Abstract

Uncovering the neurobiological abnormalities that may contribute to the manifestation of psychopathic traits is an important step toward understanding the etiology of this disorder. Although many studies have examined gray matter volume (GMV) in relation to psychopathy, few have examined how dimensions of psychopathic traits interactively relate to GMV, an approach that holds promise for parsing heterogeneity in neurobiological risk factors for this disorder. The aim of this study was to investigate the affective-interpersonal (Factor 1) and impulsive-antisocial (Factor 2) dimensions of psychopathy in relation to cortical surface and subcortical GMV in a mixed-gender, high-risk community sample with significant justice-system involvement (N = 156, 50.0% men). Cortex-wide analysis indicated that (i) the Factor 1 traits correlated negatively with GMV in two cortical clusters, one in the right rostral middle frontal region and one in the occipital lobe, and (ii) the interaction of the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial traits was negatively associated with GMV bilaterally in the parietal lobe, such that individuals high on both trait dimensions evidenced reduced GMV relative to individuals high on only one psychopathy factor. An interactive effect also emerged for bilateral amygdalar and hippocampal GMV, such that Factor 1 psychopathic traits were significantly negatively associated with GMV only at high (but not low) levels of Factor 2 traits. Results extend prior research by demonstrating the neurobiological correlates of psychopathy differ based on the presentation of Factor 1 and 2 traits.

Psychopathy is a construct defined by a constellation of personality traits that include callousness, shallow affect, grandiosity, impulsivity and antisocial behavior (Hare, 2003). Psychopathic traits are robustly associated with negative clinical outcomes, including substance use disorders and incarceration (Beaver et al., 2014), which makes identifying the etiology of these personality traits an important area of research. Etiological models of psychopathy recognize that psychopathy is a multidimensional construct comprised of at least two primary dimensions: Factor 1 (F1) represents the affective and interpersonal symptoms of psychopathy (e.g. lack of remorse, shallow affect, superficial charm, deceitfulness) and Factor 2 (F2) captures the chronic engagement in impulsive and antisocial acts (e.g. need for stimulation, irresponsibility, criminal behavior) (Hare, 2003; Hare and Neumann, 2005; Lilienfeld et al., 2015). Although high scores on both factors are needed for an individual to meet criteria for a diagnosis of psychopathy (Hare et al., 1990), a growing body of research has illustrated that the psychopathy factors show unique relations with etiological mechanisms that are not apparent when psychopathy is examined as a unitary construct (Benning et al., 2003; Harpur et al., 1989; Verona, Patrick and Joiner, 2001; Ross et al., 2009). At the same time, individuals who score high on both psychopathy factors are known to be at highest risk for criminal recidivism and the most difficult to treat (Douglas et al., 2018), underscoring the importance of characterizing the etiological profiles of individuals who manifest elevations on both primary dimensions of psychopathy. Given that distinct neurobiological abnormalities may predispose individuals to develop the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial features of the disorder (Umbach et al., 2015), the objective of this study was to further understand the neurobiology of psychopathy by examining associations between the psychopathy factors and gray matter volume (GMV).

Etiological pathways to psychopathy factors

The psychopathy dimensions are theorized to index separable etiological pathways to criminal behavior that are characterized by distinct risk factors (Harpur et al., 1989; Hall et al., 2004; Fowles and Dindo, 2006; Sadeh et al., 2013a). For example, the interpersonal-affective (F1) dimension has been associated with low levels of fear, resilience to mood disorders and abnormal attentional functioning (e.g. Benning et al., 2003; Harpur et al., 1989; Sadeh and Verona, 2008), whereas the impulsive-antisocial (F2) dimension has been linked to high levels of distress and psychopathology, working memory deficits and psychosocial adversity (Harpur et al., 1989; Benning et al., 2003). Abnormalities in neural circuitry postulated to modulate the experience of fear, such as the amygdala and paralimbic system (e.g. Kiehl, 2006; Blair, 2013), are thought to motivate the interpersonal-affective symptoms of psychopathy. For example, functional studies have shown that F1 traits are negatively associated with amygdala activation to emotional stimuli (Gordon et al., 2004; Buckholtz et al., 2010). Conversely, F2 traits are typically positively associated with activation in emotional processing and reward anticipation regions, including the amygdala and nucleus accumbens (Gordon et al., 2004; Buckholtz et al., 2010), suggesting hyper-responsivity to motivationally relevant stimuli.

There is also evidence that differences in executive functioning drive etiological differences related to the two psychopathy factors. Research suggests that the interpersonal-affective traits (F1) drive psychopathic individuals to perform abnormally on attentionally demanding tasks (e.g. Sadeh and Verona, 2008; Koenigs et al., 2011; Baskin-Sommers et al., 2013; Larson et al., 2013). In contrast, deficits in working memory and response inhibition are differentially associated with the impulsive-antisocial dimension (F2) (Sadeh and Verona, 2008). Given this pattern, there are likely both functional and structural neurobiological differences that explain these differences in cognition between the two psychopathy dimensions (Moreira et al., 2019).

Psychopathy factors and gray matter volume

Consistent with the theory that the psychopathy dimensions index distinct risk processes for antisocial behavior, research examining F1 and F2 traits and GMV have shown some differential correlates. Previous research suggests that affective-interpersonal F1 traits have specifically been negatively associated with GMV in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Korponay et al., 2017), the orbitofrontal cortex (De Brito et al., 2021) and the anterior and posterior cingulate cortices (Caldwell et al., 2019), consistent with the paralimbic hypothesis (Kiehl, 2006). Impulsive-antisocial F2 psychopathic traits also evidence some distinct GMV associations. Findings have shown positive associations with GMV of the basal ganglia (e.g. the striatum) (Cope et al., 2012; Korponay and Koenigs, 2021) and negative associations between F2 traits and the middle occipital gyrus (De Brito et al., 2021) and the supplementary motor area (Leutgeb et al., 2015), all regions important for reward processing and impulse control. There have also been F2 trait associations with GMV in the prefrontal cortex, although the findings have been mixed, with some studies reporting positive GMV associations in the prefrontal cortex (Korponay et al., 2017; Korponay and Koenigs, 2021) and others reporting negative associations (De Brito et al., 2021), including with the OFC. It is possible that these observed discrepancies could be due to the types of populations being studied, as recent research has pointed out that the neural mechanisms underlying impulsive traits may differ in forensic vs community samples (Korponay and Koenigs, 2021). In spite of these possible specific GMV associations with the psychopathic trait dimensions, it should also be noted that distinct associations are not always observed, as evidenced by studies showing both psychopathy factors are negatively associated with GMV in the orbitofrontal cortex (Ermer et al., 2012) and the amygdala (Yang et al., 2009b; Ermer et al., 2012; Pardini et al., 2014; Vieira et al., 2015; Ling and Raine, 2018). Thus, whether the psychopathy dimensions are associated with unique or overlapping neurobiological vulnerabilities remains unclear and requires further investigation.

Neurobiology of interactive effects of psychopathic traits

In addition to the unique variance associated with each psychopathy factor, it may be important to characterize the neurobiological correlates of their interactive effects. Many studies have examined neurobiological differences between individuals who meet criteria for a diagnosis of psychopathy (i.e. individuals who score high on both F1 and F2) vs those who do not (Griffiths and Jalava, 2017; Hofhansel et al., 2020) and report reductions in GMV in paralimbic structures (OFC, insula) (Nummenmaa et al., 2021), and the supplementary motor area (Leutgeb et al., 2015) in psychopathic individuals, as well as mixed findings with striatal volumes (Boccardi et al., 2013; Lam et al., 2017). While informative about extreme manifestations of psychopathic traits, such group-based designs are inconsistent with research showing psychopathy is best characterized as a continuous, rather than taxonic construct (e.g. Edens et al., 2006). The continuous analysis of psychopathic traits, and their interactive effects, provides insight into the link between the severity of psychopathic traits and GMV characteristics. However, almost no research has examined how factor-level interactions relate to abnormalities in GMV (Hofhansel et al., 2020), which is important for clarifying how one psychopathy factor may attenuate or accentuate the effects of the other (Walsh and Kosson, 2008; Sprague et al., 2012; Verona et al., 2012).

Current study

To further understand how the interplay of the psychopathy factors relates to GMV, the aim of this study was to investigate the main and interactive effects of affective-interpersonal (F1) and impulsive-antisocial (F2) psychopathic traits on GMV in a mixed-gender, high-risk community sample. In light of the broader literature on the external correlates of the psychopathy dimensions, we expected the interpersonal-affective dimension (F1) to be negatively associated with GMV in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, cingulate cortices and subcortical limbic regions, specifically the amygdala and hippocampus (Caldwell et al., 2019; De Brito et al., 2021). We expected the impulsive-antisocial dimension (F2) to be negatively related to GMV in supplementary motor area and positively associated with subcortical reward processing regions, specifically the subregions of the striatum (Glenn et al., 2010; Cope et al., 2012; Leutgeb et al., 2015; Korponay and Koenigs, 2021). We also expected that the interactive effects of the psychopathy factors would parallel the neurobiological abnormalities observed for individuals high on psychopathy.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from the community using flyers and online advertisements. To be eligible for the study, participants were required to be between 18 and 50 years old and fluent in English. Participants were excluded if they reported a serious medical condition, history of head trauma resulting in loss of consciousness for over 30 min or lasting effects, current psychosis, any MRI contraindications or an estimated IQ of less than 80. Written and oral informed consent was obtained from all subjects, as approved by the University of Delaware Institutional Review Board.

We excluded two participants with unreliable self-report data, one participant missing handedness, and 10 participants that did not pass MRI data quality assurance standards. This resulted in a final sample size of 156 adults (50.0% men; M/SDage= 30.7/ 8.4 years old). The sample was racially and socioeconomically diverse. Participants identified as Caucasian (55.8%), Black or African American (34.0%), Asian American (9.6%) or ‘Other’ (1.9%) and 13.5% identified as Hispanic or Latino. The median household income was just over $36 000 for the last year, and most participants came from communities with high rates of violent and non-violent crime (https://www.neighborhoodscout.com/de/wilmington/crime on 7 January 2021). About half of the sample reported being arrested at least once in their lifetime.

Measures

Psychopathic traits

The Self-Report Psychopathy Scale-Fourth Edition (SRP-4) (Paulhus et al., 2015) is a 64-item measure developed based on the well-established Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (Hare, 2003) and is designed to assess psychopathic traits in community and forensic samples. Each item is scored on a five-point Likert Scale (1 = ‘Strongly Disagree’, 5 = ‘Strongly Agree’). Responses on the Affective and Interpersonal facets were summed to create a combined Affective-Interpersonal factor (F1) and responses on the Lifestyle and Antisocial facets were summed to create a combined Impulsive-Antisocial factor (F2). In this sample, Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities were 0.89 and 0.88 for F1 and F2, respectively.

Substance use

The Risky, Impulsive and Self-Destructive Behavior Questionnaire (Sadeh and Baskin-Sommers, 2017) was used to assess the frequency of lifetime alcohol and drug use (cannabis, cocaine, opioids, prescription pills, psychedelic drugs). Participants reported how many times they engaged in substance use in the past month and throughout their lifetime. Responses were categorized into five bins that constrained the range of possible responses at the high end of the distribution: 0, 1–10, 11–50, 51–100, >100 times. Positive skewness was further reduced using a Blom transformation.

MRI acquisition

Data were collected using a Siemens 3T Magnetom Prisma scanner with a 64-channel head coil. T1-weighted multi-echo MPRAGE anatomical scan (resolution = 1 mm3, TR = 2530 ms, TEs = 1.69, 3.55, 5.41, 7.27 ms) was collected, which has the advantage of less distortion and higher contrast than standard MPRAGE sequences, resulting in more reliable cortical models (van der Kouwe et al., 2008). A T2-weighted variable flip-angle turbo spin-echo scan (resolution = 1 mm3, TR = 3200 ms, TE = 564 ms) was collected, to be used in FreeSurfer to better differentiate the gray-matter-dura boundary.

Gray matter volume

The cortical surface volume of each vertex was estimated using FreeSurfer’s (v6) standard morphometric pipeline (Fischl, 2012). Data were spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 15 mm full-width at half maximum, following previous similar analyses (Bernal-Rusiel et al., 2010; Hyatt et al., 2012). The Freesurfer (v6) standard volume-based stream was used to segment the subcortical volumes (amygdala, hippocampus, caudate, putamen and nucleus accumbens) and label cortical and subcortical tissue classes, the steps of which are fully described elsewhere (Fischl et al., 2002, 2004).

Data analytic plan

General linear models were used to examine associations between psychopathic traits and GMV. For all models, SRP-4 F1 and SRP-4 F2 scores were included in each model simultaneously to examine the unique associations of each factor with GMV, and the interaction term was entered in a separate step. Age, biological sex (sex), scaled verbal IQ scores (Wechsler, 2008), estimated total intracranial volume (eTIV) and handedness were included in all analyses as covariates of no interest, based on previous analyses, and given associations between these variables, psychopathy and brain morphology in previous research (Szabo et al., 2001; Baskin-Sommers et al., 2016; Sajous-Turner et al., 2020; Pan et al., 2021). We also conducted follow-up analyses for significant results examining whether the findings were attributable to substance use using total lifetime drug and alcohol use (substance use).

Cortical surface volumes

FreeSurfer’s QDEC application was used to test general linear models examining the associations between the main and interactive effects of SRP-4 F1 and SRP-4 F2 traits and cortical GMV. All variables were standardized prior to analysis. To correct for multiple comparisons, we applied a standard correction procedure using the FreeSurfer analysis software. Volumetric results were corrected for the number of tests conducted using pre-cached Gaussian Monte Carlo simulation (10 000 iterations, cluster-wise threshold: P < 0.05, sign: absolute) that was created based on the size of the ROI examined (Hagler et al., 2006).

Subcortical volumes

We employed multivariate hierarchical linear regression analysis using SPSS version 26 to evaluate associations between psychopathic traits and subcortical volumes. Specifically, we entered amygdala, hippocampus, caudate, putamen and nucleus accumbens for each hemisphere as dependent variables (total of 10 dependent variables), the psychopathy factors and their interaction as predictors, and age, sex, VIQ, eTIV and handedness as covariates in this analysis. All variables were standardized prior to analysis. We present the covariates, main effects and interactive effects in separate steps of the regression to allow for interpretation of the variance in the dependent variables accounted for by variables of no interest (covariates) separately from the explanatory variables (psychopathy dimensions).

Results

Descriptive statistics

On average, scores on the affective-interpersonal (SRP-4 F1: M/SD = 74.7/17.4) and impulsive-antisocial (SRP-4 F2: M/SD = 71.8/18.0) trait dimensions were higher than those typically observed in community samples (SRP-4 F1: M/SD = 61.6/11.8; SRP-4 F2: M/SD = 63.1/15.6) and lower than those normed from forensic samples (SRP-4 F1: M/SD = 89.0/16.5; SRP-4 F2: M/SD = 100.6/16.2) (Paulhus et al., 2017). As expected, the psychopathy factors were moderately intercorrelated (r = 0.56, P <0.001).

Psychopathic trait relations with cortical surface volumes

Cortex-wide analysis of GMV identified two clusters associated with the psychopathy factors that survived correction for multiple comparisons. In the right hemisphere, SRP-4 F1 was negatively associated with GMV in a cluster centered on the rostral middle frontal gyrus, and spanning the fronto-marginal gyrus, and the orbital gyrus. In the left hemisphere, SRP-4 F1 was negatively associated with GMV in a cluster centered on the pericalcarine region and spanning the cuneus and lingual gyrus (Figure 1). No clusters survived correction for multiple comparisons for the SRP-4 F2 analysis.

F1 psychopathic traits negatively related to cortical volume in regions of both hemispheres, after controlling for age, sex, VIQ, eTIV, handedness and F2. Left Hemisphere: Peak F-Value = −3.54. Peak MNI (x,y,z) = (−21.6, −73.7,3.1). No. of Vertices = 3150. Cluster Size (mm2) = 1747. Right Hemisphere: Peak F-Value = −4.61. Peak MNI (x,y,z) = (29.4,35.4,22.2). No. of Vertices = 2974. Cluster Size (mm2) = 1884.
Fig. 1.

F1 psychopathic traits negatively related to cortical volume in regions of both hemispheres, after controlling for age, sex, VIQ, eTIV, handedness and F2. Left Hemisphere: Peak F-Value = −3.54. Peak MNI (x,y,z) = (−21.6, −73.7,3.1). No. of Vertices = 3150. Cluster Size (mm2) = 1747. Right Hemisphere: Peak F-Value = −4.61. Peak MNI (x,y,z) = (29.4,35.4,22.2). No. of Vertices = 2974. Cluster Size (mm2) = 1884.

The interaction of the psychopathy factors (SRP-4 F1 × SRP-4 F2) was negatively associated with GMV in two cortical clusters and positively associated with GMV in one cortical cluster (Table 1 and Figure 2). The first cluster in the left hemisphere was negatively associated with the interaction, peaked in the superior parietal lobule and included the angular gyrus, precuneus, paracentral lobule and superior frontal gyrus (Cluster A). The second cluster in the left hemisphere was positively associated with the interaction, peaked in the lingual gyrus and spanned the cuneus (Cluster B). The third cluster in the right hemisphere was negatively associated with the interaction, peaked in the supramarginal gyrus and spanned the angular gyrus, anterior transverse temporal gyrus, superior parietal lobule, lateral aspect of the superior temporal gyrus and temporal plane of the superior temporal gyrus (Cluster C). For clusters A and C, as scores on F2 increased, the association between SRP-4 F1 and cortical volume became more strongly negative. Specifically, for these clusters, the correlations between SRP-4 F1 and cortical GMV were positive at low values of F2 (−0.5 SD, rs = 0.16–0.28) and negative at high values of F2 (+0.5 SD, rs = −0.31 to −0.46). For Cluster B, the association was the opposite; as scores on F2 decreased, the association between SRP-4 F1 and cortical volume became more strongly positive. Specifically for Cluster B, the correlation between SRP-4 F1 and cortical GMV was negative at low values of F2 (−0.5 SD, r = −0.47) and positive at high values of F2 (+0.5 SD, r = 0.35).

Table 1.

Gray matter volume clusters showing a significant relationship with F1 × F2 psychopathic traits

Cluster No.HemisphereAnnotationPeak F-valuePeak MNI (x,y,z)No. of verticesCluster size (mm2)
ALSPL−4.07−30.8, −39.6, 44.610 1414271
AG
Precuneus
PCL
SFG
BLLG4.51−6.1, −84.4, −5.328342157
Cuneus
CRSMG−3.1255.0, −39.0, 41.977263283
AG
ATTG
SPL
STG-TP
STG-L
Cluster No.HemisphereAnnotationPeak F-valuePeak MNI (x,y,z)No. of verticesCluster size (mm2)
ALSPL−4.07−30.8, −39.6, 44.610 1414271
AG
Precuneus
PCL
SFG
BLLG4.51−6.1, −84.4, −5.328342157
Cuneus
CRSMG−3.1255.0, −39.0, 41.977263283
AG
ATTG
SPL
STG-TP
STG-L

N = 156. All clusters survived Monte Carlo Simulation correction for multiple comparisons (P < 0.05). Covariates included age, sex, verbal IQ, handedness and eTIV. RH = right hemisphere. LH = left hemisphere. AG = angular gyrus. ATTG = Anterior transverse temporal gyrus. LG = lingual gyrus. PCL = paracentral lobule. SFG = superior frontal gyrus. SMG = supramarginal gyrus. SPL = superior parietal lobule. STG-L = lateral aspect of the superior temporal gyrus. STG-TP = temporal plane of the superior temporal gyrus.

Table 1.

Gray matter volume clusters showing a significant relationship with F1 × F2 psychopathic traits

Cluster No.HemisphereAnnotationPeak F-valuePeak MNI (x,y,z)No. of verticesCluster size (mm2)
ALSPL−4.07−30.8, −39.6, 44.610 1414271
AG
Precuneus
PCL
SFG
BLLG4.51−6.1, −84.4, −5.328342157
Cuneus
CRSMG−3.1255.0, −39.0, 41.977263283
AG
ATTG
SPL
STG-TP
STG-L
Cluster No.HemisphereAnnotationPeak F-valuePeak MNI (x,y,z)No. of verticesCluster size (mm2)
ALSPL−4.07−30.8, −39.6, 44.610 1414271
AG
Precuneus
PCL
SFG
BLLG4.51−6.1, −84.4, −5.328342157
Cuneus
CRSMG−3.1255.0, −39.0, 41.977263283
AG
ATTG
SPL
STG-TP
STG-L

N = 156. All clusters survived Monte Carlo Simulation correction for multiple comparisons (P < 0.05). Covariates included age, sex, verbal IQ, handedness and eTIV. RH = right hemisphere. LH = left hemisphere. AG = angular gyrus. ATTG = Anterior transverse temporal gyrus. LG = lingual gyrus. PCL = paracentral lobule. SFG = superior frontal gyrus. SMG = supramarginal gyrus. SPL = superior parietal lobule. STG-L = lateral aspect of the superior temporal gyrus. STG-TP = temporal plane of the superior temporal gyrus.

F1 × F2 predicts lower cortical surface volume in the left and right hemispheres, after controlling for age, sex, VIQ, handedness and eTIV.
Fig. 2.

F1 × F2 predicts lower cortical surface volume in the left and right hemispheres, after controlling for age, sex, VIQ, handedness and eTIV.

Next, we examined whether the observed findings could be accounted for primarily by history of lifetime substance use, given the positive association between the impulsive-antisocial traits of psychopathy and substance use. All reported findings remained significant when this variable was included as a covariate in the models.

Psychopathic trait relations with subcortical volumes

Next, we examined associations between the psychopathy factors and subcortical GMV. The results are displayed in Table 2. The multivariate effect was significant for SRP-4 F1 [F(10, 139) = 1.98, P = 0.04], and this effect remained significant with substance use included in the model [F(10, 138) = 1.96, P = 0.04]. SRP-4 F1 was significantly inversely related to GMV in bilateral hippocampus (left: β = −0.14, SD = 0.07, P= 0.04; right: β = −0.14, SD = 0.07, P= 0.04) and marginally related to left caudate GMV (β = −0.14, SD = 0.08, P= 0.05). The addition of substance use as a covariate in the model strengthened the relationship between SRP-F1 and caudate GMV, such that it was significantly inversely related to volume in left caudate (β = −0.16, SD = 0.07, P= 0.03). No multivariate main effect was observed for SRP-4 F2 in relation to subcortical GMV.

Table 2.

SRP-4 psychopathy traits relate to subcortical gray matter volumes in bilateral amygdala and hippocampus

Between-Subjects Effects (ß-Statistic)
Multivariate TestAmygdalaHippocampusCaudateNAccPutamen
F StatisticP-Valuenp2LRLRLRLRLR
Step 1
Age3.020.0020.18−0.22*−0.14*−0.04−0.06−0.22*−0.22*−0.12−0.16*−0.27*−0.27*
Sex2.760.0040.16−0.040.04−0.08−0.10−0.14−0.11−0.21*−0.150.160.13
Verbal IQ2.990.0390.12−0.18*−0.070.050.000.01−0.01−0.060.01−0.020.03
eTIV21.06<0.0010.600.68*0.71*0.77*0.78*0.69*0.70*0.65*0.64*0.53*0.53*
Handedness0.850.5810.06
Step 2
SRP4- F1 Affective- Interpersonal Traits1.980.040.120.05−0.07−0.14*−0.14*−0.15−0.130.06−0.06−0.09−0.08
SRP-4 F2 Impulsive Antisocial Traits0.870.570.06
Step 3
SRP-4 F1 × SRP-4 F22.300.020.14−0.98*−1.62*−0.87*−1.20*−0.53−0.71−0.41−0.22−0.35−0.63
Between-Subjects Effects (ß-Statistic)
Multivariate TestAmygdalaHippocampusCaudateNAccPutamen
F StatisticP-Valuenp2LRLRLRLRLR
Step 1
Age3.020.0020.18−0.22*−0.14*−0.04−0.06−0.22*−0.22*−0.12−0.16*−0.27*−0.27*
Sex2.760.0040.16−0.040.04−0.08−0.10−0.14−0.11−0.21*−0.150.160.13
Verbal IQ2.990.0390.12−0.18*−0.070.050.000.01−0.01−0.060.01−0.020.03
eTIV21.06<0.0010.600.68*0.71*0.77*0.78*0.69*0.70*0.65*0.64*0.53*0.53*
Handedness0.850.5810.06
Step 2
SRP4- F1 Affective- Interpersonal Traits1.980.040.120.05−0.07−0.14*−0.14*−0.15−0.130.06−0.06−0.09−0.08
SRP-4 F2 Impulsive Antisocial Traits0.870.570.06
Step 3
SRP-4 F1 × SRP-4 F22.300.020.14−0.98*−1.62*−0.87*−1.20*−0.53−0.71−0.41−0.22−0.35−0.63

Note: N = 156. SRP-4 = Self-Report Psychopathy Scale–4th Ed. NAcc = Nucleus Accumbens. L = Left. R = Right. Between-subjects effects are only presented for significant multivariate effects. All predictor and outcome variables were standardized. *P < 0.05.

Table 2.

SRP-4 psychopathy traits relate to subcortical gray matter volumes in bilateral amygdala and hippocampus

Between-Subjects Effects (ß-Statistic)
Multivariate TestAmygdalaHippocampusCaudateNAccPutamen
F StatisticP-Valuenp2LRLRLRLRLR
Step 1
Age3.020.0020.18−0.22*−0.14*−0.04−0.06−0.22*−0.22*−0.12−0.16*−0.27*−0.27*
Sex2.760.0040.16−0.040.04−0.08−0.10−0.14−0.11−0.21*−0.150.160.13
Verbal IQ2.990.0390.12−0.18*−0.070.050.000.01−0.01−0.060.01−0.020.03
eTIV21.06<0.0010.600.68*0.71*0.77*0.78*0.69*0.70*0.65*0.64*0.53*0.53*
Handedness0.850.5810.06
Step 2
SRP4- F1 Affective- Interpersonal Traits1.980.040.120.05−0.07−0.14*−0.14*−0.15−0.130.06−0.06−0.09−0.08
SRP-4 F2 Impulsive Antisocial Traits0.870.570.06
Step 3
SRP-4 F1 × SRP-4 F22.300.020.14−0.98*−1.62*−0.87*−1.20*−0.53−0.71−0.41−0.22−0.35−0.63
Between-Subjects Effects (ß-Statistic)
Multivariate TestAmygdalaHippocampusCaudateNAccPutamen
F StatisticP-Valuenp2LRLRLRLRLR
Step 1
Age3.020.0020.18−0.22*−0.14*−0.04−0.06−0.22*−0.22*−0.12−0.16*−0.27*−0.27*
Sex2.760.0040.16−0.040.04−0.08−0.10−0.14−0.11−0.21*−0.150.160.13
Verbal IQ2.990.0390.12−0.18*−0.070.050.000.01−0.01−0.060.01−0.020.03
eTIV21.06<0.0010.600.68*0.71*0.77*0.78*0.69*0.70*0.65*0.64*0.53*0.53*
Handedness0.850.5810.06
Step 2
SRP4- F1 Affective- Interpersonal Traits1.980.040.120.05−0.07−0.14*−0.14*−0.15−0.130.06−0.06−0.09−0.08
SRP-4 F2 Impulsive Antisocial Traits0.870.570.06
Step 3
SRP-4 F1 × SRP-4 F22.300.020.14−0.98*−1.62*−0.87*−1.20*−0.53−0.71−0.41−0.22−0.35−0.63

Note: N = 156. SRP-4 = Self-Report Psychopathy Scale–4th Ed. NAcc = Nucleus Accumbens. L = Left. R = Right. Between-subjects effects are only presented for significant multivariate effects. All predictor and outcome variables were standardized. *P < 0.05.

A significant SRP-4 F1 × SRP-4 F2 interaction did emerge in the multivariate analysis [F(10, 138) = 2.30, P = 0.02], and this effect remained significant with substance use in the model [F(10, 137) = 2.28, P = 0.02]. The interaction effect was significantly associated with GMV in bilateral amygdala (left: β = −0.98, SD = 0.46, P= 0.03; right: β = −1.62, SD = 0.40, P< 0.001) and bilateral hippocampus (left: β = −0.87, SD = 0.41, P= 0.03; right: β = −1.20, SD = 0.41, P= 0.004). To decompose the interaction effect, we conducted follow-up analyses and found that the multivariate test for SRP-4 F1 was significant at high levels of SRP-4 F2 (+0.5 SD) [F(4, 36) = 6.54, P = 0.001], but not low levels of SRP-4 F2 (−0.5 SD) [F(4, 32) = 0.76, P = 0.56]. At high values of SRP-4 F2, SRP-4 F1 was negatively associated with bilateral amygdala and hippocampal volume, whereas it was positively related to bilateral amygdala and hippocampal volume at low levels of SRP-4 F2 (Figure 3).

Associations between F1 psychopathic traits and subcortical volumes, split by high and low levels of F2.
Fig. 3.

Associations between F1 psychopathic traits and subcortical volumes, split by high and low levels of F2.

Discussion

Given that dimensions of psychopathic traits are theorized to index separable etiological pathways to criminal behavior, and there is evidence that these traits are at least partly neurobiologically instantiated (Hofhansel et al., 2020), we used a multidimensional framework to examine unique associations between the psychopathy factors and GMV. Consistent with research indicating individuals who score high on both the affective-interpersonal (F1) and impulsive-antisocial dimensions (F2) of psychopathy are neurobiologically distinct from those high (Figure 2) on only one psychopathy factor (Seara-Cardoso et al., 2012), we found that GMV in several brain regions was associated with the interaction of the factor-level traits. Specifically, the interaction of the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial trait dimensions was associated with reduced GMV in clusters of the right temporal lobe, bilateral parietal lobes, amygdala and hippocampus. Notably, each of these findings indicated that decrements in GMV increased as individuals high on impulsive-antisocial traits showed increasing levels of the affective-interpersonal traits. The interaction was also associated with greater volume in a cluster of the left occipital lobe. These findings underscore the potentially unique etiology of individuals who manifest elevations on both of the primary psychopathy dimensions. Interestingly, we also found that the unique variance associated with the affective-interpersonal trait dimension was negatively associated with GMV in one cluster centered on the rostral middle frontal gyrus and one cluster of the pericalcarine region, suggesting lower cortical surface volume in these regions among individuals who score high selectively on the callousness, superficial charm and deceitfulness that are core features of the disorder. Overall, our results provide new evidence that the neurobiological profiles of individuals high on both the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial trait dimensions are different from those who score high selectively on one trait dimension, findings that are important for potentially parsing etiological heterogeneity in psychopathy.

Although most research that has examined neurobiological variation associated with the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial traits has considered these dimensions in an additive fashion, there is a growing body of research that suggests these traits may be better conceptualized as interactive (Walsh and Kosson, 2008; Sprague et al., 2012; Verona et al., 2012). We found interactive effects of the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial dimensions on GMV, such that reductions in GMV were more pronounced in individuals high on both primary psychopathy traits relative to those who were high on only one trait dimension in three cortical clusters. One cluster centered on the superior parietal lobule and included the angular gyrus, precuneus, paracentral lobule and superior frontal gyrus. The superior parietal lobule has been previously associated with violent behavior and psychopathic traits (Yang et al., 2015; Lamsma et al., 2017) and is known to play a role in many sensory and cognitive processes (Wang et al., 2015). The angular gyrus plays a role in memory and reasoning and is thought to be part of a ‘moral neural circuit’ that has been shown to function abnormally in psychopathic individuals (Glenn et al., 2009). The paracentral lobule is part of a sensorimotor network (Spasojević et al., 2013) that has previously been found to function abnormally with respect to psychopathic traits (Espinoza et al., 2018). The superior frontal gyrus has been previously found to support attention (Li et al., 2013), perception and working memory (du Boisgueheneuc et al., 2006), so it follows that lower GMV (Cope et al., 2012) and abnormal functioning of this region (Sadeh et al., 2013b) have both been previously associated with psychopathic traits (particularly the affective-interpersonal dimension), consistent with theories that attentional deficits in psychopathy contribute to core features of the disorder (Smith and Lilienfeld, 2015). The precuneus is an important node of the default mode network (Fransson and Marrelec, 2008) and likely contributes to disturbed self-referential processing and moral decision making in individuals with psychopathic traits (Juárez et al., 2013; Freeman et al., 2014; Espinoza et al., 2018).

The second cluster in the left occipital lobe was positively associated with the interaction, peaked in the lingual gyrus and spanned the cuneus. While abnormalities within occipital regions involved in basic visual processing are not traditionally predicted by neurobiological models of psychopathy, recently meta-analyses have begun to show structural alterations in these areas (De Brito et al., 2021) that may relate to functional findings. Some recent fMRI studies have reported reduced functional connectivity between the occipital cortex and other cortical (Juárez et al., 2013) and subcortical structures (Contreras-Rodríguez et al., 2015) in individuals with psychopathic traits, which other authors have speculated could compromise normal processes that upregulate activity in these regions during emotional processing tasks (Anderson et al., 2017; Espinoza et al., 2018), but these hypotheses require further testing. Future research is needed to examine the relationship between greater GMV and functional connectivity within the context of the whole-brain networks (e.g. executive control, salience), as it is likely that relatively greater or lower GMV can contribute to abnormalities in brain functioning.

The third cluster in the right hemisphere was negatively associated with the interaction, peaked in the supramarginal gyrus and spanned the superior parietal lobule, angular gyrus, anterior transverse temporal gyrus, lateral aspect of the superior temporal gyrus and temporal plane of the superior temporal gyrus. Reductions in GMV of the supramarginal gyrus have been previously linked to psychopathic traits, and this region is thought to be a part of the ‘theory of mind’ network (Hofhansel et al., 2020). Reduced volume in the temporal cortex associated with psychopathic traits is also consistent with past research (Muller et al., 2008; Ermer et al., 2012; Ly et al., 2012; De Brito et al., 2021) and converges with functional data showing reduced activation in the superior temporal lobe of psychopathic individuals during moral judgement tasks (Harenski et al., 2010).

Given the preponderance of research implicating alterations in several subcortical structures in the etiology of psychopathy, we examined the unique and interactive effects of F1 and F2 traits on amygdalar, hippocampal and striatal (caudate, putamen, nucleus accumbens) volumes. We found that F1 psychopathic traits were negatively associated with bilateral hippocampal volumes, which is in line with previous research (Laakso et al., 2001; Boccardi et al., 2010). Models that characterize psychopathy as a disorder of the paralimbic system (Kiehl, 2006) posit that damage to paralimbic regions such as the hippocampus give rise to the abnormal fear conditioning observed in psychopathic individuals (Glenn and Raine, 2008), particularly with respect to the affective-interpersonal dimension. It is not known when in development these abnormalities arise so longitudinal studies establishing causality are still needed.

Interestingly, we did not find main effects of F1 or F2 on amygdala volumes in either hemisphere. This was somewhat surprising given prior research showing negative relationships between the affective-interpersonal traits and amygdala volume (Glenn and Raine, 2008; Pardini et al., 2014). A recent meta-analysis also did not find amygdala volume differences to be related to psychopathy dimensions (De Brito et al., 2021), possibly suggesting that psychopathy may be more reliably associated with functional abnormalities of the amygdala rather than structural ones. Similarly, we did not find any associations between F1 or F2 traits and striatal volumes, which is in contrast to previous research showing associations particularly with respect to the impulsive-antisocial dimension (Korponay et al., 2017; De Brito et al., 2021). In the integrated emotions system model of psychopathy (Blair, 2013), abnormal functioning of reward regions such as the striatum, particularly the caudate is thought to be central to perpetuating the reward-processing abnormalities that characterize the impulsive-antisocial component of the disorder. Our lack of findings may be at least partially explained by evidence showing that examining the unique variance of affective-personal traits (controlling for impulsive-antisocial traits) is different than examining bivariate associations without controlling for the overlap between the psychopathy dimensions (Hicks and Patrick, 2006; Lynam et al., 2006; Baskin-Sommers et al., 2009), with the former approach (taken here) providing insight into the distinct neurobiological correlates of these traits, and thus, how neurobiology may contribute to the manifestation of these psychopathic phenotypes. Future investigations will be needed to tease apart structural variation in the brain as it relates to these separate factors, although the current findings point to at least one distinct neurobiological correlate associated with scoring high selectively on the affective-interpersonal trait dimension.

Tests of the interaction of the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial traits revealed that the interactive effect of these traits was significantly associated with amygdala and hippocampal volumes in both hemispheres. Specifically, findings showed that the relationship between F1 psychopathic traits and bilateral amygdala and hippocampal volumes depended on scores on F2, such that as F2 scores increased, the relationship between F1 scores and amygdala volumes became more strongly negative. This extends previous literature showing that there is an inverse relationship between amygdala volume and psychopathy (Weber et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2009a; Ermer et al., 2012; Boccardi et al., 2013). Additionally, these results suggest that lower amygdala and hippocampal volume size could be an indicator of dysregulation of both emotional (F1) and behavioral (F2) systems, instead of primarily affective-interpersonal traits, which has been previously suggested (Yang et al., 2009b).

Although much research has demonstrated that psychopathic traits are associated with neuroanatomical abnormalities, relatively less research has examined the continuous relationship between trait severity of the specific psychopathic factor dimensions and GMV. To address this gap, the current study investigated the unique variance associated with the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial trait dimensions on brain volumes. We identified a negative relationship between Factor 1 psychopathic traits and GMV in left occipital and right prefrontal (orbitofrontal) regions. The GMV reductions we found in prefrontal and orbitofrontal regions in relation to Factor 1 psychopathic traits are well aligned with previous research (Hofhansel et al., 2020; De Brito et al., 2021) and with neurobiological models of psychopathy, including the integrated emotions systems model (Blair, 2005) as well as the paralimbic hypothesis (Kiehl, 2006). Specifically, abnormalities in the functioning of prefrontal and orbitofrontal regions are thought to give rise to the deficits in decision making (Koenigs, 2012), reward processing (Murray et al., 2018) and moral judgment (Pujol et al., 2012) that characterize the disorder. GMV associations between occipital regions and psychopathic traits are relatively new and understudied (De Brito et al., 2021), but may relate to functional studies showing that individuals with psychopathic traits display abnormal responding in occipital regions during affective processing tasks (Muller et al., 2003).

Overall, the current study identified several regions thought to be part of a limbic circuit that is central to neural dysfunction in psychopathy (e.g. orbitofrontal cortex, supramarginal gyrus, amygdala, hippocampus) (Anderson and Kiehl, 2014). However, we did not find any GMV reductions in a few commonly implicated paralimbic regions, including the anterior or posterior cingulate cortices or temporal pole (Ermer et al., 2012). We also did not observe any main associations between F2 traits (controlling for F1) and GMV, which differs from some prior research that has linked these traits to alterations in frontostriatal regions, the supplementary motor area and paralimbic regions (e.g. OFC, insula) (Leutgeb et al., 2015; Korponay and Koenigs, 2021). However, the majority of the studies presented in the psychopathy literature contain predominantly male samples and often contrast psychopathic with non-psychopathic individuals (Koenigs et al., 2011; Hofhansel et al., 2020). These could be a few of the factors that explain the differences we observed. Additionally, very few published studies to our knowledge have used the SRP to examine GMV (Pardini et al., 2014) and even fewer have examined the SRP factors using a whole-cortex approach (Johanson et al., 2020). More research is needed to clarify how psychopathic trait relations with GMV differ in clinical vs community samples, or as a function of biological sex, sample characteristics that may have contributed to differences between the current results and prior research. A recent systematic review has suggested that the neural mechanisms underlying impulsive psychopathic traits differ in community and forensic psychopathy samples (Korponay and Koenigs, 2021). Our examination of the impulsive-antisocial traits as a continuous variable in a sample with psychopathy scores that fell between the ranges typically observed in community and clinical samples may have obscured these differences and, for example, contributed to our null findings for the main effect of F2 traits. Future replications of these analyses, as well as research directly comparing the neural correlates of impulsivity in psychopathic and non-psychopathic groups, are needed to clarify the neural regions involved in pathological impulsivity vs those that may only be relevant to psychopathic traits studied at the community level.

Findings from the current study must be interpreted in the context of its limitations. First, the use of a community mixed-gender sample, while filling an important gap, does not allow for the results to be generalizable to individuals diagnosed with psychopathy. Additionally, psychopathic traits were assessed with a self-report measure rather than the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, which is the gold-standard method. Furthermore, we only examined several subcortical regions of interest, so a future direction of this research should be to examine how psychopathic traits relate to variation across all subcortical structures. The study also had several notable strengths, first, the investigation of psychopathic traits dimensionally in a relatively large, mixed-gender sample of adults recruited from the general community. The sample was both socioeconomically and ethnically diverse and reported relatively high rates of lifetime involvement in the criminal justice system, making it well-suited to study the proposed aims.

In conclusion, the current study adds to a growing body of literature that focuses on examining the factor-level interactions of the psychopathy dimensions on neurobiological characteristics. Results provide new insight into the neurobiological profiles of individuals who manifest both the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial dimensions of psychopathy and how they differ from individuals high on only one trait dimension. Further work examining psychopathic traits both interactively and specifically will be necessary to understand the direct link between the severity of psychopathic behavior and possible biological markers.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health awarded to Naomi Sadeh [2P20GM103653-06-6527, 1R01MH116228], Nadia Bounoua [1F31MH120936] and Rickie Miglin [F31DA053782]. These Institutes had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Conflict of interest

None declared.

References

Anderson
 
N.E.
,
Steele
 
V.R.
,
Maurer
 
J.M.
, et al. (
2017
).
Differentiating emotional processing and attention in psychopathy with functional neuroimaging
.
Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience
,
17
(
3
),
491
515
.

Anderson
 
N.E.
,
Kiehl
 
K.A.
(
2014
).
Psychopathy & aggression: when paralimbic dysfunction leads to violence
.
Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences
,
17
:
369
93
.

Baskin-Sommers
 
A.R.
,
Zeier
 
J.D.
,
Newman
 
J.P.
(
2009
).
Self-reported attentional control differentiates the major factors of psychopathy
.
Personality and Individual Differences
,
47
(
6
),
626
30
.

Baskin-Sommers
 
A.R.
,
Curtin
 
J.J.
,
Newman
 
J.P.
(
2013
).
Emotion-modulated startle in psychopathy: clarifying familiar effects
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
122
(
2
), 458.

Baskin-Sommers
 
A.R.
,
Neumann
 
C.S.
,
Cope
 
L.M.
,
Kiehl
 
K.A.
(
2016
).
Latent-variable modeling of brain gray-matter volume and psychopathy in incarcerated offenders
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
125
(
6
),
811
7
.

Beaver
 
K.M.
,
Nedelec
 
J.L.
,
Da Silva Costa
 
C.
, et al. (
2014
).
The association between psychopathic personality traits and health-related outcomes
.
Journal of Criminal Justice
,
42
(
5
),
399
407
.

Benning
 
S.D.
,
Patrick
 
C.J.
,
Hicks
 
B.M.
,
Blonigen
 
D.M.
,
Krueger
 
R.F.
(
2003
).
Factor structure of the psychopathic personality inventory: validity and implications for clinical assessment
.
Psychological Assessment
,
15
:
340
50
.

Bernal-Rusiel
 
J.L.
,
Atienza
 
M.
,
Cantero
 
J.L.
(
2010
).
Determining the optimal level of smoothing in cortical thickness analysis: a hierarchical approach based on sequential statistical thresholding
.
NeuroImage
,
52
(
1
),
158
71
.

Blair
 
R.J.R.
(
2005
).
Applying a cognitive neuroscience perspective to the disorder of psychopathy
.
Development and Psychopathology
,
17
(
3
),
865
91
.

Blair
 
R.J.R.
(
2013
).
Psychopathy: cognitive and neural dysfunction
.
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience
,
15
(
2
), 181.

Boccardi
 
M.
,
Ganzola
 
R.
,
Rossi
 
R.
, et al. (
2010
).
Abnormal hippocampal shape in offenders with psychopathy
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
31
(
3
),
438
47
.

Boccardi
 
M.
,
Bocchetta
 
M.
,
Aronen
 
H.J.
, et al. (
2013
).
Atypical nucleus accumbens morphology in psychopathy: another limbic piece in the puzzle
.
International Journal of Law and Psychiatry
,
36
(
2
),
157
67
.

Buckholtz
 
J.W.
,
Treadway
 
M.T.
,
Cowan
 
R.
, et al. (
2010
).
Mesolimbic dopamine reward system hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits
.
Nature Neuroscience
,
13
:
419
21
.

Caldwell
 
B.M.
,
Anderson
 
N.E.
,
Harenski
 
K.A.
, et al. (
2019
).
The structural brain correlates of callous-unemotional traits in incarcerated male adolescents
.
NeuroImage: Clinical
,
22
: 101703.

Contreras-Rodríguez
 
O.
,
Pujol
 
J.
,
Batalla
 
I.
, et al. (
2015
).
Functional connectivity bias in the prefrontal cortex of psychopaths
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
78
(
9
),
647
55
.

Cope
 
L.M.
,
Shane
 
M.S.
,
Segall
 
J.M.
, et al. (
2012
).
Examining the effect of psychopathic traits on gray matter volume in a community substance abuse sample
.
Psychiatry Research: NeuroImaging
,
204
(
2
),
91
100
.

De Brito
 
S.A.
,
McDonald
 
D.
,
Camilleri
 
J.A.
,
Rogers
 
J.C.
(
2021
).
Cortical and subcortical gray matter volume in psychopathy: a voxel-wise meta-analysis
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
130
(
6
), 627.

Douglas
 
K.S.
,
Vincent
 
G.M.
,
Edens
 
J.F.
(
2018
). Risk for criminal recidivism: the role of psychopathy. In: Patrick, C.J., editor.
Handbook of Psychopathy
, 2nd edn, New York, NY:
The Guilford Press
,
682
709
.

du Boisgueheneuc
 
F.
,
Levy
 
R.
,
Volle
 
E.
, et al. (
2006
).
Functions of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans: a lesion study
.
Brain
,
129
(
12
),
3315
28
.

Edens
 
J.F.
,
Marcus
 
D.K.
,
Lilienfeld
 
S.O.
,
Poythress
 
N.G.
(
2006
).
Psychopathic, not psychopath: taxometric evidence for the dimensional structure of psychopathy
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
115
(
1
),
131
44
.

Ermer
 
E.
,
Cope
 
L.M.
,
Nyalakanti
 
P.K.
,
Calhoun
 
V.D.
,
Kiehl
 
K.A.
(
2012
).
Aberrant paralimbic gray matter in criminal psychopathy
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
121
(
3
),
649
58
.

Espinoza
 
F.A.
,
Vergara
 
V.M.
,
Reyes
 
D.
, et al. (
2018
).
Aberrant functional network connectivity in psychopathy from a large (N = 985) forensic sample
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
39
(
6
),
2624
34
.

Fischl
 
B.
,
Salat
 
D.H.
,
Busa
 
E.
, et al. (
2002
).
Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain
.
Neuron
,
33
(
3
),
341
55
.

Fischl
 
B.
,
Salat
 
D.H.
,
van der Kouwe
 
A.J.W.
, et al. (
2004
).
Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images
.
NeuroImage
,
23
:
S69
84
.

Fischl
 
B.
(
2012
).
FreeSurfer
.
NeuroImage
,
62
(
2
),
774
81
.

Fowles
 
D.C.
,
Dindo
 
L.
(
2006
). A dual-deficit model of psychopathy. In: Patrick, C.J., editor.
Handbook of Psychopathy
. New York, NY:
The Guilford Press
,
14
34
.

Fransson
 
P.
,
Marrelec
 
G.
(
2008
).
The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: evidence from a partial correlation network analysis
.
NeuroImage
,
42
(
3
),
1178
84
.

Freeman
 
S.M.
,
Clewett
 
D.V.
,
Bennett
 
C.M.
,
Kiehl
 
K.A.
,
Gazzaniga
 
M.S.
,
Miller
 
M.B.
(
2014
).
The posteromedial region of the default mode network shows attenuated task-induced deactivation in psychopathic prisoners
.
Neuropsychology
,
29
(
3
), 493.

Glenn
 
A.L.
,
Raine
 
A.
,
Schug
 
R.A.
(
2009
).
The neural correlates of moral decision-making in psychopathy
.
Molecular Psychiatry
,
14
(
1
),
5
6
.

Glenn
 
A.L.
,
Raine
 
A.
,
Yaralian
 
P.S.
,
Yang
 
Y.
(
2010
).
Increased volume of the striatum in psychopathic individuals
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
67
(
1
),
52
8
.

Glenn
 
A.L.
,
Raine
 
A.
(
2008
).
The neurobiology of psychopathy
.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America
,
31
(
3
),
463
75
.

Gordon
 
H.L.
,
Baird
 
A.A.
,
End
 
A.
(
2004
).
Functional differences among those high and low on a trait measure of psychopathy
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
7
:
516
21
.

Griffiths
 
S.Y.
,
Jalava
 
J.V.
(
2017
).
A comprehensive neuroimaging review of PCL-R defined psychopathy
.
Aggression and Violent Behavior
,
36
:
60
75
.

Hagler
 
D.J.
,
Saygin
 
A.P.
,
Sereno
 
M.I.
(
2006
).
Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data
.
NeuroImage
,
33
(
4
),
1093
103
.

Hall
 
J.R.
,
Benning
 
S.D.
,
Patrick
 
C.J.
(
2004
).
Criterion-related validity of the three-factor model of psychopathy: personality, behavior, and adaptive functioning
.
Assessment
,
11
(
1
),
4
16
.

Hare
 
R.D.
,
Harpur
 
T.J.
,
Hakstian
 
A.R.
,
Forth
 
A.E.
,
Hart
 
S.D.
,
Newman
 
J.P.
(
1990
).
The revised psychopathy checklist: reliability and factor structure
.
Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
,
2
(
3
),
338
41
.

Hare
 
R.D.
(
2003
).
The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised
. 2ndedn.
Toronto, ON
:
Multi-Health Systems
.

Hare
 
R.D.
,
Neumann
 
C.S.
(
2005
). Structural models of psychopathy.
Current Psychiatry Reports
,
7
(
1
),
57
64
.

Harenski
 
C.L.
,
Harenski
 
K.A.
,
Shane
 
M.S.
,
Kiehl
 
K.A.
(
2010
).
Aberrant neural processing of moral violations in criminal psychopaths
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
119
(
4
), 863.

Harpur
 
T.J.
,
Hare
 
R.D.
,
Hakstian
 
A.R.
(
1989
).
Two-factor conceptualization of psychopathy: construct validity and assessment implications
.
Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
,
1
(
1
),
6
17
.

Hicks
 
B.M.
,
Patrick
 
C.J.
(
2006
).
Psychopathy and negative emotionality: analyses of suppressor effects reveal distinct relations with emotional distress, fearfulness, and anger-hostility
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
115
(
2
),
276
87
.

Hofhansel
 
L.
,
Weidler
 
C.
,
Votinov
 
M.
,
Clemens
 
B.
,
Raine
 
A.
,
Habel
 
U.
(
2020
).
Morphology of the criminal brain: gray matter reductions are linked to antisocial behavior in offenders
.
Brain Structure and Function
,
225
(
7
),
2017
28
.

Hyatt
 
C.J.
,
Haney-Caron
 
E.
,
Stevens
 
M.C.
(
2012
).
Cortical thickness and folding deficits in conduct-disordered adolescents
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
72
(
3
),
207
14
.

Johanson
 
M.
,
Vaurio
 
O.
,
Tiihonen
 
J.
,
Lähteenvuo
 
M.
(
2020
).
A systematic literature review of neuroimaging of psychopathic traits
.
Frontiers in Psychiatry
,
10
: 1027.

Juárez
 
M.
,
Kiehl
 
K.A.
,
Calhoun
 
V.D.
(
2013
).
Intrinsic limbic and paralimbic networks are associated with criminal psychopathy
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
34
(
8
),
1921
30
.

Kiehl
 
K.A.
(
2006
).
A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy: evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction
.
Psychiatry Research
,
142
(
2
),
107
28
.

Koenigs
 
M.
,
Baskin-Sommers
 
A.
,
Zeier
 
J.
,
Newman
 
J.P.
(
2011
).
Investigating the neural correlates of psychopathy: a critical review
.
Molecular Psychiatry
,
16
(
8
),
792
9
.

Koenigs
 
M.
(
2012
).
The role of prefrontal cortex in psychopathy
.
Reviews in the Neurosciences
,
23
(
3
),
253
62
.

Korponay
 
C.
,
Pujara
 
M.
,
Deming
 
P.
, et al. (
2017
).
Impulsive-antisocial psychopathic traits linked to increased volume and functional connectivity within prefrontal cortex
.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
,
12
(
7
),
1169
78
.

Korponay
 
C.
,
Koenigs
 
M.
(
2021
).
Gray matter correlates of impulsivity in psychopathy and in the general population differ by kind, not by degree: a comparison of systematic reviews
.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
,
16
(
7
),
683
95
.

Laakso
 
M.P.
,
Vaurio
 
O.
,
Koivisto
 
E.
, et al. (
2001
).
Psychopathy and the posterior hippocampus
.
Behavioural Brain Research
,
118
(
2
),
187
93
.

Lam
 
B.Y.H.
,
Yang
 
Y.
,
Schug
 
R.A.
,
Han
 
C.
,
Liu
 
J.
,
Lee
 
T.M.C.
(
2017
).
Psychopathy moderates the relationship between orbitofrontal and striatal alterations and violence: the investigation of individuals accused of homicide
.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
11
, 579.doi: .

Lamsma
 
J.
,
Mackay
 
C.
,
Fazel
 
S.
(
2017
).
Structural brain correlates of interpersonal violence: systematic review and voxel-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies
.
Psychiatry Research: NeuroImaging
,
267
:
69
73
.

Larson
 
C.L.
,
Baskin-Sommers
 
A.R.
,
Stout
 
D.M.
, et al. (
2013
).
The interplay of attention and emotion: top-down attention modulates amygdala activation in psychopathy
.
Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience
,
13
(
4
),
757
70
.

Leutgeb
 
V.
,
Leitner
 
M.
,
Wabnegger
 
A.
, et al. (
2015
).
Brain abnormalities in high-risk violent offenders and their association with psychopathic traits and criminal recidivism
.
Neuroscience
,
308
:
194
201
.

Li
 
W.
,
Qin
 
W.
,
Liu
 
H.
, et al. (
2013
).
Subregions of the human superior frontal gyrus and their connections
.
NeuroImage
,
78
:
46
58
.

Lilienfeld
 
S.O.
,
Watts
 
A.L.
,
Francis Smith
 
S.
,
Berg
 
J.M.
,
Latzman
 
R.D.
(
2015
).
Psychopathy deconstructed and reconstructed: identifying and assembling the personality building blocks of Cleckley’s chimera
.
Journal of Personality
,
83
(
6
),
593
610
.

Ling
 
S.
,
Raine
 
A.
(
2018
).
The neuroscience of psychopathy and forensic implications
.
Psychology, Crime and Law
,
24
(
3
),
296
312
.

Ly
 
M.
,
Motzkin
 
J.C.
,
Philippi
 
C.L.
, et al. (
2012
).
Cortical thinning in psychopathy
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
169
(
7
),
743
9
.

Lynam
 
D.R.
,
Hoyle
 
R.H.
,
Newman
 
J.P.
(
2006
).
The perils of partialling: cautionary tales from aggression and psychopathy
.
Assessment
,
13
(
3
),
328
41
.

Moreira
 
D.
,
Azeredo
 
A.
,
Barbosa
 
F.
(
2019
).
Neurobiological findings of the psychopathic personality in adults: one century of history
.
Aggression and Violent Behavior
,
47
:
137
59
.

Müller
 
J.L.
,
Sommer
 
M.
,
Wagner
 
V.
, et al. (
2003
).
Abnormalities in emotion processing within cortical and subcortical regions in criminal psychopaths: evidence from a functional magnetic resonance imaging study using pictures with emotional content
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
54
(
2
),
152
62
.

Müller
 
J.L.
,
Gänßbauer
 
S.
,
Sommer
 
M.
, et al. (
2008
).
Gray matter changes in right superior temporal gyrus in criminal psychopaths. Evidence from voxel-based morphometry
.
Psychiatry Research: NeuroImaging
,
163
(
3
),
213
22
.

Murray
 
L.
,
Waller
 
R.
,
Hyde
 
L.W.
(
2018
).
A systematic review examining the link between psychopathic personality traits, antisocial behavior, and neural reactivity during reward and loss processing
.
Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment
,
9
(
6
), 497.

Nummenmaa
 
L.
,
Lukkarinen
 
L.
,
Sun
 
L.
, et al. (
2021
).
Brain basis of psychopathy in criminal offenders and general population
.
Cerebral Cortex
,
31
(
9
),
1
11
.doi: .

Pan
 
N.
,
Wang
 
S.
,
Zhao
 
Y.
, et al. (
2021
).
Brain gray matter structures associated with trait impulsivity: a systematic review and voxel-based meta-analysis
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
42
(
7
),
2214
35
.

Pardini
 
D.A.
,
Raine
 
A.
,
Erickson
 
K.
,
Loeber
 
R.
(
2014
).
Lower amygdala volume in men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic traits, and future violence
.
Biological Psychiatry
,
75
(
1
),
73
80
.

Paulhus
 
D.L.
,
Neumann
 
C.S.
,
Hare
 
R.D.
,
Williams
 
K.M.
,
Hemphill
 
J.F.
(
2015
).
Self-Report Psychopathy Scale
. 4thedn.
Toronto, ON
:
Multi-Health Systems
.

Paulhus
 
D.L.
,
Neumann
 
C.S.
,
Hare
 
R.D.
(
2017
).
Manual for the Self-report Psychopathy Scale
.
Toronto, ON
:
Multi-Health Systems
.

Pujol
 
J.
,
Batalla
 
I.
,
Contreras-Rodríguez
 
O.
, et al. (
2012
).
Breakdown in the brain network subserving moral judgment in criminal psychopathy
.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
,
7
(
8
),
917
23
.

Ross
 
S.R.
,
Benning
 
S.D.
,
Patrick
 
C.J.
,
Thompson
 
A.
,
Thurston
 
A.
(
2009
).
Factors of the psychopathic personality inventory: criterion-related validity and relationship to the BIS/BAS and five-factor models of personality
.
Assessment
,
16
:
71
87
.

Sadeh
 
N.
,
Baskin-Sommers
 
A.
(
2017
).
Risky, impulsive, and self-destructive behavior questionnaire (RISQ): a validation study
.
Assessment
,
24
(
8
),
1080
94
.

Sadeh
 
N.
,
Javdani
 
S.
,
Verona
 
E.
(
2013a
).
Analysis of monoaminergic genes, childhood abuse, and dimensions of psychopathy
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
122
(
1
),
167
79
.

Sadeh
 
N.
,
Spielberg
 
J.M.
,
Heller
 
W.
, et al. (
2013b
).
Emotion disrupts neural activity during selective attention in psychopathy
.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
,
8
(
3
),
235
46
.

Sadeh
 
N.
,
Verona
 
E.
(
2008
).
Psychopathic traits associated with abnormal selective attention and impaired cognitive control
.
Neuropsychology
,
22
:
669
80
.

Sajous-Turner
 
A.
,
Anderson
 
N.E.
,
Widdows
 
M.
, et al. (
2020
).
Aberrant brain gray matter in murderers
.
Brain Imaging and Behavior
,
14
(
5
),
2050
61
.

Seara-Cardoso
 
A.
,
Neumann
 
C.
,
Roiser
 
J.
,
McCrory
 
E.
,
Viding
 
E.
(
2012
).
Investigating associations between empathy, morality and psychopathic personality traits in the general population
.
Personality and Individual Differences
,
52
(
1
),
67
71
.

Smith
 
S.F.
,
Lilienfeld
 
S.O.
(
2015
).
The response modulation hypothesis of psychopathy: a meta-analytic and narrative analysis
.
Psychological Bulletin
,
141
(
6
), 1145.

Spasojević
 
G.
,
Malobabic
 
S.
,
Pilipović-Spasojević
 
O.
,
Djukić-Macut
 
N.
,
Maliković
 
A.
(
2013
).
Morphology and digitally aided morphometry of the human paracentral lobule
.
Folia Morphologica
,
72
(
1
),
10
6
.

Sprague
 
J.
,
Javdani
 
S.
,
Sadeh
 
N.
,
Newman
 
J.P.
,
Verona
 
E.
(
2012
).
Borderline personality disorder as a female phenotypic expression of psychopathy?
 
Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment
,
3
(
2
),
127
39
.

Szabo
 
C.A.
,
Xiong
 
J.
,
Lancaster
 
J.L.
,
Rainey
 
L.
,
Fox
 
P.
(
2001
).
Amygdalar and hippocampal volumetry in control participants: differences regarding handedness
.
American Journal of Neuroradiology
,
22
(
7
),
1342
5
.

Umbach
 
R.
,
Berryessa
 
C.M.
,
Raine
 
A.
(
2015
).
Brain imaging research on psychopathy: implications for punishment, prediction, and treatment in youth and adults
.
Journal of Criminal Justice
,
43
(
4
),
295
306
.

van der Kouwe
 
A.J.W.
,
Benner
 
T.
,
Salat
 
D.H.
,
Fischl
 
B.
(
2008
).
Brain morphometry with multiecho MPRAGE
.
NeuroImage
,
40
(
2
),
559
69
.

Verona
 
E.
,
Patrick
 
C.J.
,
Joiner
 
T.E.
(
2001
).
Psychopathy, antisocial personality, and suicide risk
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
110
(
3
), 462.

Verona
 
E.
,
Sprague
 
J.
,
Javdani
 
S.
(
2012
).
Gender and factor-level interactions in psychopathy: implications for self-directed violence risk and borderline personality disorder symptoms
.
Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment
,
3
(
3
),
247
62
.

Vieira
 
J.B.
,
Ferreira-Santos
 
F.
,
Almeida
 
P.R.
,
Barbosa
 
F.
,
Marques-Teixeira
 
J.
,
Marsh
 
A.A.
(
2015
).
Psychopathic traits are associated with cortical and subcortical volume alterations in healthy individuals
.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
,
10
(
12
),
1693
704
.

Walsh
 
Z.
,
Kosson
 
D.S.
(
2008
).
Psychopathy and violence: the importance of factor level interactions
.
Psychological Assessment
,
20
(
2
),
114
20
.

Wang
 
J.
,
Yang
 
Y.
,
Fan
 
L.
, et al. (
2015
).
Convergent functional architecture of the superior parietal lobule unraveled with multimodal neuroimaging approaches: parcellation of superior parietal lobule
.
Human Brain Mapping
,
36
(
1
),
238
57
.

Weber
 
S.
,
Habel
 
U.
,
Amunts
 
K.
,
Schneider
 
F.
(
2008
).
Structural brain abnormalities in psychopaths—a review
.
Behavioral Sciences and the Law
,
26
(
1
),
7
28
.

Wechsler
 
D.
(
2008
).
Wechsler Adult Intelligence scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV)
. Vol.
22
.
San Antonio, TX
:
NCS Pearson
,
816
27
.

Yang
 
Y.
,
Raine
 
A.
,
Colletti
 
P.
,
Toga
 
A.W.
,
Narr
 
K.L.
(
2009a
).
Abnormal temporal and prefrontal cortical gray matter thinning in psychopaths
.
Molecular Psychiatry
,
14
(
6
),
561
2
.

Yang
 
Y.
,
Raine
 
A.
,
Narr
 
K.L.
,
Colletti
 
P.
,
Toga
 
A.W.
(
2009b
).
Localization of deformations within the amygdala in individuals with psychopathy
.
Archives of General Psychiatry
,
66
(
9
), 986.

Yang
 
Y.
,
Narr
 
K.L.
,
Baker
 
L.A.
, et al. (
2015
).
Frontal and striatal alterations associated with psychopathic traits in adolescents
.
Psychiatry Research: NeuroImaging
,
231
(
3
),
333
40
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]