Abstract

The premenstrual phase of the menstrual cycle is associated with marked changes in normal and abnormal motivated behaviors. Animal studies suggest that such effects may result from actions of gonadal hormones on the mesolimbic dopamine (DA) system. We therefore investigated premenstrual changes in reward-related neural activity in terminal regions of the DA system in humans. Twenty-eight healthy young women underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging on 2 days during the menstrual cycle, once during the late follicular phase and once during the premenstrual phase, in counterbalanced order. Using a modified version of the monetary incentive delay task, we assessed responsiveness of the ventral striatum to reward anticipation. Our results show enhanced ventral striatal responses during the premenstrual as compared to the follicular phase. Moreover, this effect was most pronounced in women reporting more premenstrual symptoms. These findings provide support for the notion that changes in functioning of mesolimbic incentive processing circuits may underlie premenstrual changes in motivated behaviors. Notably, increases in reward-cue responsiveness have previously been associated with DA withdrawal states. Our findings therefore suggest that the sharp decline of gonadal hormone levels in the premenstrual phase may trigger a similar withdrawal-like state.

INTRODUCTION

The premenstrual phase of the menstrual cycle, when progesterone and estradiol levels decline after a relatively stable period with high levels, is associated with marked changes in normal and abnormal motivated behaviors. For example, increases have been shown of food cravings (Tomelleri and Grunewald, 1987; Dye et al., 1995) and excessive cleaning (Dillon and Brooks, 1992) in healthy women during the premenstrual phase. Such symptoms are aggravated in women with premenstrual syndrome (PMS) and premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) (York et al., 1989; Reed et al., 2008). Moreover, women with obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) and nicotine and cocaine abusers show an exacerbation of compulsive and craving symptoms, respectively, in the premenstrual phase (Allen et al., 2000; Evans et al., 2002; Franklin et al., 2004; Vulink et al., 2006). Because such motivated behaviors are associated with dopaminergic (DA) functioning, these behavioral changes are thought to arise from effects of gonadal hormone fluctuations on the mesolimbic DA system (Chen et al., 1996; Thompson and Moss, 1997; Reddy and Kulkarni, 1998, 1999). Previous human neuroimaging studies have shown menstrual cycle dependent (Fernández et al., 2003; Goldstein et al., 2005; Protopopescu et al., 2005; Dreher et al., 2007; Weis et al., 2008) and exogenous gonadal hormone related changes in brain function (Hermans et al., 2008; van Wingen et al., 2008 and 2009; Hermans et al., 2010). However, neural mechanisms in the premenstrual phase potentially underlying the change in motivated behaviors during this phase have never been investigated in humans.

A substantial body of animal research has shown that gonadal hormones exert actions on the mesolimbic DA system (Kuppers et al., 2000; Mani, 2006). Estrogen and progesteron receptors are expressed in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), a terminal region of the mesolimbic DA system (Roy et al., 1986; Shughrue et al., 1998; McEwen, 2002; Mani, 2008). Both progesterone and estrogens have been shown to increase DA turnover in this region (Di Paolo et al., 1985; Bazzett and Becker, 1994; Di Paolo, 1994; Thompson and Moss, 1994; Petitclerc et al., 1995; Le Saux et al., 2006; Larson and Carroll, 2007). The striatum moreover exhibits high accumulations of neuroactive metabolites of progesterone such as allopregnanolone, which are allosteric modulators of the γ-aminobutyric acid type A (GABA-A) receptor (Bixo et al., 1986; Wang et al., 1995). The decline in gonadal hormone levels in the premenstrual phase may therefore explain alterations in DA-related motivated behaviors. In support of this notion, animal studies have found that experimentally induced hormone fluctuations can result in concomitant increases in DA-related motivated behaviors such as compulsions (Flaisher-Grinberg et al., 2009).

Human studies using blood oxygenation level dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging (BOLD-fMRI) have repeatedly demonstrated reward-related neural activity in a set of regions that are innervated by DA projections from the mesolimbic pathway (Knutson and Cooper, 2005; McBride et al., 2006). BOLD responses in the ventral striatum (the approximate location of the NAcc) have been shown to be particularly strong during reward anticipation (Knutson et al., 2001a and b), and when instrumental responses are required to attain rewards (Bjork and Hommer, 2007). Moreover, responses in this region to food-related stimuli have been shown to be increased when participants are in a state of hunger (LaBar et al., 2001). Increased ventral striatal BOLD responses to drug-related cues have been associated with craving and drug withdrawal (Kilts et al., 2001, 2004; Rolls and McCabe, 2007; Wrase et al., 2007), although opposite effects have also been observed for non-drug related reward cues (Wrase et al., 2007; van Hell et al., 2010). Based on behavioral findings described above, we expected to find a more generally increased reward sensitivity in women in the premenstrual phase, and therefore predicted that ventral striatal activation during anticipation of monetary rewards would be enhanced in the premenstrual phase. In addition, we explored whether cycle-dependent changes in ventral striatal activity would be associated with inter-individual differences in neuroactive steroid fluctuations and self-report measures of premenstrual symptoms.

To address these questions, 28 healthy women underwent fMRI once in the premenstrual phase and once in the late follicular phase of the menstrual cycle in a counterbalanced crossover design. Women were scanned in these particular menstrual cycle phases because PMS and PMDD patients are most symptomatic in the premenstrual phase and are least symptomatic in the late follicular phase (Sveindottir and Bäckström, 2000). In the MRI scanner, participants performed a modified version of the monetary incentive delay (MID) task (Knutson et al., 2001a) in which cues were presented that either signaled trials that were potentially rewarding or non-rewarding. Interindividual differences in premenstrual symptoms were assessed using the Moos Menstrual Distress Questionnaire (MDQ; Moos, 1968). Additionally, we collected saliva for measuring neuroactive steroid levels (allopregnanolone). For menstrual cycle phase verification, we used ovulation predictor kits and obtained self-reports of the onset of menstruation before and after test sessions.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Twenty-eight healthy right-handed women (mean age: 22.8 years, range: 18–38 years) participated in this study. None of the women had used hormonal contraceptives for the previous 3 months and all had regular menstrual cycles (26–32 days over the past year). Women were excluded when they consumed more than three alcoholic beverages per day on average and smoked more than 20 cigarettes per week. In addition, they were asked not to consume alcohol 24 h prior to the experiment or to use psychoactive substances for 72 h. They had no history of psychiatric disorders (as determined with the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview, M.I.N.I) (Sheehan et al., 1998) and were additionally screened specifically for PMDD or PMS using the Dutch version of the MDQ (Moos, 1968). All women were physically healthy and reported no MRI contraindications. Data of one woman were excluded from data analysis due to excessive head movement during scanning. The study was approved by the local ethical review board (CMO Region Arnhem-Nijmegen, The Netherlands) and all women provided written informed consent.

Design and procedure

Women were tested in a crossover design with the counterbalanced factors menstrual cycle phase (late follicular vs premenstrual) and condition (potentially rewarding vs non-rewarding) as within subject factors. During a screening day prior to scanning sessions, subjects completed the MINI (Sheehan et al., 1998) and the MDQ (Moos, 1968). Each woman was scanned in the afternoon or evening once during the late follicular phase and once during the premenstrual phase. Time of day was kept equal for both test sessions (within participants). Timing of test sessions within the menstrual cycle phase was ascertained as follows. For the late follicular phase, test sessions were planned between Days 8 and 12 (mean time point of test session: Day 10, s.d.: 1 day) with respect to the first day of the menstrual cycle (i.e. start of menstruation). To determine the premenstrual phase, two time points were used. First, participants contacted the experimenter when they had a positive result from their LH-peak assessments (as determined using commercially available ovulation predictor kits; Wondfo Biotech, Guangzhou, China). Subsequently, an appointment was made for premenstrual scanning ∼14 days after the start of ovulation. To ascertain that this test session took place in the premenstrual phase, participants were asked to contact the experimenter again when their next menstruation started (mean time point of test session: 2 days before menstruation started, s.d.: 1 day). Menstrual cycle phases could further be verified retrospectively using salivary neuroactive steroid concentrations (see below).

On testing days, participants arrived between 2 and 7 pm. Approximately 1.5 h before scanning, subjects practiced the MID task outside the scanner. In order to measure salivary concentrations of allopregnanolone, 10 ml of saliva was collected at the beginning of each test session (5 min after arrival). Saliva was collected in plastic tubes and kept frozen at −20°C. Participants lay in the scanner in a supine position. During and between preparation scans, participants performed a training run of the MID.

MID task

This task was based on the MID task developed by Knutson et al. (2001) and consisted of 25 potentially rewarding trials, 25 non-rewarding trials and 25 periods of low-level fixation with a mean duration equal to trials. In total, trials lasted between 8.5 and 11.5 s (mean 10 s). Thus, the total duration of the task was 12.5 min. At the beginning of each trial, a cue (cue duration: 3.5–8.5 s; mean: 6 s) was presented signaling a potentially rewarding (red square) or non-rewarding (green square) trial. Following this cue, a target was presented to which subjects had to respond as fast as possible (by pressing a button) irrespective of the cue type. When the button was pushed within the presentation time of the circle, the target remained on the screen, thus providing the participant with feedback that the target was hit. Otherwise, it disappeared. When the target was hit in a rewarding trial, they earned one euro. After disappearance of the target (duration: 1.2–5.3 s; mean 3.25 s), short feedback was provided (500 ms) of the current cumulative gain. To ascertain that reward outcome was similar across participants and sessions, the target duration was variable (150–500 ms) and shortened with 20 ms for the subsequent trial when the previous target was hit. The target duration was lengthened with 10 ms in the subsequent trial when the previous target was missed. This procedure results in a hit rate of ∼33% on average, ensuring that all participants won approximately the same amount of money (between 8 and 11 euros). Prior to the experiment, practice trials were presented outside and inside the scanner to familiarize the participants with the task. They were required to hit the target in five and three potentially rewarding trials, respectively, before procedures continued. This task was performed twice, once during the late follicular phase and once during the premenstrual phase.

MR data acquisition

MRI scans were collected using a Siemens (Erlangen, Germany) TIM Trio 3.0 T MRI scanner equipped with an eight channel phased array head coil. The following scans were obtained: two runs of 402 T2* weighted BOLD images each (gradient echo EPI, TE/TR: 25/1890 ms, flip angle 80°, FOV: 212 × 212 mm, matrix 64 × 64, 3-mm slice thickness, 0.3 mm slice gap, 37 ascending slices. To reduce signal drop-out and geometric distortions, we used a short TE, an oblique axial angulation (de Zwart, 2006), and reduced echo-train length by means of factor 2 accelerated GRAPPA (Griswold et al., 2002). Structural scans were obtained using an MP-RAGE sequence (TE/TR: 2.96/2300 ms, flip angle: 8°, FOV: 256 × 256 × 192 mm, voxel size: 1 mm isotropic, GRAPPA acceleration factor 2).

Analysis of behavioral performance

For each participant, reaction time (RT) data of responses to targets during the modified MID task were filtered by removing values outside the 150–1000 ms range. In addition, those RTs exceeding 3 s.d's from the mean were removed. Furthermore, one participant, who had mean RTs > 3 s.d. from the group average, was excluded from further RT analyses. RTs were analysed using a repeated measures ANOVA with menstrual cycle phase and reward condition as within-subject factors. Alpha was set at 0.05. As a result of the adaptive procedure adjusting the target presentation time (see above), the total amount of monetary gain in the MID task does not reflect performance.

Analysis of fMRI data

fMRI data were analysed using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM5; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London). To allow for T1 equilibration, the first five EPI-volumes of each run were discarded. The remaining images were realigned to the first volume, slice time corrected, coregistered to the structural MR image, spatially normalized to standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 coordinate space, resampled into 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 voxels, and smoothed with an isotropic 8-mm full-width half maximum Gaussian kernel.

Statistical analysis was performed within the framework of the general linear model. For each of the two runs, the rewarding and non-rewarding trials were modeled as separate regressors in an event-related manner for the duration of the anticipation cue (i.e. between 3.5 and 8.5 s for both the red and the green square). Subsequently, these regressors were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function implemented in SPM5. The six parameters corresponding with head movement obtained from the realignment procedure were also included in the model for both sessions, as well as a high-pass filter with a 1/128 Hz cut-off frequency. We applied proportional global signal scaling to reduce effects due to global signal variations between scan sessions. The single subject parameter estimates of each session and condition obtained from the first level analysis were included into subsequent second level analyses treating subjects as random variables. A repeated measures ANOVA was used, with menstrual cycle phase (late follicular vs late premenstrual) and reward condition (potentially rewarding vs non-rewarding) as within-subject factors, with non-sphericity corrections for repeated measures. For our main region of interest (NAcc), a small volume correction (SVC) was used that was based on an anatomical mask of this region. This mask was created as follows: bilateral NAcc was delineated in T1 weighted scans of 60 separate individuals (21 males, 39 females; mean age: 21.9 years, age range: 18–38 years) using an automated segmentation procedure as implemented in FSL FIRST (see http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/first/index.html). Subsequently, all T1-weighted images were normalized into MNI152 space using SPM5 as described earlier, and the same transformation was applied to all segmented images. After visual inspection, all segmented images (boolean maps) were averaged, resulting in a probability mask. This mask was thresholded at a probability of 0.75 (total volume: 2179 mm3). Given the uncertainties in exact localization of a small area such as the NAcc, we refer to the region covered by this ROI mask with the more cautious term ventral striatum. A FWE correction for multiple comparisons across the entire brain was used for all other brain regions.

For additional correlation analyses with allopregnanolone changes across the menstrual cycle and MDQ scores, contrast estimates reflecting the premenstrual increase in reward-related responses were extracted from the largest ventral striatal cluster (consisting of 16 voxels in the left ventral striatum) in the interaction effect (peak voxel at −6, 20, −4; ‘Results’ section). The MDQ consists of eight different subscales (pain, water resistance, autonomic reactions, negative affect, impaired concentration, behavioral changes, arousal and control), and for every subscale a score for three cycle phases (during menstruation, premenstrual and rest of the cycle) was obtained. For every subscale, we calculated a difference score reflecting premenstrual symptoms (premenstrual vs rest of the cycle). Subsequently, by taking the sum across all subscales, a total MDQ difference score was calculated, which was used for the correlational analysis. A similar analysis was performed with the difference in allopregnanolone levels between the late follicular and the premenstrual phase.

Gonadal steroid analysis

Saliva samples were collected to assess levels of neuroactive steroids (allopregnanolone). The sampling procedure and assay of salivary allopregnanolone has been detailed previously (Ossewaarde et al., 2010). The samples were thawed and allopregnanolone extracted with diethyl ether. A separation of cross reacting steroids was made with high performance liquid chromatography and thereafter a radioimmunoassay (RIA) was made using an allopregnanolone antiserum kindly provided by Dr R.H. Purdy, Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, the Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA (Purdy et al., 1990) and [11,12] 3H-allopregnanolone (Perkin Elmer Life Sciences, Boston, USA). The sensitivity of the assay was 19 pg. The recovery of allopregnanolone was calculated by using spiked saliva samples and was mean 76.2% range (62–92%) and the results are compensated for recovery. The intraassay coefficient of variation was 21% in these saliva samples. Due to the experimental nature and novelty of the allopregnanolone assessment based on saliva, the entire samples were used for this analysis. Note, however, that progesterone and allopregnanolone levels are strongly positively correlated with each (Wang et al., 1996).

RESULTS

Allopregnanolone assessment

The saliva concentrations of allopregnanolone were higher in the premenstrual phase (mean ± s.d., 0.050 nmol/l ± 0.016) than the late follicular phase (mean ± s.d., 0.035 nmol/l ± 0.010) (t25 = 4.67, P < 0.001). Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests confirmed that the distributions of allopregnanolone concentrations in the two cycle phases did not deviate from a normal distribution (both Z < 1). Moreover, distributions contained no outliers. Note that levels of allopregnanolone are tightly linked to levels of progesterone (Wang et al., 1996). Although estradiol and/or progesterone levels are more commonly used for confirmation of menstrual cycle phase, these data therefore do indicate that participants were tested during the intended menstrual cycle phase.

MID Task, RTs

A repeated-measures ANOVA with reward condition (potentially rewarding vs non-rewarding) and menstrual cycle phase (late follicular vs premenstrual) as within subject factors revealed a main effect of reward condition [F(1,25) = 61.50, P < 0.001], indicating that responses to the target during potentially rewarding trials were faster than to non-rewarding trials (mean ± s.d. in ms, premenstrual phase: potentially rewarding 227 ± 20; non-rewarding 261 ± 30, late follicular phase: reward 228 ± 21; non-reward 259 ± 27). There was neither a main effect of menstrual cycle phase [F(1,25) = 0.56, ns], nor an interaction between menstrual cycle phase and reward condition [F(1,25) = 0.21, ns]. The adaptive reinforcement schedule indeed resulted in a rewarding outcome in ∼33% of potentially rewarding trials: observed mean percentages of hits (and s.d.) were 39.2% (4.9%) and 35.9% (6.4%) for potentially rewarding and non-rewarding conditions, respectively.

Functional MRI results

fMRI data were analysed with a second-level repeated measures ANOVA with reward condition (potentially rewarding vs non-rewarding) and menstrual cycle phase (late follicular vs premenstrual) as within subject factors, using SVCs for our main region of interest: the ventral striatum (‘Materials and methods’ section). The main effect of reward condition showed expected activations in ventral and dorsal striatum, midbrain, parietal regions, insula and anterior cingulate gyrus, confirming previous observations (Knutson et al., 2001a, b) (Table 1). Next, we investigated whether reward condition effects differed between the two menstrual cycle phases. A significant interaction between reward condition and menstrual cycle phase was observed in the ventral striatum (P = 0.015, SVC), with larger reward condition effects in the premenstrual phase (Figure 1A, Table 1). Separate tests for the premenstrual and late follicular phases nonetheless showed robust reward condition effects in the ventral striatum in both menstrual cycle phases (both P < 0.001, SVC; Figure 1B and C, Table 1). No main effects were found of the factor menstrual cycle phase in the ventral striatum (P > 0.05, SVC) or other reward-related brain areas (all P > 0.001, uncorrected). Thus, these findings support our hypothesis of enhanced ventral striatal reward-related activity in the premenstrual phase.

Statistical brain activation T-maps showing menstrual cycle phase by reward interaction, and simple effects. (A) Menstrual cycle phase by reward interaction (P < 0.001, uncorrected, for visualization purposes). (B) Reward activity during the late follicular phase (P < 0.05, whole brain FWE corrected). (C) Reward activity during the premenstrual phase (P < 0.05, whole brain FWE corrected). R, right side of the brain; L, left side of the brain; FWE, family wise error.
Fig. 1

Statistical brain activation T-maps showing menstrual cycle phase by reward interaction, and simple effects. (A) Menstrual cycle phase by reward interaction (P < 0.001, uncorrected, for visualization purposes). (B) Reward activity during the late follicular phase (P < 0.05, whole brain FWE corrected). (C) Reward activity during the premenstrual phase (P < 0.05, whole brain FWE corrected). R, right side of the brain; L, left side of the brain; FWE, family wise error.

Table 1

Local maxima for significant areas of activation for the main and simple effects, and menstrual cycle phase × reward interaction

SidexyzT-value
Main effect of reward condition: reward > non-reward
    Ventral striatumR88−47.64**
    Ventral striatumL−86−49.01**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L46549.35***
    CerebellumL−38−54−308.87***
    CerebellumR30−58−266.95***
    InsulaR4620−86.92***
    InsulaL−322806.76***
    Primary motor cortexR52−2505.58***
    Primary motor cortexL−46−6585.87***
    Parahippocampal gyrusR14−24−125.67***
    MidbrainR/L0−18−185.54***
    Mid-cingulate gyrusR16−28445.44***
    HippocampusL−22−24−105.00***
    Middle frontal gyrusR3442265.13***
    Middle frontal gyrusL−3446244.95***
Menstrual cycle phase × reward interaction: premenstrual > late follicular
    Ventral striatumR820−23.54*
    Ventral striatumL−822−23.48*
Premenstrual reward > premenstrual non-reward
    Ventral striatumR1014−66.97**
    Ventral striatumL−88−47.46**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L48507.51***
    CerebellumL−38−54−307.76***
    ThalamusL−6−468.02***
    HippocampusR164−145.43***
    HippocampusL−16−6−105.47***
    InsulaR4620−66.07***
    InsulaL−3228−25.79***
    Middle frontal gyrusR3440265.01***
    Mid-cingulate gyrusR16−30464.97***
Follicular reward > follicular non-reward
    Ventral striatumR88−45.94**
    Ventral striatumL−106−67.28**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L46547.63***
    CerebellumR32−56−285.16***
    CerebellumL−36−52−327.20***
    InsulaR3224−86.13***
    InsulaL4620−66.07***
    MidbrainR/L2−18−185.75***
    Parahippocampal gyrusR12−22−145.46***
    Temporal poleR601025.01***
SidexyzT-value
Main effect of reward condition: reward > non-reward
    Ventral striatumR88−47.64**
    Ventral striatumL−86−49.01**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L46549.35***
    CerebellumL−38−54−308.87***
    CerebellumR30−58−266.95***
    InsulaR4620−86.92***
    InsulaL−322806.76***
    Primary motor cortexR52−2505.58***
    Primary motor cortexL−46−6585.87***
    Parahippocampal gyrusR14−24−125.67***
    MidbrainR/L0−18−185.54***
    Mid-cingulate gyrusR16−28445.44***
    HippocampusL−22−24−105.00***
    Middle frontal gyrusR3442265.13***
    Middle frontal gyrusL−3446244.95***
Menstrual cycle phase × reward interaction: premenstrual > late follicular
    Ventral striatumR820−23.54*
    Ventral striatumL−822−23.48*
Premenstrual reward > premenstrual non-reward
    Ventral striatumR1014−66.97**
    Ventral striatumL−88−47.46**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L48507.51***
    CerebellumL−38−54−307.76***
    ThalamusL−6−468.02***
    HippocampusR164−145.43***
    HippocampusL−16−6−105.47***
    InsulaR4620−66.07***
    InsulaL−3228−25.79***
    Middle frontal gyrusR3440265.01***
    Mid-cingulate gyrusR16−30464.97***
Follicular reward > follicular non-reward
    Ventral striatumR88−45.94**
    Ventral striatumL−106−67.28**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L46547.63***
    CerebellumR32−56−285.16***
    CerebellumL−36−52−327.20***
    InsulaR3224−86.13***
    InsulaL4620−66.07***
    MidbrainR/L2−18−185.75***
    Parahippocampal gyrusR12−22−145.46***
    Temporal poleR601025.01***

*P < 0.05, FWE rate corrected for the reduced search volume (ventral striatum).

**P < 0.001, FWE rate corrected for the reduced search volume (ventral striatum).

***P < 0.05, FWE rate corrected for the entire brain.

R, right; L, left.

Table 1

Local maxima for significant areas of activation for the main and simple effects, and menstrual cycle phase × reward interaction

SidexyzT-value
Main effect of reward condition: reward > non-reward
    Ventral striatumR88−47.64**
    Ventral striatumL−86−49.01**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L46549.35***
    CerebellumL−38−54−308.87***
    CerebellumR30−58−266.95***
    InsulaR4620−86.92***
    InsulaL−322806.76***
    Primary motor cortexR52−2505.58***
    Primary motor cortexL−46−6585.87***
    Parahippocampal gyrusR14−24−125.67***
    MidbrainR/L0−18−185.54***
    Mid-cingulate gyrusR16−28445.44***
    HippocampusL−22−24−105.00***
    Middle frontal gyrusR3442265.13***
    Middle frontal gyrusL−3446244.95***
Menstrual cycle phase × reward interaction: premenstrual > late follicular
    Ventral striatumR820−23.54*
    Ventral striatumL−822−23.48*
Premenstrual reward > premenstrual non-reward
    Ventral striatumR1014−66.97**
    Ventral striatumL−88−47.46**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L48507.51***
    CerebellumL−38−54−307.76***
    ThalamusL−6−468.02***
    HippocampusR164−145.43***
    HippocampusL−16−6−105.47***
    InsulaR4620−66.07***
    InsulaL−3228−25.79***
    Middle frontal gyrusR3440265.01***
    Mid-cingulate gyrusR16−30464.97***
Follicular reward > follicular non-reward
    Ventral striatumR88−45.94**
    Ventral striatumL−106−67.28**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L46547.63***
    CerebellumR32−56−285.16***
    CerebellumL−36−52−327.20***
    InsulaR3224−86.13***
    InsulaL4620−66.07***
    MidbrainR/L2−18−185.75***
    Parahippocampal gyrusR12−22−145.46***
    Temporal poleR601025.01***
SidexyzT-value
Main effect of reward condition: reward > non-reward
    Ventral striatumR88−47.64**
    Ventral striatumL−86−49.01**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L46549.35***
    CerebellumL−38−54−308.87***
    CerebellumR30−58−266.95***
    InsulaR4620−86.92***
    InsulaL−322806.76***
    Primary motor cortexR52−2505.58***
    Primary motor cortexL−46−6585.87***
    Parahippocampal gyrusR14−24−125.67***
    MidbrainR/L0−18−185.54***
    Mid-cingulate gyrusR16−28445.44***
    HippocampusL−22−24−105.00***
    Middle frontal gyrusR3442265.13***
    Middle frontal gyrusL−3446244.95***
Menstrual cycle phase × reward interaction: premenstrual > late follicular
    Ventral striatumR820−23.54*
    Ventral striatumL−822−23.48*
Premenstrual reward > premenstrual non-reward
    Ventral striatumR1014−66.97**
    Ventral striatumL−88−47.46**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L48507.51***
    CerebellumL−38−54−307.76***
    ThalamusL−6−468.02***
    HippocampusR164−145.43***
    HippocampusL−16−6−105.47***
    InsulaR4620−66.07***
    InsulaL−3228−25.79***
    Middle frontal gyrusR3440265.01***
    Mid-cingulate gyrusR16−30464.97***
Follicular reward > follicular non-reward
    Ventral striatumR88−45.94**
    Ventral striatumL−106−67.28**
    Supplementary motor areaR/L46547.63***
    CerebellumR32−56−285.16***
    CerebellumL−36−52−327.20***
    InsulaR3224−86.13***
    InsulaL4620−66.07***
    MidbrainR/L2−18−185.75***
    Parahippocampal gyrusR12−22−145.46***
    Temporal poleR601025.01***

*P < 0.05, FWE rate corrected for the reduced search volume (ventral striatum).

**P < 0.001, FWE rate corrected for the reduced search volume (ventral striatum).

***P < 0.05, FWE rate corrected for the entire brain.

R, right; L, left.

Additionally, we investigated the relationship between menstrual cycle related changes in ventral striatal activity and individual differences in the sensitivity to gonadal hormone changes as reflected by allopregnanolone changes and the total MDQ difference score (displaying premenstrual symptoms as compared to symptoms during the rest of the cycle). Reward-related activation (potentially rewarding vs non-rewarding activity) in the ventral striatum during the late follicular phase was subtracted from activation during the premenstrual phase, yielding an estimate of the interaction effect described earlier. Although there was no significant correlation with menstrual cycle related changes in allopregnanolone levels (P > 0.05), results showed a positive correlation between the interaction effect and total MDQ difference scores [r(27) = 0.46, P < 0.05]. This indicates that women with the highest total MDQ difference scores had the largest premenstrual increase in ventral striatal activity. When exploring the different subscales, negative affect [r(27) = 0.42, P < 0.05], behavioral changes [r(27) = 0.45, P < 0.05] and control [r(27) = 0.54, P < 0.01] showed a significant positive association with the menstrual cycle related change in ventral striatal reward activity. The subscales pain, water resistance, autonomic reactions and arousal did not reach the statistical threshold (all P > 0.05).

DISCUSSION

The aim of the present study was to identify potential neural mechanisms underlying changes in motivated behaviors across the menstrual cycle. As hypothesized, we observed premenstrual increases in reward-related BOLD responses in the ventral striatum, a target region of the mesolimbic DA system. Notably, this premenstrual increase in ventral striatal activity was most pronounced in women who reported more premenstrual symptoms. Together, these findings support the notion that dysregulations of dopaminergic incentive processing circuits underlie premenstrual symptoms related to changes in motivated behaviors.

The observed increase of reward-related responses in the ventral striatum during the premenstrual phase can be explained by two possible neurobiological mechanisms. First, the observed findings may be caused by a delayed effect of changes in absolute levels of gonadal hormones across the menstrual cycle. In line with this notion, serum progesterone concentrations robustly predict negative symptom severity in women with PMS with a delay of ∼3–4 days (Wang et al., 1996). These patients moreover exhibit recurrent food cravings which are related to the steroid and mood state fluctuations across the menstrual cycle (Dye et al., 1995; Dye and Blundell, 1997). Furthermore, it has been shown that especially hedonic intake of palatable food is increased by administration of gonadal steroids, suggesting direct effects on reward seeking (Chen et al., 1996; Reddy and Kulkarni, 1998, 1999). Second, premenstrual increases in ventral striatal responsiveness may be caused by the ‘decline’ of gonadal hormone levels during the premenstrual phase. Because gonadal hormones are known to potentiate DA release (Di Paolo et al., 1985; Bazzett and Becker, 1994; Di Paolo, 1994; Thompson and Moss, 1994; Petitclerc et al., 1995; Le Saux et al., 2006; Larson and Carroll, 2007), their decline in the premenstrual phase after a period of increased levels may prompt a down-regulation of endogenous DA activity and thus mimic a withdrawal state.

Previous studies have shown that dopaminergic withdrawal states are associated with enhanced cue-induced DA release, which is thought to attribute ‘incentive salience’, or motivational value, to drug or reward associated stimuli (Robinson and Berridge, 1993). Notably, in humans, such withdrawal states appear to prompt changes in motivated behavior and its neural substrates that are analogous to the findings reported here, with enhanced ventral striatal responsiveness to food or drug cues (Kilts et al., 2001; Rolls and McCabe, 2007). Therefore, increased ventral striatal responsiveness in the premenstrual phase may result from a similar DA withdrawal state that is caused by the abrupt decline in gonadal hormone levels. To corroborate this notion, future research in humans should investigate whether the present findings generalize to other periods of gonadal hormone decline, such as postpartum or (peri) menopausal periods. Furthermore, effects of controlled hormone withdrawal can be investigated either through termination of hormone replacement therapy or hormonal contraception, or through pharmacological gonadal suppression using administration of gonadotropin-releasing hormone agonists such as leuprolide. Such studies may shed new light on potential gonadal hormone withdrawal effects both on neuronal and behavioral levels.

An important limitation of the present study is that the BOLD-fMRI signal in the ventral striatum cannot provide direct information about dopaminergic neurotransmission. However, a recent fMRI study has shown that BOLD responses during reward anticipation in the ventral striatum correlate positively with DA release (Schott et al., 2008). It has been suggested that the ventral striatal BOLD signal reflects changes in postsynaptic D1-receptor agonism (Knutson and Gibbs, 2007). Pharmacological fMRI studies have indeed shown that DA agonists induce ventral striatal BOLD responses (Breiter et al., 1997; Vollm et al., 2004). Although this suggests that the ventral striatal BOLD signal is related to DA function, our findings do not necessarily represent elevated DA release specifically. Moreover, conclusions regarding the specificity of our finding to reward anticipation are limited by the fact that our task only included trials with anticipated rewards and non-rewarding control trials.

In conclusion, the present study demonstrated enhanced reward-related neural responses in the ventral striatum during the premenstrual phase. These findings provide support for the notion that changes in functioning of mesolimbic incentive processing circuits may underlie premenstrual increases in normal and abnormal motivated behaviors such as food and drug cravings. These effects are likely caused by the sudden decrease in gonadal hormone levels during the premenstrual phase and may thus reflect a withdrawal-like state. The results of the present study may generalize to other periods of instability in the gonadal hormone milieu and may therefore provide novel insights into the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders associated with gonadal hormonal fluctuations.

This work was supported by grant numbers 918.66.613 and 451.07.019 from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) and grant no. 14x-11198 from the Swedish research council.

REFERENCES

Allen
SS
Hatsukami
D
Christianson
D
Brown
S
,
Effects of transdermal nicotine on craving, withdrawal and premenstrual symptomatology in short-term smoking abstinence during different phases of the menstrual cycle
Nicotine Tobacco Research
,
2000
, vol.
2
(pg.
231
-
41
)
Bazzett
TJ
Becker
JB
,
Sex differences in the rapid and acute effects of estrogen on striatal D2 dopamine receptor binding
Brain Research
,
1994
, vol.
637
(pg.
163
-
72
)
Bixo
M
Bäckström
T
Winblad
B
Selstam
G
Andersson
A
,
Comparison between pre- and postovulatory distributions of oestradiol and progesterone in the brain of the PMSG-treated rat
Acta Physiologica Scandinavica
,
1986
, vol.
128
(pg.
241
-
6
)
Bjork
JM
Hommer
DW
,
Anticipating instrumentally obtained and passively-received rewards: a factorial fMRI investigation
Behavioural Brain Research
,
2007
, vol.
177
(pg.
165
-
70
)
Breiter
HC
Gollub
RL
Weisskoff
RM
et al.
,
Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion
Neuron
,
1997
, vol.
19
(pg.
591
-
611
)
Chen
SW
Rodriguez
L
Davies
MF
Loew
GH
,
The hyperphagic effect of 3 alpha-hydroxylated pregnane steroids in male rats
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior
,
1996
, vol.
53
(pg.
777
-
82
)
de Zwart
JA
van Gelderen
P
Golay
X
Ikonomidou
VN
Duyn
JH
,
Accelerated parallel imaging for functional imaging of the human brain
NMR Biomed
,
2006
, vol.
19
(pg.
342
-
51
)
Di Paolo
T
,
Modulation of brain dopamine transmission by sex steroids
Reviews in the Neurosciences
,
1994
, vol.
5
(pg.
27
-
41
)
Di Paolo
T
Rouillard
C
Bedard
P
,
17 beta-Estradiol at a physiological dose acutely increases dopamine turnover in rat brain
European Journal of Pharmacology
,
1985
, vol.
117
(pg.
197
-
203
)
Dillon
KM
Brooks
D
,
Unusual cleaning behavior in the luteal phase
Psychological Reports
,
1992
, vol.
70
(pg.
35
-
9
)
Dreher
JC
Schmidt
PJ
Kohn
P
et al.
,
Menstrual cycle phase modulates reward-related neural function in women
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
2007
, vol.
104
(pg.
2465
-
70
)
Dye
L
Blundell
JE
,
Menstrual cycle and appetite control: implications for weight regulation
Human Reproduction
,
1997
, vol.
12
(pg.
1142
-
51
)
Dye
L
Warner
P
Bancroft
J
,
Food craving during the menstrual cycle and its relationship to stress, happiness of relationship and depression; a preliminary enquiry
Journal of Affective Disorders
,
1995
, vol.
34
(pg.
157
-
64
)
Evans
SM
Haney
M
Foltin
RW
,
The effects of smoked cocaine during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle in women
Psychopharmacology
,
2002
, vol.
159
(pg.
397
-
406
)
Fernández
G
Weis
S
Stoffel-Wagner
B
et al.
,
Menstrual cycle-dependent neural plasticity in the adult human brain is hormone, task, and region specific
Journal of Neuroscience
,
2003
, vol.
23
(pg.
3790
-
5
)
Flaisher-Grinberg
S
Albelda
N
Gitter
L
et al.
,
Ovarian hormones modulate ‘compulsive’ lever-pressing in female rats
Hormones and Behavior
,
2009
, vol.
55
(pg.
356
-
65
)
Franklin
TR
Napier
K
Ehrman
R
et al.
,
Retrospective study: influence of menstrual cycle on cue-induced cigarette craving
Nicotine Tobacco Research
,
2004
, vol.
6
(pg.
171
-
5
)
Goldstein
JM
Jerram
M
Poldrack
R
et al.
,
Hormonal cycle modulates arousal circuitry in women using functional magnetic resonance imaging
Journal of Neuroscience
,
2005
, vol.
25
(pg.
9309
-
16
)
Griswold
MA
Jakob
P
Heidemann
R
et al.
,
Generalized autocalibrating partially parallel acquisitions (GRAPPA)
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine
,
2002
, vol.
47
(pg.
1202
-
10
)
Hermans
EJ
Ramsey
NF
van Honk
J
,
Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans
Biological Psychiatry
,
2008
, vol.
63
(pg.
263
-
70
)
Hermans
EJ
Bos
PA
Ossewaarde
L
et al.
,
Effects of exogenous testosterone on the ventral striatal BOLD response during reward anticipation in healthy women
Neuroimage
,
2010
, vol.
52
(pg.
277
-
83
)
Kilts
CD
Gross
RE
Ely
TD
Drexler
KP
,
The neural correlates of cue-induced craving in cocaine-dependent women
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
2004
, vol.
161
(pg.
233
-
41
)
Kilts
CD
Schweitzer
JB
Quinn
CK
et al.
,
Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction
Archives of General Psychiatry
,
2001
, vol.
58
(pg.
334
-
41
)
Knutson
B
Adams
CM
Fong
GW
Hommer
D
,
Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens
Journal of Neuroscience
,
2001
, vol.
21
pg.
RC159
Knutson
B
Cooper
JC
,
Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction
Current Opinion in Neurology
,
2005
, vol.
18
(pg.
411
-
7
)
Knutson
B
Fong
GW
Adams
CM
Varner
JL
Hommer
D
,
Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI
Neuroreport
,
2001
, vol.
12
(pg.
3683
-
7
)
Knutson
B
Gibbs
SE
,
Linking nucleus accumbens dopamine and blood oxygenation
Psychopharmacology
,
2007
, vol.
191
(pg.
813
-
22
)
Kuppers
E
Ivanova
T
Karolczak
M
Beyer
C
,
Estrogen: a multifunctional messenger to nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons
Journal of Neurocytology
,
2000
, vol.
29
(pg.
375
-
85
)
LaBar
KS
Gitelman
DR
Parrish
TB
et al.
,
Hunger selectively modulates corticolimbic activation to food stimuli in humans
Behavioral Neuroscience
,
2001
, vol.
115
(pg.
493
-
500
)
Larson
EB
Carroll
ME
,
Estrogen receptor beta, but not alpha, mediates estrogen's effect on cocaine-induced reinstatement of extinguished cocaine-seeking behavior in ovariectomized female rats
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2007
, vol.
32
(pg.
1334
-
45
)
Le Saux
M
Morissette
M
Di Paolo
T
,
ERbeta mediates the estradiol increase of D2 receptors in rat striatum and nucleus accumbens
Neuropharmacology
,
2006
, vol.
50
(pg.
451
-
7
)
Mani
S
,
Progestin receptor subtypes in the brain: the known and the unknown
Endocrinology
,
2008
, vol.
149
(pg.
2750
-
6
)
Mani
SK
,
Signaling mechanisms in progesterone-neurotransmitter interactions
Neuroscience
,
2006
, vol.
138
(pg.
773
-
81
)
McBride
D
Barrett
SP
Kelly
JT
Aw
A
Dagher
A
,
Effects of expectancy and abstinence on the neural response to smoking cues in cigarette smokers: an fMRI study
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2006
, vol.
31
(pg.
2728
-
38
)
McEwen
B
,
Estrogen actions throughout the brain
Recent Progress in Hormone Research
,
2002
, vol.
57
(pg.
357
-
84
)
Moos
RH
,
The development of a menstrual distress questionnaire
Psychosomatic Medicine
,
1968
, vol.
30
(pg.
853
-
67
)
Ossewaarde
L
Hermans
EJ
van Wingen
GA
et al.
,
Neural mechanisms underlying changes in stress-sensitivity across the menstrual cycle
Psychoneuroendocrinology
,
2010
, vol.
35
(pg.
47
-
55
)
Petitclerc
M
Bedard
PJ
Di Paolo
T
,
Progesterone releases dopamine in male and female rat striatum: a behavioral and microdialysis study
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
,
1995
, vol.
19
(pg.
491
-
7
)
Protopopescu
X
Pan
H
Altemus
M
et al.
,
Orbitofrontal cortex activity related to emotional processing changes across the menstrual cycle
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
2005
, vol.
102
(pg.
16060
-
5
)
Purdy
RH
Moore
PH
Jr
Rao
PN
et al.
,
Radioimmunoassay of 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha-pregnan-20-one in rat and human plasma
Steroids
,
1990
, vol.
55
(pg.
290
-
6
)
Reddy
DS
Kulkarni
SK
,
The role of GABA-A and mitochondrial diazepam-binding inhibitor receptors on the effects of neurosteroids on food intake in mice
Psychopharmacology
,
1998
, vol.
137
(pg.
391
-
400
)
Reddy
DS
Kulkarni
SK
,
Sex and estrous cycle-dependent changes in neurosteroid and benzodiazepine effects on food consumption and plus-maze learning behaviors in rats
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior
,
1999
, vol.
62
(pg.
53
-
60
)
Reed
SC
Levin
FR
Evans
SM
,
Changes in mood, cognitive performance and appetite in the late luteal and follicular phases of the menstrual cycle in women with and without PMDD (premenstrual dysphoric disorder)
Hormones and Behavior
,
2008
, vol.
54
(pg.
185
-
93
)
Robinson
TE
Berridge
KC
,
The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction
Brain Research Brain Research Reviews
,
1993
, vol.
18
(pg.
247
-
91
)
Rolls
ET
McCabe
C
,
Enhanced affective brain representations of chocolate in cravers vs. non-cravers
European Journal of Neuroscience
,
2007
, vol.
26
(pg.
1067
-
76
)
Roy
EJ
Wilson
MA
Kelley
DB
,
Estrogen-induced progestin receptors in the brain and pituitary of the South African clawed frog, Xenopus laevis
Neuroendocrinology
,
1986
, vol.
42
(pg.
51
-
6
)
Schott
BH
Minuzzi
L
Krebs
RM
et al.
,
Mesolimbic functional magnetic resonance imaging activations during reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ventral striatal dopamine release
Journal of Neuroscience
,
2008
, vol.
28
(pg.
14311
-
19
)
Sheehan
DV
Lecrubier
Y
Sheehan
KH
et al.
,
The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
,
1998
, vol.
59
(pg.
22
-
33
)
Shughrue
PJ
Scrimo
PJ
Merchenthaler
I
,
Evidence for the colocalization of estrogen receptor-beta mRNA and estrogen receptor-alpha immunoreactivity in neurons of the rat forebrain
Endocrinology
,
1998
, vol.
139
(pg.
5267
-
70
)
Sveindottir
H
Bäckström
T
,
Prevalence of menstrual cycle symptom cyclicity and premenstrual dysphoric disorder in a random sample of women using and not using oral contraceptives
Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica
,
2000
, vol.
79
(pg.
405
-
13
)
Thompson
TL
Moss
RL
,
Estrogen regulation of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens: genomic- and nongenomic-mediated effects
Journal of Neurochemistry
,
1994
, vol.
62
(pg.
1750
-
6
)
Thompson
TL
Moss
RL
,
Modulation of mesolimbic dopaminergic activity over the rat estrous cycle
Neuroscience Letters
,
1997
, vol.
229
(pg.
145
-
8
)
Tomelleri
R
Grunewald
KK
,
Menstrual cycle and food cravings in young college women
Journal of the American Dietetic Association
,
1987
, vol.
87
(pg.
311
-
5
)
van Hell
HH
Vink
M
Ossewaarde
L
et al.
,
Chronic effects of cannabis use on the human reward system: an fMRI study
European Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2010
, vol.
20
(pg.
153
-
63
)
van Wingen
GA
van Broekhoven
F
Verkes
RJ
et al.
,
Progesterone selectively increases amygdala reactivity in women
Molecular Psychiatry
,
2008
, vol.
13
(pg.
325
-
33
)
van Wingen
GA
Zylicz
SA
Pieters
S
et al.
,
Testosterone increases amygdala reactivity in middle-aged women to a young adulthood level
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2009
, vol.
34
(pg.
539
-
47
)
Vollm
BA
de Araujo
IE
Cowen
PJ
et al.
,
Methamphetamine activates reward circuitry in drug naive human subjects
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2004
, vol.
29
(pg.
1715
-
22
)
Vulink
NC
Denys
D
Bus
L
Westenberg
HG
,
Female hormones affect symptom severity in obsessive-compulsive disorder
International Clinical Psychopharmacology
,
2006
, vol.
21
(pg.
171
-
5
)
Wang
M
Seippel
L
Purdy
RH
Bäckström
T
,
Relationship between symptom severity and steroid variation in women with premenstrual syndrome: study on serum pregnenolone, pregnenolone sulfate, 5 alpha-pregnane-3,20-dione and 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha-pregnan-20-one
Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism
,
1996
, vol.
81
(pg.
1076
-
82
)
Wang
MD
Wahlström
G
Gee
KW
Bäckström
T
,
Potency of lipid and protein formulation of 5 alpha-pregnanolone at induction of anaesthesia and the corresponding regional brain distribution
British Journal of Anaesthesia
,
1995
, vol.
74
(pg.
553
-
7
)
Weis
S
Hausmann
M
Stoffers
B
et al.
,
Estradiol modulates functional brain organization during the menstrual cycle: an analysis of interhemispheric inhibition
Journal of Neuroscience
,
2008
, vol.
28
(pg.
13401
-
10
)
Wrase
J
Schlagenhauf
F
Kienast
T
et al.
,
Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics
Neuroimage
,
2007
, vol.
35
(pg.
787
-
94
)
York
R
Freeman
E
Lowery
B
Strauss
JF
,
Characteristics of premenstrual syndrome
Obstetrics and Gynecology
,
1989
, vol.
73
(pg.
601
-
5
)
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]