, defined by >10 sessions. Participants were matched on a one-to-one basis across patient and control groups, such that a control participant was matched to each enrolled patient based on sex and within a certain range based on age (±2 years) and education (±2 years).
After the screening evaluation, 25 were ineligible for the following reasons: no longer met criteria on the SCS-R score (n = 4 patients, n = 5 controls), not meeting criteria for primary SAD or primary BDD (n = 9 patients), current or past cognitive behavioral therapy or formal meditation (n = 4 patients), trying to get pregnant (n = 1 patient), history of alcohol use disorder (n = 1 control) and inadequate match with a clinical patient (n = 1 control). An additional six participants withdrew prior to completing the fMRI scan due to time commitment (n = 2 patients, n = 1 control), lost to follow-up (n = 2 controls) and moved out of state (n = 1 control). This left a final sample of 30 patients (15 with a primary diagnosis of SAD, 15 with a primary diagnosis of BDD) and 28 age-, sex- and education-matched healthy individuals.
Procedures
Participants attended an initial evaluation, which involved a comprehensive diagnostic interview using the SCID-5, clinician-administered symptom assessments (e.g. LSAS, BDD-YBOCS and Brown Assessment of Beliefs Scale [BABS] (Eisen et al., 1998) to assess levels of insight) and self-report measures of depression severity using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (Beck et al., 1996), rumination using the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS) (Treynor et al., 2003), handedness using the Edinburgh Handedness Scale (Oldfield, 1971) and estimated verbal IQ using the Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (WRAT-4) (Wilkinson and Robertson, 2006). Eligible participants returned for an MRI scan between 7 and 10 days of the initial evaluation. The study was approved by the Partners Human Research Committee and all participants provided written informed consent.
MRI acquisition
Images were acquired with a 3T Siemens Prisma scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, Iselin, NJ) equipped for echo planar imaging and a 64-channel head coil. Participants who had never undergone MRI scans completed a mock scan on a separate day or just prior to their actual scan to acclimate them to the scanning environment and to train them to remain still, with the exception of one patient with BDD and two healthy controls. During scanning, head movements were restricted using foam cushions. A high-resolution anatomical scan was acquired using a T1-weighted 3D multiecho magnetization-prepared rf-spoiled rapid gradient-echo MEMPRAGE sequence with EPI-based volumetric navigators for prospective motion correction and selective reacquisition (Tisdall et al., 2012) (TR = 2530 ms, flip angle = 7°, TEs = 1.7 ms/3.6 ms/5.4 ms/7.3 ms, iPAT = 2, FOV = 256 mm, 176 in-plane sagittal slices; voxel size = 1mm3 isotropic, scan duration 5 min 34 s). Two resting state functional connectivity MRI scans were acquired with a T2*-weighted gradient echo sequence for blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast (TR = 2000 ms, flip angle = 85°, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 205 mm, 32 continuous horizontal slices parallel to the intercommissural plane, voxel size = 3.2 × 3.2 × 3.3 mm, interleaved, scan duration 6 min 6 s). The resting state sequences included prospective acquisition correction for head motion to adjust slice position and orientation during image acquisition (Thesen et al., 2000). Participants were instructed to keep their eyes open for the duration of the scan.
MRI pre-processing and data quality
Automated pre-processing steps were performed using the CONN toolbox version 17 (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012), which employs routines from the Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM8; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK). The first four volumes of each resting state run were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration. Images were segmented into gray matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid masks, corrected for slice timing and spatially realigned to the reference image. The volumes were then normalized to the MNI template provided in SPM8, resampled to 2 mm voxels and spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel with a full width at half-maximum of 8 mm. A temporal band-pass filter of 0.008–0.09 Hz was applied to the time series. Nuisance variables (white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, movement parameters) were addressed using the anatomical CompCor method (Behzadi et al., 2007), which uses a component base noise reduction method, rather than global signal regression (Chai et al., 2012), to reduce physiological and other noise. Artifactual volumes were identified using the Artifact Detection Tools (ART; http://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/) and defined as volumes with > 0.9 mm head displacement in the x, y or z directions or global mean intensity more than five SDs above the entire scan. Residual head motion parameters (rotation and translations in x, y and z directions and their first-order temporal derivatives, calculated using Power et al.’s (2012) measure of framewise displacement) and artifactual volumes (flagged by ART) were regressed out in the model. There were no group differences in residual head motion, number of artifactual volumes or proportion of artifactual volumes to total number of volumes, in either resting state run (Table 1).
Demographic characteristics . | Clinical (n = 30) Mean ± SD/n (%) . | Control (n = 28) Mean ± SD/n (%) . | t/χ2 . | P . | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 25.43 ± 3.76 | 25.71 ± 3.89 | 0.28 | 0.78 | |
WRAT-4 | 71.50 ± 22.57 | 74.04 ± 19.08 | 0.46 | 0.65 | |
Sex | 0.20 | 0.65 | |||
Female | 23 (76.7) | 20 (71.4) | |||
Male | 7 (23.3) | 8 (28.6) | |||
Race | 1.65 | 0.65 | |||
Caucasian | 19 (63.3) | 18 (64.3) | |||
Asian | 8 (26.7) | 5 (17.9) | |||
Black/African American | 2 (6.7) | 2 (7.1) | |||
More than one race | 1 (3.3) | 3 (10.7) | |||
Ethnicity | 0.09 | 0.76 | |||
Non-Hispanic or Latino | 26 (86.7) | 25 (89.3) | |||
Hispanic or Latino | 4 (13.3) | 3 (10.7) | |||
Unspecified | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | |||
Highest Level of Education | 2.96 | 0.40 | |||
Some College | 3 (10.0) | 0 (0.0) | |||
College Graduate | 17 (56.7) | 18 (64.3) | |||
Some Postgraduate | 1 (3.3) | 1 (3.6) | |||
Postgraduate/Professional Degree | 9 (30.0) | 9 (32.1) | |||
Household Income | 7.14 | 0.41 | |||
Not given | 4 | 2 | |||
$5000–9999 | 0 | 1 | |||
$10 000–14 999 | 0 | 2 | |||
$15 000–24 999 | 4 | 1 | |||
$25 000–39 999 | 4 | 6 | |||
$35 000–49 999 | 10 | 6 | |||
$50 000–74 999 | 3 | 3 | |||
>$75 000 | 5 | 7 | |||
Occupational Status | 1.93 | 0.59 | |||
Full-time employment | 17 | 17 | |||
Part-time employment | 1 | 1 | |||
Student | 10 | 10 | |||
Unemployed | 2 | 0 | |||
Handedness | 77.83 (33.21) | 86.43 (17.84) | −1.22 | 0.23 | |
Head motion | |||||
Resting state run 1 | 0.16 ± 0.11 | 0.15 ± 0.05 | −0.19 | 0.85 | |
Resting state run 2 | 0.18 ± 0.09 | 0.20 ± 0.09 | 0.95 | 0.35 | |
Number of artifactual volumes (% of total volumes) | |||||
Resting state run 1 | 9.57 (5.41%) | 8.00 (4.52%) | −0.71 | 0.48 | |
Resting state run 2 | 7.63 (4.31%) | 6.92 (3.91%) | −0.60 | 0.55 | |
Clinical characteristics | BDD (n = 15) Mean ± SD/n (%) | SAD (n = 15) Mean ± SD/n (%) | ta | P | |
Age of onset (years) | 14.20 ± 2.62 | 13.20 ± 3.21 | – | 0.93 | 0.36 |
BDI-II | 15.53 ±12.51 | 17.00 ± 14.65 | 0.39 (0.69) | 0.30 | 0.77 |
LSAS | – | 80.27 ± 16.04 | – | – | – |
BDD-YBOCS | 30.20 ± 6.57 | – | – | – | – |
BABS | 14.80 ± 4.36 | 14.33 ± 2.90 | – | 0.35 | 0.73 |
Public SCS-R | 20.07 ± 1.10 | 20.07 ± 1.28 | 4.57 (2.43) | 0.00 | 1.00 |
RRS | 53.13 ± 13.97 | 53.33 ± 16.42 | 28.63 (8.01) | 0.04 | 0.97 |
Demographic characteristics . | Clinical (n = 30) Mean ± SD/n (%) . | Control (n = 28) Mean ± SD/n (%) . | t/χ2 . | P . | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 25.43 ± 3.76 | 25.71 ± 3.89 | 0.28 | 0.78 | |
WRAT-4 | 71.50 ± 22.57 | 74.04 ± 19.08 | 0.46 | 0.65 | |
Sex | 0.20 | 0.65 | |||
Female | 23 (76.7) | 20 (71.4) | |||
Male | 7 (23.3) | 8 (28.6) | |||
Race | 1.65 | 0.65 | |||
Caucasian | 19 (63.3) | 18 (64.3) | |||
Asian | 8 (26.7) | 5 (17.9) | |||
Black/African American | 2 (6.7) | 2 (7.1) | |||
More than one race | 1 (3.3) | 3 (10.7) | |||
Ethnicity | 0.09 | 0.76 | |||
Non-Hispanic or Latino | 26 (86.7) | 25 (89.3) | |||
Hispanic or Latino | 4 (13.3) | 3 (10.7) | |||
Unspecified | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | |||
Highest Level of Education | 2.96 | 0.40 | |||
Some College | 3 (10.0) | 0 (0.0) | |||
College Graduate | 17 (56.7) | 18 (64.3) | |||
Some Postgraduate | 1 (3.3) | 1 (3.6) | |||
Postgraduate/Professional Degree | 9 (30.0) | 9 (32.1) | |||
Household Income | 7.14 | 0.41 | |||
Not given | 4 | 2 | |||
$5000–9999 | 0 | 1 | |||
$10 000–14 999 | 0 | 2 | |||
$15 000–24 999 | 4 | 1 | |||
$25 000–39 999 | 4 | 6 | |||
$35 000–49 999 | 10 | 6 | |||
$50 000–74 999 | 3 | 3 | |||
>$75 000 | 5 | 7 | |||
Occupational Status | 1.93 | 0.59 | |||
Full-time employment | 17 | 17 | |||
Part-time employment | 1 | 1 | |||
Student | 10 | 10 | |||
Unemployed | 2 | 0 | |||
Handedness | 77.83 (33.21) | 86.43 (17.84) | −1.22 | 0.23 | |
Head motion | |||||
Resting state run 1 | 0.16 ± 0.11 | 0.15 ± 0.05 | −0.19 | 0.85 | |
Resting state run 2 | 0.18 ± 0.09 | 0.20 ± 0.09 | 0.95 | 0.35 | |
Number of artifactual volumes (% of total volumes) | |||||
Resting state run 1 | 9.57 (5.41%) | 8.00 (4.52%) | −0.71 | 0.48 | |
Resting state run 2 | 7.63 (4.31%) | 6.92 (3.91%) | −0.60 | 0.55 | |
Clinical characteristics | BDD (n = 15) Mean ± SD/n (%) | SAD (n = 15) Mean ± SD/n (%) | ta | P | |
Age of onset (years) | 14.20 ± 2.62 | 13.20 ± 3.21 | – | 0.93 | 0.36 |
BDI-II | 15.53 ±12.51 | 17.00 ± 14.65 | 0.39 (0.69) | 0.30 | 0.77 |
LSAS | – | 80.27 ± 16.04 | – | – | – |
BDD-YBOCS | 30.20 ± 6.57 | – | – | – | – |
BABS | 14.80 ± 4.36 | 14.33 ± 2.90 | – | 0.35 | 0.73 |
Public SCS-R | 20.07 ± 1.10 | 20.07 ± 1.28 | 4.57 (2.43) | 0.00 | 1.00 |
RRS | 53.13 ± 13.97 | 53.33 ± 16.42 | 28.63 (8.01) | 0.04 | 0.97 |
WRAT-4 = Wide Range Achievement Test-4. Score reflects word reading subtest standard score. Head motion based on Power et al. (2012) measure of framewise displacement. BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II. LSAS = Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. BDD-YBOCS = Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale Modified for BDD. BABS = Brown Assessment of Beliefs Scale. Public SCS-R = Public Subscale of Self-Consciousness Scale-Revised. RRS = Ruminative Responses Scale.
t-Statistics reflect differences between social anxiety disorder (SAD) and body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) groups.
Demographic characteristics . | Clinical (n = 30) Mean ± SD/n (%) . | Control (n = 28) Mean ± SD/n (%) . | t/χ2 . | P . | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 25.43 ± 3.76 | 25.71 ± 3.89 | 0.28 | 0.78 | |
WRAT-4 | 71.50 ± 22.57 | 74.04 ± 19.08 | 0.46 | 0.65 | |
Sex | 0.20 | 0.65 | |||
Female | 23 (76.7) | 20 (71.4) | |||
Male | 7 (23.3) | 8 (28.6) | |||
Race | 1.65 | 0.65 | |||
Caucasian | 19 (63.3) | 18 (64.3) | |||
Asian | 8 (26.7) | 5 (17.9) | |||
Black/African American | 2 (6.7) | 2 (7.1) | |||
More than one race | 1 (3.3) | 3 (10.7) | |||
Ethnicity | 0.09 | 0.76 | |||
Non-Hispanic or Latino | 26 (86.7) | 25 (89.3) | |||
Hispanic or Latino | 4 (13.3) | 3 (10.7) | |||
Unspecified | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | |||
Highest Level of Education | 2.96 | 0.40 | |||
Some College | 3 (10.0) | 0 (0.0) | |||
College Graduate | 17 (56.7) | 18 (64.3) | |||
Some Postgraduate | 1 (3.3) | 1 (3.6) | |||
Postgraduate/Professional Degree | 9 (30.0) | 9 (32.1) | |||
Household Income | 7.14 | 0.41 | |||
Not given | 4 | 2 | |||
$5000–9999 | 0 | 1 | |||
$10 000–14 999 | 0 | 2 | |||
$15 000–24 999 | 4 | 1 | |||
$25 000–39 999 | 4 | 6 | |||
$35 000–49 999 | 10 | 6 | |||
$50 000–74 999 | 3 | 3 | |||
>$75 000 | 5 | 7 | |||
Occupational Status | 1.93 | 0.59 | |||
Full-time employment | 17 | 17 | |||
Part-time employment | 1 | 1 | |||
Student | 10 | 10 | |||
Unemployed | 2 | 0 | |||
Handedness | 77.83 (33.21) | 86.43 (17.84) | −1.22 | 0.23 | |
Head motion | |||||
Resting state run 1 | 0.16 ± 0.11 | 0.15 ± 0.05 | −0.19 | 0.85 | |
Resting state run 2 | 0.18 ± 0.09 | 0.20 ± 0.09 | 0.95 | 0.35 | |
Number of artifactual volumes (% of total volumes) | |||||
Resting state run 1 | 9.57 (5.41%) | 8.00 (4.52%) | −0.71 | 0.48 | |
Resting state run 2 | 7.63 (4.31%) | 6.92 (3.91%) | −0.60 | 0.55 | |
Clinical characteristics | BDD (n = 15) Mean ± SD/n (%) | SAD (n = 15) Mean ± SD/n (%) | ta | P | |
Age of onset (years) | 14.20 ± 2.62 | 13.20 ± 3.21 | – | 0.93 | 0.36 |
BDI-II | 15.53 ±12.51 | 17.00 ± 14.65 | 0.39 (0.69) | 0.30 | 0.77 |
LSAS | – | 80.27 ± 16.04 | – | – | – |
BDD-YBOCS | 30.20 ± 6.57 | – | – | – | – |
BABS | 14.80 ± 4.36 | 14.33 ± 2.90 | – | 0.35 | 0.73 |
Public SCS-R | 20.07 ± 1.10 | 20.07 ± 1.28 | 4.57 (2.43) | 0.00 | 1.00 |
RRS | 53.13 ± 13.97 | 53.33 ± 16.42 | 28.63 (8.01) | 0.04 | 0.97 |
Demographic characteristics . | Clinical (n = 30) Mean ± SD/n (%) . | Control (n = 28) Mean ± SD/n (%) . | t/χ2 . | P . | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 25.43 ± 3.76 | 25.71 ± 3.89 | 0.28 | 0.78 | |
WRAT-4 | 71.50 ± 22.57 | 74.04 ± 19.08 | 0.46 | 0.65 | |
Sex | 0.20 | 0.65 | |||
Female | 23 (76.7) | 20 (71.4) | |||
Male | 7 (23.3) | 8 (28.6) | |||
Race | 1.65 | 0.65 | |||
Caucasian | 19 (63.3) | 18 (64.3) | |||
Asian | 8 (26.7) | 5 (17.9) | |||
Black/African American | 2 (6.7) | 2 (7.1) | |||
More than one race | 1 (3.3) | 3 (10.7) | |||
Ethnicity | 0.09 | 0.76 | |||
Non-Hispanic or Latino | 26 (86.7) | 25 (89.3) | |||
Hispanic or Latino | 4 (13.3) | 3 (10.7) | |||
Unspecified | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | |||
Highest Level of Education | 2.96 | 0.40 | |||
Some College | 3 (10.0) | 0 (0.0) | |||
College Graduate | 17 (56.7) | 18 (64.3) | |||
Some Postgraduate | 1 (3.3) | 1 (3.6) | |||
Postgraduate/Professional Degree | 9 (30.0) | 9 (32.1) | |||
Household Income | 7.14 | 0.41 | |||
Not given | 4 | 2 | |||
$5000–9999 | 0 | 1 | |||
$10 000–14 999 | 0 | 2 | |||
$15 000–24 999 | 4 | 1 | |||
$25 000–39 999 | 4 | 6 | |||
$35 000–49 999 | 10 | 6 | |||
$50 000–74 999 | 3 | 3 | |||
>$75 000 | 5 | 7 | |||
Occupational Status | 1.93 | 0.59 | |||
Full-time employment | 17 | 17 | |||
Part-time employment | 1 | 1 | |||
Student | 10 | 10 | |||
Unemployed | 2 | 0 | |||
Handedness | 77.83 (33.21) | 86.43 (17.84) | −1.22 | 0.23 | |
Head motion | |||||
Resting state run 1 | 0.16 ± 0.11 | 0.15 ± 0.05 | −0.19 | 0.85 | |
Resting state run 2 | 0.18 ± 0.09 | 0.20 ± 0.09 | 0.95 | 0.35 | |
Number of artifactual volumes (% of total volumes) | |||||
Resting state run 1 | 9.57 (5.41%) | 8.00 (4.52%) | −0.71 | 0.48 | |
Resting state run 2 | 7.63 (4.31%) | 6.92 (3.91%) | −0.60 | 0.55 | |
Clinical characteristics | BDD (n = 15) Mean ± SD/n (%) | SAD (n = 15) Mean ± SD/n (%) | ta | P | |
Age of onset (years) | 14.20 ± 2.62 | 13.20 ± 3.21 | – | 0.93 | 0.36 |
BDI-II | 15.53 ±12.51 | 17.00 ± 14.65 | 0.39 (0.69) | 0.30 | 0.77 |
LSAS | – | 80.27 ± 16.04 | – | – | – |
BDD-YBOCS | 30.20 ± 6.57 | – | – | – | – |
BABS | 14.80 ± 4.36 | 14.33 ± 2.90 | – | 0.35 | 0.73 |
Public SCS-R | 20.07 ± 1.10 | 20.07 ± 1.28 | 4.57 (2.43) | 0.00 | 1.00 |
RRS | 53.13 ± 13.97 | 53.33 ± 16.42 | 28.63 (8.01) | 0.04 | 0.97 |
WRAT-4 = Wide Range Achievement Test-4. Score reflects word reading subtest standard score. Head motion based on Power et al. (2012) measure of framewise displacement. BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II. LSAS = Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. BDD-YBOCS = Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale Modified for BDD. BABS = Brown Assessment of Beliefs Scale. Public SCS-R = Public Subscale of Self-Consciousness Scale-Revised. RRS = Ruminative Responses Scale.
t-Statistics reflect differences between social anxiety disorder (SAD) and body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) groups.
Functional connectivity analyses
Seed-to-voxel functional connectivity analyses of the DMN were conducted using the canonical DMN seed, the PCC, provided by the CONN toolbox (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012). First level functional connectivity maps were generated by extracting the average BOLD time series from the PCC seed and correlating it with every gray matter voxel in the whole brain. Correlation values were then transformed using Fisher’s z transformation to yield a map for each resting state run in which the value at each voxel represented connectivity with the PCC seed. Two resting state runs for each subject were averaged. We examined group differences in DMN connectivity with t-tests at every voxel. Whole brain correction for multiple comparisons was applied using a voxel level uncorrected threshold of P < 0.001 and a false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected cluster threshold of P < 0.05.
In the patient group, we conducted correlation analyses to test the relations between DMN connectivity in regions that significantly differed between groups in the whole brain and trait maladaptive SFA (Public SCS-R) and other clinical variables (depression severity, rumination, social anxiety severity and body dysmorphic severity). To determine whether correlations with trait maladaptive SFA were significantly stronger than correlations with other clinical variables, we used Steiger’s calculation for testing the equality of two correlation coefficients measured on the same individuals (Steiger, 1980; Lee and Preacher, 2013). As our a priori hypotheses were that DMN connectivity measures would show a stronger association with trait maladaptive SFA compared to other clinical variables, these analyses were tested at P < 0.05.
Results
Sample characteristics
As shown in Table 1, there were no differences between patients and controls in age, sex, race, ethnicity, education, handedness and estimated verbal IQ. There were also no differences between patients with SAD and BDD across these characteristics, except age. Those with SAD were significantly older than those with BDD (SAD: M = 27.00, SD = 4.41; BDD: M = 23.87, SD = 2.13; t = −2.48, P = 0.02). Five patients were taking the following medications at a stable dose for at least two months prior to study enrollment: escitalopram (n = 1, SAD), citalopram (n = 3, BDD) and amphetamine (n = 1, BDD). Full sample details regarding psychiatric comorbidities are described in supplementary materials (Supplementary Table S1).
Abnormal DMN functional connectivity in self-focused patients with SAD and BDD
Contrary to our hypothesis, no regions showed significantly greater functional connectivity within the DMN in patients versus controls. Compared with healthy controls, patients exhibited reduced functional connectivity between the PCC and a set of cortical regions, including the right cingulate cortex, bilateral superior parietal lobule (SPL), left post-central gyrus and bilateral insula (Table 2; Figure 1A). None of these regions were within the DMN. To determine whether there was significantly increased or decreased connectivity in the patient group, we conducted one-sample t-tests for PCC connectivity to each of the six brain regions (Table 2) in each group separately. For PCC connectivity to the two bilateral SPL clusters, there was significantly decreased connectivity in the patient group (significantly less than 0), whereas controls showed significantly increased connectivity (significantly greater than 0). Patients also showed the same pattern of decreased connectivity (significantly less than 0) in all of the remaining clusters, except for PCC connectivity to the left post-central gyrus cluster, which was not significantly different from 0. Controls showed significantly increased connectivity of the PCC to all of these brain regions, except for the right cingulate cortex and bilateral insula, which were not significantly different from 0. Figure 2 shows unthresholded statistical maps of PCC functional connectivity.
Clusters showing significant group differences in functional connectivity with the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)
MNI coordinates | ||||||
Region | Voxel size | x | y | z | BA | t(56) |
R cingulate cortex | 214 | 8 | 2 | 44 | 24 | 4.72 |
R superior parietal lobule/postcentral gyrus | 207 | 12 | −48 | 70 | 7 | 4.11 |
L postcentral gyrus | 185 | −42 | −20 | 32 | 2 | 4.72 |
L postcentral gyrus/superior parietal lobule | 152 | −14 | −44 | 60 | 7 | 4.20 |
R anterior insula/central operculum | 150 | 44 | −4 | 2 | 13 | 4.42 |
L insula/putamen | 115 | −34 | −12 | 4 | – | 4.71 |
MNI coordinates | ||||||
Region | Voxel size | x | y | z | BA | t(56) |
R cingulate cortex | 214 | 8 | 2 | 44 | 24 | 4.72 |
R superior parietal lobule/postcentral gyrus | 207 | 12 | −48 | 70 | 7 | 4.11 |
L postcentral gyrus | 185 | −42 | −20 | 32 | 2 | 4.72 |
L postcentral gyrus/superior parietal lobule | 152 | −14 | −44 | 60 | 7 | 4.20 |
R anterior insula/central operculum | 150 | 44 | −4 | 2 | 13 | 4.42 |
L insula/putamen | 115 | −34 | −12 | 4 | – | 4.71 |
All reported clusters are significant at PFDR-corrected <0.05, two-sided, based on whole-brain correction. There were no clusters in which patients showed significantly greater functional connectivity than controls. Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates and t-statistics are given for peak voxel location.
Clusters showing significant group differences in functional connectivity with the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)
MNI coordinates | ||||||
Region | Voxel size | x | y | z | BA | t(56) |
R cingulate cortex | 214 | 8 | 2 | 44 | 24 | 4.72 |
R superior parietal lobule/postcentral gyrus | 207 | 12 | −48 | 70 | 7 | 4.11 |
L postcentral gyrus | 185 | −42 | −20 | 32 | 2 | 4.72 |
L postcentral gyrus/superior parietal lobule | 152 | −14 | −44 | 60 | 7 | 4.20 |
R anterior insula/central operculum | 150 | 44 | −4 | 2 | 13 | 4.42 |
L insula/putamen | 115 | −34 | −12 | 4 | – | 4.71 |
MNI coordinates | ||||||
Region | Voxel size | x | y | z | BA | t(56) |
R cingulate cortex | 214 | 8 | 2 | 44 | 24 | 4.72 |
R superior parietal lobule/postcentral gyrus | 207 | 12 | −48 | 70 | 7 | 4.11 |
L postcentral gyrus | 185 | −42 | −20 | 32 | 2 | 4.72 |
L postcentral gyrus/superior parietal lobule | 152 | −14 | −44 | 60 | 7 | 4.20 |
R anterior insula/central operculum | 150 | 44 | −4 | 2 | 13 | 4.42 |
L insula/putamen | 115 | −34 | −12 | 4 | – | 4.71 |
All reported clusters are significant at PFDR-corrected <0.05, two-sided, based on whole-brain correction. There were no clusters in which patients showed significantly greater functional connectivity than controls. Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates and t-statistics are given for peak voxel location.

Group differences in DMN connectivity. (A) Statistical map of group differences in lateral and medial views of cortical surface at PFDR-corrected <0.05. Greater connectivity in controls is depicted in red. There were no regions of significantly greater connectivity in patients. (B) Distribution of connectivity between PCC seed and right SPL cluster is shown separately in each group. Black bars represent group means.

Unthresholded statistical maps of PCC functional connectivity displayed on a template brain. (A) Healthy control group. (B) Patients with SAD and BDD. Positive connectivity is displayed in warm colors; negative connectivity is displayed in blue. Units on the color key reflect t-values.
We conducted the same analysis while controlling for concurrent medications by covarying medication status. This resulted in a similar pattern of results, but only at a voxel-level threshold of P < 0.005 (cluster-level FDR-corrected threshold of P < 0.05; Supplementary Table S3).
Associations between DMN functional connectivity and maladaptive SFA
Connectivity measures within the six regions showing group differences were extracted for individual subjects and correlated with Public SCS-R scores, BDI-II, RRS, LSAS and BDD-YBOCS scores. There were two significant inverse correlations between PCC-bilateral SPL connectivity and Public SCS-R scores (right SPL: r = −0.573, P = 0.001; left SPL: r = −0.530, P = 0.003), reflecting that greater SFA was associated with reduced connectivity of the PCC with the SPL (Table 3). PCC-bilateral SPL connectivity did not correlate significantly with either depression severity, rumination, social anxiety or body dysmorphic symptom severity. Only for right PCC- SPL connectivity were correlations with Public SCS-R scores significantly different from correlations with BDI-II (P = 0.05), RRS (P = 0.05) and BDD-YBOCS (P = 0.01), but not LSAS scores (P = 0.37). The correlations for left PCC-left SPL connectivity with Public SCS-R scores did not significantly differ from the other clinical measures (all P’s > 0.14). A full correlation matrix of connectivity and clinical measures in patients and controls are reported in Supplementary Table S2.
Correlations between PCC connectivity and clinical measures in patients only
Public SCS-R | LSAS | BDD-YBOCS | BDI-II | RRS | |
1. PCC-right CC | −0.198 | −0.398 | −0.378 | −0.178 | −0.232 |
2. PCC-right SPL | −0.573*** | −0.417 | −0.049 | −0.150 | −0.133 |
3. PCC-left PCG | 0.034 | −0.552* | −0.439 | −0.405* | −0.433* |
4. PCC-left SPL | −0.530** | −0.464 | −0.345 | −0.265 | −0.198 |
5. PCC-right INS | −0.017 | −0.116 | −0.615* | −0.280 | −0.402* |
6. PCC-left INS | −0.018 | −0.127 | −0.182 | −0.242 | −0.259 |
Public SCS-R | LSAS | BDD-YBOCS | BDI-II | RRS | |
1. PCC-right CC | −0.198 | −0.398 | −0.378 | −0.178 | −0.232 |
2. PCC-right SPL | −0.573*** | −0.417 | −0.049 | −0.150 | −0.133 |
3. PCC-left PCG | 0.034 | −0.552* | −0.439 | −0.405* | −0.433* |
4. PCC-left SPL | −0.530** | −0.464 | −0.345 | −0.265 | −0.198 |
5. PCC-right INS | −0.017 | −0.116 | −0.615* | −0.280 | −0.402* |
6. PCC-left INS | −0.018 | −0.127 | −0.182 | −0.242 | −0.259 |
PCC = posterior cingulate cortex. CC = cingulate cortex. SPL = superior parietal lobule. PCG = post-central gyrus. INS = insula. LSAS = Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. BDD-YBOCS = Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale Modified for BDD. BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II. RRS = Ruminative Response Scale. ***P ≤ 0.001, **P ≤ 0.01, *P ≤ 0.05.
Correlations between PCC connectivity and clinical measures in patients only
Public SCS-R | LSAS | BDD-YBOCS | BDI-II | RRS | |
1. PCC-right CC | −0.198 | −0.398 | −0.378 | −0.178 | −0.232 |
2. PCC-right SPL | −0.573*** | −0.417 | −0.049 | −0.150 | −0.133 |
3. PCC-left PCG | 0.034 | −0.552* | −0.439 | −0.405* | −0.433* |
4. PCC-left SPL | −0.530** | −0.464 | −0.345 | −0.265 | −0.198 |
5. PCC-right INS | −0.017 | −0.116 | −0.615* | −0.280 | −0.402* |
6. PCC-left INS | −0.018 | −0.127 | −0.182 | −0.242 | −0.259 |
Public SCS-R | LSAS | BDD-YBOCS | BDI-II | RRS | |
1. PCC-right CC | −0.198 | −0.398 | −0.378 | −0.178 | −0.232 |
2. PCC-right SPL | −0.573*** | −0.417 | −0.049 | −0.150 | −0.133 |
3. PCC-left PCG | 0.034 | −0.552* | −0.439 | −0.405* | −0.433* |
4. PCC-left SPL | −0.530** | −0.464 | −0.345 | −0.265 | −0.198 |
5. PCC-right INS | −0.017 | −0.116 | −0.615* | −0.280 | −0.402* |
6. PCC-left INS | −0.018 | −0.127 | −0.182 | −0.242 | −0.259 |
PCC = posterior cingulate cortex. CC = cingulate cortex. SPL = superior parietal lobule. PCG = post-central gyrus. INS = insula. LSAS = Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. BDD-YBOCS = Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale Modified for BDD. BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II. RRS = Ruminative Response Scale. ***P ≤ 0.001, **P ≤ 0.01, *P ≤ 0.05.
In patients, PCC connectivity with the left post-central gyrus was significantly correlated with depression severity, rumination, and social anxiety severity and PCC connectivity with the right anterior insula was significantly correlated with rumination and body dysmorphic severity (all P’s < 0.05). No other DMN connectivity measures were significantly correlated with clinical measures in patients. In controls, none of the connectivity measures were significantly associated with maladaptive SFA, depression severity or rumination.
To examine whether DMN connectivity differences between patients and controls were driven by either the SAD or BDD group, we compared the six connectivity measures in simple post-hoc comparisons between SAD vs controls, BDD vs controls, and SAD vs BDD. Our post-hoc tests showed that connectivity differences between individuals with SAD vs controls remained (all P’s ≤ .001), and connectivity differences between BDD vs controls (all P’s ≤ .005) remained, in the same direction of our original findings. There were no significant differences in connectivity between SAD vs BDD (P’s ranging between 0.21 and 0.87). The same pattern of findings emerged when comparing SAD patients to controls specifically matched to the SAD group, comparing BDD patients to controls specifically matched to the BDD group, and comparing SAD vs BDD, while controlling for age.
Discussion
Contrary to our hypothesis that was limited to the DMN, we did not find evidence of greater resting state functional connectivity in the DMN in patients with SAD or BDD with maladaptive SFA. Instead, we found decreased PCC connectivity to regions outside of the DMN in patients as compared to controls. Specifically, patients, relative to controls, exhibited abnormally reduced functional connectivity between the PCC, a key component of the DMN, and brain regions that are typically activated during cognitive task execution, specifically dorsal cingulate cortex, bilateral SPL, bilateral insula and post-central gyrus, which reflect certain anatomical components of the dorsal attention network (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Fox et al., 2005) and salience network (SN) (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Menon and Uddin, 2010). These connectivity differences were not driven by one of the two patient groups, as the same pattern of differences emerged within both groups and there were no significant differences between patient vs. control groups. Of all the connectivity measures that showed group differences, only PCC connectivity with bilateral SPL was significantly associated with self-reported maladaptive SFA in patients, in an inverse direction, such that greater maladaptive SFA was associated with reduced PCC-bilateral SPL connectivity. Our results suggest that maladaptive SFA may reflect a failure to modulate the balance of attention between internal and external milieus in both disorders.
There were several regions in our results that belong to the ‘task positive network’, which typically includes the intraparietal sulcus, inferior parietal lobule, frontal eye fields, dorsolateral mPFC, middle temporal regions, insula, and supplementary motor area (Fox et al., 2005). Our results suggest that abnormal interactions between the DMN and such task positive regions as the SPL, post-central gyrus and anterior insula may reflect difficulties shifting between these networks. The SPL is considered a component of the dorsal attention network (DAN), together with the frontal eye fields and intraparietal sulcus, which become activated during directed attention (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). In our study, reduced PCC-SPL connectivity in the patient group may reflect abnormal DMN-DAN connectivity, as an index of the biased attention toward internal processing (at the detriment of external processing) that is characteristic of maladaptive SFA, which is consistent with two recent studies in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder showing similar DMN-DAN functional connectivity disruptions (Cyr et al., 2020; Pagliaccio et al., 2021). However, our finding contrasts with prior literature showing that the DMN and DAN are typically anticorrelated networks, as we found positive DMN-DAN connectivity in controls and negative DMN-DAN connectivity in patients (as shown in Figure 1B). This may point to the complex and dynamic nature of voluntary attentional orienting that is not captured by our functional connectivity measure (which takes the correlation between the average time series of two brain regions) and may be better assessed using time-varying dynamic connectivity, as discussed by Maillet et al. (2019). For example, it is possible that the content of thoughts differed between patients and controls during the resting state scan, which led to varying amounts of sustained attention toward internal thoughts. Another possibility is that our sampling strategy recruited especially ‘low self-focused’ healthy controls, and it remains unclear whether such participants would perform differently on cognitive tasks or display different connectivity patterns.
Our findings suggest that reduced PCC connectivity with the SPL may specifically reflect maladaptive SFA across patients with SAD and BDD. In subsequent tests comparing associations between connectivity and clinical variables, there was some evidence supporting its specific association with maladaptive SFA, which was stronger and significantly different from its association with depression severity, rumination and body dysmorphic severity. However, its association with maladaptive SFA and social anxiety severity did not differ significantly. This is not surprising given that maladaptive SFA is typically highly correlated with social anxiety severity (Hofmann, 2007), and had a correlation of r = 0.378 in our sample. In fact, we expected maladaptive SFA to be more highly correlated with both SAD and BDD severity, given that it is such a prominent feature of the two conditions. The lower strength of the associations that we found may be due to restricted range on the upper tail of the Public SCS-R measure. It is also noteworthy that this measure captures the social self-presentation concerns that are more characteristic of SFA in SAD and BDD, compared to SFA in depression, which is more ruminative in nature (Watkins, 2004; Watkins and Teasdale, 2004). This explains why PCC-right SPL connectivity may specifically index maladaptive SFA in SAD, but not in depression. PCC- SPL connectivity (bilaterally) was found to be associated with maladaptive SFA, which is consistent with evidence supporting a largely bilateral dorsal attention system (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), as well as a study in obsessive-compulsive disorder showing abnormalities in DMN subsystem functional connectivity with bilateral SPL (Beucke et al., 2014). We also found that PCC connectivity with other regions, such as the left postcentral gyrus and right anterior insula, may actually be associated with rumination, rather than maladaptive SFA. These associations need to be confirmed in larger studies. Together, our findings suggest that PCC connectivity with the SPL, in particular, may reflect a specific neural signature of maladaptive SFA.
Our findings also contribute to a growing literature on abnormal patterns of connectivity in anxiety and obsessive-compulsive related disorders between regions within the DMN and other large-scale brain networks, such as the SN and frontoparietal network (Broyd et al., 2009). For example, one study showed that individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder displayed reduced anticorrelations between the DMN and the anterior insula (Posner et al., 2017), whereas another study found increased DMN connectivity with the insula (Peng et al., 2014). The anterior insula is a major node of the SN involved in processing interoceptive information, as well as detecting salient stimuli and switching between externally oriented attention networks and internally oriented self-referential networks to guide behavior (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Menon and Uddin, 2010). In our study, reduced PCC-anterior insula connectivity was not linked to maladaptive SFA. However, it may reflect abnormal interactions between the DMN and SN attributable to heightened anxiety sensitivity and interoceptive awareness to the physical sensations of anxiety, which has been described in SAD (Hofmann, 2007), but less well-studied in BDD. Part of the reason for inconsistent findings in studies examining DMN connectivity in anxiety and obsessive-compulsive related disorders (and perhaps why we did not confirm our original group-related hypothesis) is that the traditional case-control approach is limited by its ability to address comorbidities and other patient-related effects of an illness that are not present in controls. It is therefore important for future research conducting case-control comparisons of DMN connectivity to examine relationships between connectivity and clinical variables to better discern neural phenotypes associated with disorders.
A strength of our approach was the transdiagnostic examination of maladaptive SFA, which has implications for a common mechanism underlying two seemingly disparate psychiatric disorders, as well as the possibility of reclassification of these disorders in the DSM. SAD and BDD are currently classified as an anxiety disorder and an obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorder, respectively, in DSM-5. By enrolling individuals with SAD and BDD with similar levels of maladaptive SFA (as well as depression severity and insight), we identified a common pattern of dysconnectivity (between PCC and SPL) that correlated with Public SCS-R scores. This finding opens the possibility for engaging a common target in existing and new interventions for SAD and BDD.
Our study had some limitations. First, our Public SCS-R measure of trait maladaptive SFA was a subscale measure of trait self-consciousness that has not been well-studied in clinical populations. It was surprising that Public SCS-R scores were not more highly correlated with SAD and BDD symptom severity, which may be due to clinical heterogeneity in our sample. Despite the fact that there were only seven items in this scale and we employed narrow selection criteria based on scores from this scale, there was enough variability in both the patient and control groups on this measure to demonstrate significant associations with our neural measures. More research is needed to examine the construct validity of maladaptive SFA as distinct from related constructs such as rumination and repetitive negative thinking, as well as its reliability as a stable trait-like construct. Second, the correlation between maladaptive SFA and PCC-SPL connectivity was unexpected and therefore requires replication. Third, we only examined connectivity with the PCC as a commonly studied seed region of the DMN; however, future studies could examine other nodes of the DMN using a similar seed-based approach, or in a multivariate manner, such as with independent components analysis. Lastly, our sample composition limits the generalizability of our findings to the general population, as our sample comprised of predominantly Caucasian, highly educated, young, female participants. Studies have highlighted the problematic consequences of non-representative sampling on findings of brain structure and function (Falk et al., 2013; LeWinn et al., 2017), which underscore the need to broaden recruitment efforts and develop stronger community partnerships to capture the full dimension of variability in neural measures in clinical neuroscience studies.
Limitations notwithstanding, our study is the first to demonstrate the neural correlates of maladaptive SFA in a transdiagnostic sample. Specifically, PCC connectivity with the SPL may index maladaptive SFA across SAD and BDD, two disorders that are classified in separate categories in the DSM-5. Our findings lend support for a transdiagnostic conceptualization of maladaptive SFA as a cognitive dimension along the broad anxiety spectrum, represented by disruptions between large-scale brain networks involved in mediating internal and external attention. Our study provides the foundational basis for further research on the neural correlates of maladaptive SFA as a predictor of treatment response and potential novel target for intervention.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to acknowledge Dr Mark Vangel for statistical assistance, Dr Susanne Hoeppner for database management and Rachel Porth, Jin Shin and Lucas Petre for assistance with data acquisition.
Funding
This work was supported by a NIMH Career Development Award K23 MH109593 to A.F. B.B. is supported by K01 MH114012. J.F. is supported by R01MH121520 (Feusner) and R21MH110865 (Feusner).
Conflict of interest
S.W. has received research grant awards from Koa Health, royalty payments from New Harbinger Publications, Guilford Publications, Oxford University Press and Springer, honorarium payments from the International OCD Foundation, Elsevier, Tourette Association of America and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and Brattleboro Retreat, travel reimbursement from the International OCD Foundation and Brattleboro Retreat, as well as payment for serving on the Scientific Advisory Board of One-Mind PsyberGuide. No other co-authors have any competing interests to disclose.
Clinical trials registration
ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02808702 https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02808702.
Supplementary data
Supplementary data is available at SCAN online.