Abstract

Our two closest living primate relatives, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus), exhibit significant behavioral differences despite belonging to the same genus and sharing a very recent common ancestor. Differences have been reported in multiple aspects of social behavior, including aggression, sex, play and cooperation. However, the neurobiological basis of these differences has only been minimally investigated and remains uncertain. Here, we present the first ever comparison of chimpanzee and bonobo brains using diffusion tensor imaging, supplemented with a voxel-wise analysis of T1-weighted images to specifically compare neural circuitry implicated in social cognition. We find that bonobos have more gray matter in brain regions involved in perceiving distress in both oneself and others, including the right dorsal amygdala and right anterior insula. Bonobos also have a larger pathway linking the amygdala with the ventral anterior cingulate cortex, a pathway implicated in both top–down control of aggressive impulses as well as bottom–up biases against harming others. We suggest that this neural system not only supports increased empathic sensitivity in bonobos, but also behaviors like sex and play that serve to dissipate tension, thereby limiting distress and anxiety to levels conducive with prosocial behavior.

INTRODUCTION

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) are our closest living primate relatives, having diverged from a common ancestor with humans ∼6 million years ago (Goodman et al., 1990), and from each other just 1–2 million years ago (Becquet et al., 2007; Hey, 2010). Despite their close phylogenetic relationship, chimpanzees and bonobos exhibit noteworthy differences in temperament and behavior, with bonobos exhibiting less severe aggression (Goodall, 1986; Kano, 1992; Wrangham and Peterson, 1996; Wrangham, 1999; Parish and de Waal, 2000), more adult play (Palagi, 2006), greater variety and frequency of sexual behaviors (de Waal, 1987, 1995; Parish, 1996), greater stress reactivity (Wobber et al., 2010b), greater social tolerance (Hare et al., 2007; Hare and Kwetuenda, 2010) and behaviors that may be indicative of greater empathy (de Waal, 1997) compared with chimpanzees.

The neurobiological bases of these species differences in behavior have been only minimally investigated, largely due to the very limited availability of bonobo brains. A comparative in vivo MRI study found no significant differences in brain size between the two species (n = 4 bonobos and n = 6 chimpanzees) after controlling for body size (Rilling and Insel, 1999). A later study based on a sub-set of these MRI scans showed that the dorsal sector of the frontal lobe, as contrasted with the orbital and mesial sectors, occupies a larger proportion of the frontal lobe in chimpanzees than in bonobos (Schenker et al., 2005). Another study used the same MRI scans from living bonobos, supplemented with additional post-mortem scans, to compare the volume of seven brain regions in chimpanzees and bonobos. Chimpanzees were found to have a larger cerebellum than bonobos after adjusting for brain size, and bonobos had greater leftward asymmetries in the striatum and motor hand area compared with chimpanzees (Hopkins et al., 2009). Finally, a histological study compared the volume of the amygdaloid complex, including the basolateral division specifically, in a sample of great apes that included three chimpanzees and two bonobos. Although the sample size was too small to conclusively demonstrate significant species differences, there was a suggestion that bonobos had a larger lateral nucleus after correcting for brain size (Barger et al., 2007).

Here, for the first time, we examine interspecies differences using two approaches that have the potential to reveal detailed brain-wide differences both in gray matter regions, and in the white matter circuitry that connects these regions. First, we use voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to show differences between local gray matter architecture in the two species. Second, we use MR diffusion imaging and tractography to show differences in white matter pathways. By focusing on brain regions that show inter-species differences both in local gray matter architecture and in associated white matter pathways, we are able to highlight evolutionary changes in neural circuitry at the network level.

In light of the above-specified differences in social behavior, we hypothesized that bonobos would have increased gray matter as well as increased white matter connectivity in brain regions implicated in empathy, emotion regulation, sexual behavior and anxiety. These regions include the anterior insula, amygdala, hypothalamus, and orbitofrontal cortex.

METHODS

Subjects

This study included neuroimaging data from a total of six bonobos and seven chimpanzees. In vivo, T1-weighted scans were obtained from three bonobos and four chimpanzees. In addition, diffusion-weighted scans were acquired from an independent sample of three bonobo and three chimpanzee cadaver brains. No animals were sacrificed for the purposes of this study, and all procedures were approved by the Emory University Animal Care and Use Committee. All cadaver brains were immersion fixed in formalin. Post-mortem intervals are listed for those specimens that they are known for in Supplementary Table S1. Demographic characteristics for both samples are provided in Table 1. All animals were at least 8 years of age, and adult brain weight is achieved by 7 years in chimpanzees (Herndon et al., 1999).

Table 1

Sample demographics

SpeciesSpecimen typeNameSexAge (years)Brain volume (mm3)Body weight (g)
BonoboIn vivoJillF11.5374 33836 700.00
BonoboIn vivoBrianM8342 14835 000.00
BonoboIn vivoLorelM28356 33241 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoLazM20415 92459 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoKengeeF8390 05843 000.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoMervM21428 80162 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoMaryF36357 82744 000.00
BonoboCadaverMamboM15407 862
BonoboCadaverPsukeM27352 108
BonoboCadaverTamuliF13318 844
ChimpanzeeCadaverPolyannaF14397 504
ChimpanzeeCadaverJendaF48354 375
ChimpanzeeCadaverArtifeeF28346 373
SpeciesSpecimen typeNameSexAge (years)Brain volume (mm3)Body weight (g)
BonoboIn vivoJillF11.5374 33836 700.00
BonoboIn vivoBrianM8342 14835 000.00
BonoboIn vivoLorelM28356 33241 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoLazM20415 92459 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoKengeeF8390 05843 000.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoMervM21428 80162 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoMaryF36357 82744 000.00
BonoboCadaverMamboM15407 862
BonoboCadaverPsukeM27352 108
BonoboCadaverTamuliF13318 844
ChimpanzeeCadaverPolyannaF14397 504
ChimpanzeeCadaverJendaF48354 375
ChimpanzeeCadaverArtifeeF28346 373
Table 1

Sample demographics

SpeciesSpecimen typeNameSexAge (years)Brain volume (mm3)Body weight (g)
BonoboIn vivoJillF11.5374 33836 700.00
BonoboIn vivoBrianM8342 14835 000.00
BonoboIn vivoLorelM28356 33241 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoLazM20415 92459 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoKengeeF8390 05843 000.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoMervM21428 80162 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoMaryF36357 82744 000.00
BonoboCadaverMamboM15407 862
BonoboCadaverPsukeM27352 108
BonoboCadaverTamuliF13318 844
ChimpanzeeCadaverPolyannaF14397 504
ChimpanzeeCadaverJendaF48354 375
ChimpanzeeCadaverArtifeeF28346 373
SpeciesSpecimen typeNameSexAge (years)Brain volume (mm3)Body weight (g)
BonoboIn vivoJillF11.5374 33836 700.00
BonoboIn vivoBrianM8342 14835 000.00
BonoboIn vivoLorelM28356 33241 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoLazM20415 92459 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoKengeeF8390 05843 000.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoMervM21428 80162 500.00
ChimpanzeeIn vivoMaryF36357 82744 000.00
BonoboCadaverMamboM15407 862
BonoboCadaverPsukeM27352 108
BonoboCadaverTamuliF13318 844
ChimpanzeeCadaverPolyannaF14397 504
ChimpanzeeCadaverJendaF48354 375
ChimpanzeeCadaverArtifeeF28346 373

Image acquisition

In vivo scans.

As described previously (Rilling and Insel, 1999; Rilling and Seligman, 2002), prior to scanning, animals were anesthetized with Ketamine (10 mg/kg) and then weighed. For ketamine injections, animals were placed in a squeeze cage. Animals were encouraged to voluntarily present a hip for i.m. injection, but in some cases it was necessary to use the squeeze mechanism to restrain the animals for injection. Subjects were scanned in a supine position using the human head coil. Throughout the scan, animals received a continuous IV infusion of propofol (10–20 mg/kg/h) for anesthesia. T1-weighted images of the entire brain were acquired with a 1.5 T Phillips NT scanner (Philips Medical Systems, The Netherlands) using a gradient-echo protocol with the following parameters: slice thickness = 1.2 mm, slice interval = 0.6 mm, repetition time (TR) = 19.0 ms, echo time (TE) = 8.5 ms, number of signals averaged = 8, matrix = 256 × 256, field of view (FOV) = 180 mm. In one chimpanzee, the field of view was increased to 200 mm to accommodate the entire brain, and slice thickness and interval were adjusted to 1.4 and 0.7 mm, respectively.

Post-mortem scans.

Brains were removed from the formalin solution that they were stored in, rinsed with phosphate buffer, patted dry with paper towels and then sealed in a thin-walled plastic bag for scanning. Diffusion-weighted scans were acquired on a 4.7 T Bruker scanner with a spin-echo sequence at 1.5 mm isotropic resolution with a b value of 4500 s mm−2 and 60 diffusion directions (Figure 1). We acquired two sets of diffusion-weighted data for subsequent averaging as well as five volumes with no diffusion weighting. Scan duration was 24 h.

Horizontal section through FA color map in one chimpanzee (left) and one bonobo (right).
Fig. 1

Horizontal section through FA color map in one chimpanzee (left) and one bonobo (right).

Image analysis

In vivo scans.

Brain volumes were estimated from the brain masks generated with the brain extraction tool in FSL (Smith, 2002). A VBM style analysis (Ashburner and Friston, 2000; Good et al., 2001) was used to compare local gray matter volume estimates between chimpanzees and bonobos. This technique enables comparisons to be made across the entire brain, at every voxel (i.e. smallest volume element) of an image. Importantly, the technique controls for differences in brain size between the two comparison groups, thereby identifying differences in local gray matter volume estimates relative to whole brain size. T1 images were analyzed with FSL-VBM (Smith et al., 2004). The first step was to create a combined chimpanzee and bonobo gray matter template. So as not to bias the template toward one species, an equal number of chimpanzees and bonobos were included. Therefore, we began by brain extracting images from three of the four chimpanzees and from all three bonobos using BET (Smith, 2002). Next, tissue-type segmentation was carried out using FAST4 (Zhang et al., 2001). The resulting gray matter partial volume images were then aligned to geometric mid-space using the affine registration tool FLIRT (Jenkinson and Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002). The weighted average of all aligned images then served as the target to which all original images were non-linearly aligned using FNIRT (Andersson et al., 2007a,b), which uses a b-spline representation of the registration warp field (Rueckert et al., 1999). The resulting weighted average constituted the template for the following analysis.

All of the native gray matter images (n = 3 bonobos and n = 4 chimpanzees) were then non-linearly registered to this template. The registered partial volume images were then modulated (to allow the measurement of local expansion or contraction) by dividing by the Jacobian of the warp field. The modulated, segmented images were then smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a sigma of 2 mm. Finally, local gray matter volume estimates were compared between chimpanzees and bonobos using a voxel-wise two sample t-test. The resulting t-statistic image was thresholded at t = 4.0 which is equivalent to a two-tailed P-value of 0.01 with five degrees of freedom. Multiple comparison correction was not implemented due to limited statistical power associated with the small sample size and the accompanying risk of type 2 errors. However, as discussed above, by focusing on regions that show differences both in gray matter volume and, independently, in related white matter anatomy, we are able to further limit the chances of type I errors.

Post-mortem scans

Diffusion-weighted images were analyzed using the diffusion toolbox in the FSL software package (Smith et al., 2004). Each diffusion-weighted volume was affine-registered to a B0 volume to correct for eddy-current effects before averaging the two acquisitions, and DTIFIT was used to calculate the diffusion tensor at each voxel, from which a fractional anisotropy (FA) image was calculated. A bias-free FA template was created using affine followed by nonlinear registration as described above. Individual FA images (n = 3 bonobos and n = 3 chimpanzees) were then non-linearly registered to the template (Andersson et al., 2007a,b). Two methods were then used to compare FA between species. To compare the peak anisotropy along white matter tracts, we used tract-based spatial statistics [TBSS, (Smith et al., 2006)]. To compare the volume of local white matter pathways, we compared the Jacobians of the non-linear warp fields derived from the FA images. These Jacobians reflect the extent to which each template voxel must be either expanded or compressed to match the individual image. Both Jacobian values and FA values were compared between chimpanzees and bonobos using a two sample t-test. In parallel with the VBM analysis, the resulting t-statistic image was thresholded at t = 4.0. Like the VBM analysis described above, comparisons are made across the entire brain, at each brain voxel.

Tractography.

Tractography methods can reconstruct white matter fiber tracts based on magnitude and direction of anisotropic water diffusion in each voxel of a diffusion-weighted image. The probabilistic tractography algorithm in FSL, with adaptation to a multi-fibre model, was used for pathway reconstruction (Behrens et al., 2007). The Jacobian comparison of FA images described above yielded a difference in the bilateral ventral posterior frontal lobe. This region of interest (ROI) was warped from template space into individual subject space and then used as a seed mask for tractography. The ROI extended into gray matter. Therefore, an exclusion mask was placed in the gray matter ventral to the ROI extending back to the amygdala in order to restrict the pathway to white matter. For each subject, the total number of streamlines sent from their mask (i.e. 5000 times the number of voxels in the mask) was calculated. The average of these six values was multiplied by 0.001 and this threshold was applied to the pathway identified in all six subjects. The resulting pathway volumes were compared between chimpanzees and bonobos using a two sample t-test. To visualize group results, pathways were also thresholded more liberally at 0.0005 times the total sample number and warped back into template space. Individual pathways were then binarized and summed to create an overlap image that was then thresholded to show only those voxels where at least three of the six subjects showed evidence of a potential connection.

Finally, affine registration was used to warp tractography results into T1 space so that pathway terminations could be compared with regions where local gray matter volume estimates differed between species.

RESULTS

Overall brain size

Brain volumes obtained from the T1 scans indicated that the average chimpanzee brain volume (398 cc, n = 4) was slightly but significantly larger than the average bonobo brain volume (358 cc, n = 3; P < 0.05) (Table 1). These volume estimates are somewhat larger than other published values (Stephan et al., 1981; Herndon et al., 1999; Rilling and Insel, 1999), in part because they include some meninges and space between sulci at the brain periphery.

VBM results

VBM results show that after controlling for differences in overall brain size, bonobos had significantly higher local gray matter volume estimates than chimpanzees in limbic and paralimbic areas, and in portions of frontal and parietal cortex. On the other hand, chimpanzees had higher local gray matter volume estimates than bonobos in portions of parietal and occipital cortex (Table 2). More specifically, bonobos had higher local gray matter volume estimates in several areas of particular relevance to social cognition, and these were among the largest differences overall. These areas included the right anterior insula, right dorsal amygdala, right hypothalamus and right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (Figure 2 and Supplementary Figure S1).

Differences in local gray matter volume estimates between chimpanzees and bonobos based on analysis of T1-weighted images. The t-statistic image is thresholded at t > 4.0 and overlaid on the common species gray matter template. The figure shows three coronal sections through the gray matter template at different anterior–posterior levels. Regions in blue have higher local gray matter volume estimates in bonobos; regions in yellow have higher local gray matter volume estimates in chimpanzees. The Y-coordinate is relative to Y = 0 at the anterior commissure.
Fig. 2

Differences in local gray matter volume estimates between chimpanzees and bonobos based on analysis of T1-weighted images. The t-statistic image is thresholded at t > 4.0 and overlaid on the common species gray matter template. The figure shows three coronal sections through the gray matter template at different anterior–posterior levels. Regions in blue have higher local gray matter volume estimates in bonobos; regions in yellow have higher local gray matter volume estimates in chimpanzees. The Y-coordinate is relative to Y = 0 at the anterior commissure.

Table 2

Brain regions showing species differences in local gray matter volume estimates at a threshold of t >4.0 (P < 0.01 uncorrected for multiple comparisons)

Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo > chimpanzee
    L dlpfc (area 46)86
    R dmpfc64
    R dorsal premotor cortex49
    R ventral premotor cortex (45)45
    R anterior insula943
    Hypothalamus and adjacent basal forebrain215
    R dorsal amygdala174
    R insula20
    L temporal pole28
    L mid insula/claustrum19
    R amygdalo-hippocampal region49
    L post-central sulcus/anterior intraparietal sulcus?129
    L anterior STG181
    L entorhinal cortex25
    R intraparietal sulcus346
    R pontine nuclei117
    R inferior posterior parietal cortex25
    L inferotemporal cortex (IT)30
    Bilateral cerebellum59
    L posterior cingulate sulcus88
    Anterior parieto-occiptal sulcus90
    R calcarine sulcus33
    Left visual cortex148
    Left dorsal visual cortex117
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R frontoorbital sulcus (44/45)10
    R Dorsal insula10
    L frontal operculum47
    R anterior STG57
    R dorsal STS45
    R intraparietal sulcus/post-central sulcus23
    L tectum18
    Retrosplenial cortex23
    L temporo-parietal junction (TPJ)54
    R calcarine sulcus92
    L posterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS)159
    R extrastriate visual cortex581
    R fusiform gyrus99
    R calcarine sulcus (V1)77
    L calcarine sulcus (V1)1410
    R dorsomedial extrastriate130
    L lunate sulcus137
    R occipital pole161
    L occipital pole91
Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo > chimpanzee
    L dlpfc (area 46)86
    R dmpfc64
    R dorsal premotor cortex49
    R ventral premotor cortex (45)45
    R anterior insula943
    Hypothalamus and adjacent basal forebrain215
    R dorsal amygdala174
    R insula20
    L temporal pole28
    L mid insula/claustrum19
    R amygdalo-hippocampal region49
    L post-central sulcus/anterior intraparietal sulcus?129
    L anterior STG181
    L entorhinal cortex25
    R intraparietal sulcus346
    R pontine nuclei117
    R inferior posterior parietal cortex25
    L inferotemporal cortex (IT)30
    Bilateral cerebellum59
    L posterior cingulate sulcus88
    Anterior parieto-occiptal sulcus90
    R calcarine sulcus33
    Left visual cortex148
    Left dorsal visual cortex117
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R frontoorbital sulcus (44/45)10
    R Dorsal insula10
    L frontal operculum47
    R anterior STG57
    R dorsal STS45
    R intraparietal sulcus/post-central sulcus23
    L tectum18
    Retrosplenial cortex23
    L temporo-parietal junction (TPJ)54
    R calcarine sulcus92
    L posterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS)159
    R extrastriate visual cortex581
    R fusiform gyrus99
    R calcarine sulcus (V1)77
    L calcarine sulcus (V1)1410
    R dorsomedial extrastriate130
    L lunate sulcus137
    R occipital pole161
    L occipital pole91

Spatial extent of the regions is given in number of 0.8 mm isotropic voxels.

Table 2

Brain regions showing species differences in local gray matter volume estimates at a threshold of t >4.0 (P < 0.01 uncorrected for multiple comparisons)

Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo > chimpanzee
    L dlpfc (area 46)86
    R dmpfc64
    R dorsal premotor cortex49
    R ventral premotor cortex (45)45
    R anterior insula943
    Hypothalamus and adjacent basal forebrain215
    R dorsal amygdala174
    R insula20
    L temporal pole28
    L mid insula/claustrum19
    R amygdalo-hippocampal region49
    L post-central sulcus/anterior intraparietal sulcus?129
    L anterior STG181
    L entorhinal cortex25
    R intraparietal sulcus346
    R pontine nuclei117
    R inferior posterior parietal cortex25
    L inferotemporal cortex (IT)30
    Bilateral cerebellum59
    L posterior cingulate sulcus88
    Anterior parieto-occiptal sulcus90
    R calcarine sulcus33
    Left visual cortex148
    Left dorsal visual cortex117
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R frontoorbital sulcus (44/45)10
    R Dorsal insula10
    L frontal operculum47
    R anterior STG57
    R dorsal STS45
    R intraparietal sulcus/post-central sulcus23
    L tectum18
    Retrosplenial cortex23
    L temporo-parietal junction (TPJ)54
    R calcarine sulcus92
    L posterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS)159
    R extrastriate visual cortex581
    R fusiform gyrus99
    R calcarine sulcus (V1)77
    L calcarine sulcus (V1)1410
    R dorsomedial extrastriate130
    L lunate sulcus137
    R occipital pole161
    L occipital pole91
Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo > chimpanzee
    L dlpfc (area 46)86
    R dmpfc64
    R dorsal premotor cortex49
    R ventral premotor cortex (45)45
    R anterior insula943
    Hypothalamus and adjacent basal forebrain215
    R dorsal amygdala174
    R insula20
    L temporal pole28
    L mid insula/claustrum19
    R amygdalo-hippocampal region49
    L post-central sulcus/anterior intraparietal sulcus?129
    L anterior STG181
    L entorhinal cortex25
    R intraparietal sulcus346
    R pontine nuclei117
    R inferior posterior parietal cortex25
    L inferotemporal cortex (IT)30
    Bilateral cerebellum59
    L posterior cingulate sulcus88
    Anterior parieto-occiptal sulcus90
    R calcarine sulcus33
    Left visual cortex148
    Left dorsal visual cortex117
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R frontoorbital sulcus (44/45)10
    R Dorsal insula10
    L frontal operculum47
    R anterior STG57
    R dorsal STS45
    R intraparietal sulcus/post-central sulcus23
    L tectum18
    Retrosplenial cortex23
    L temporo-parietal junction (TPJ)54
    R calcarine sulcus92
    L posterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS)159
    R extrastriate visual cortex581
    R fusiform gyrus99
    R calcarine sulcus (V1)77
    L calcarine sulcus (V1)1410
    R dorsomedial extrastriate130
    L lunate sulcus137
    R occipital pole161
    L occipital pole91

Spatial extent of the regions is given in number of 0.8 mm isotropic voxels.

FA analysis

FA images were analyzed using two complementary techniques—TBSS analysis of FA, and FA-based warp field comparisons. It is important to note that these two techniques measure different aspects of white matter tracts. TBSS was developed specifically to measure changes in local diffusion anisotropy in a way that was robust to changes in local geometries such as tract thicknesses. By comparison, Jacobian comparisons explicitly measure the local volumetric change in tract geometry.

FA TBSS analysis

TBSS identified several regions where chimpanzees had higher FA values than bonobos (Table 3, Supplementary Figure S2). These were concentrated along visual system pathways, including the right optic tract, splenium of the corpus callosum and left parieto-occipital white matter. Chimpanzees also had higher FA in the left cerebellar white matter. There were no regions where bonobos had significantly higher FA than chimpanzees.

Table 3

Brain regions showing species differences in white matter fractional anisotropy (FA) as determined by tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) at a threshold of t > 4.0 with at least 10 contiguous voxels

Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo >chimpanzee
    None
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R genu10
    R inferior frontal white matter17
    R genu10
    R optic tract14
    R cerebral peduncle10
    Splenium of corpus callosum13
    L lateral splenium10
    L inferior parietal white matter14
    L parietal/occipital white matter37
    R parietal/occipital white matter13
    L cerebellar white matter50
Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo >chimpanzee
    None
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R genu10
    R inferior frontal white matter17
    R genu10
    R optic tract14
    R cerebral peduncle10
    Splenium of corpus callosum13
    L lateral splenium10
    L inferior parietal white matter14
    L parietal/occipital white matter37
    R parietal/occipital white matter13
    L cerebellar white matter50

Spatial extent of the regions is given in number of 1.0 mm isotropic voxels.

Table 3

Brain regions showing species differences in white matter fractional anisotropy (FA) as determined by tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) at a threshold of t > 4.0 with at least 10 contiguous voxels

Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo >chimpanzee
    None
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R genu10
    R inferior frontal white matter17
    R genu10
    R optic tract14
    R cerebral peduncle10
    Splenium of corpus callosum13
    L lateral splenium10
    L inferior parietal white matter14
    L parietal/occipital white matter37
    R parietal/occipital white matter13
    L cerebellar white matter50
Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo >chimpanzee
    None
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R genu10
    R inferior frontal white matter17
    R genu10
    R optic tract14
    R cerebral peduncle10
    Splenium of corpus callosum13
    L lateral splenium10
    L inferior parietal white matter14
    L parietal/occipital white matter37
    R parietal/occipital white matter13
    L cerebellar white matter50

Spatial extent of the regions is given in number of 1.0 mm isotropic voxels.

FA Jacobian comparison

Comparison of the warpfields that registered each subject's FA image to the FA template revealed several regions of relative expansion in bonobos compared with chimpanzees (Table 4, Supplementary Figure S3). It is important to note that the contrast that drives the Jacobian estimation comes from FA, which highlights white matter. Changes in the local Jacobian therefore reflect changes in local white matter volumes, even if they appear at the boundary between gray and white matter on the average template. Of particular relevance to social cognitive differences between the two species is expansion in bilateral ventral frontal lobe in bonobos (Figure 3), as well as in the right anterior insula. On the other hand, compared with bonobos, chimpanzees showed pronounced relative enlargement of the white matter in the vicinity of the medial thalamus (Figure 3). Chimpanzees also showed expansion relative to bonobos in primary motor cortex, the ventral temporal lobe, portions of the parietal and occipital lobes and in cerebellar white matter (Supplementary Figure S3).

Table 4

Brain regions in which the Jacobian warp field of the FA images differed between species at a threshold of t > 4.0 with at least 10 contiguous voxels

Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo > chimp
    L inferior frontal gyrus118
    Genu of corpus callosum10
    L head of caudate32
    R lateral inferior frontal cortex81
    R ventral posterior frontal white matter828
    L medial orbital gyrus and white matter505
    R mid-insula236
    R anterior STS186
    R superior frontal gyrus21
    L ventral anterior thalamus231
    Bilateral mid cingulate sulcus975
    R dorsal posterior insula1441
    R cerebral peduncle532
    L cerebral peduncle1037
    L putamen35
    R posterior intraparietal sulcus94
    R inferior lateral extrastriate cortex2935
    L anterior bank of lunate (V2)158
    R visual cortex55
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R ventral rim of cingulate sulcus30
    R motor cortex (precentral gyrus)797
    MD and adjacent midline thalamic nuclei2657
    R medial temporal white matter29
    R ventrolateral thalamus69
    R perirhinal cortex455
    R hippocampus51
    L fusiform gyrus555
    R intraparietal sulcus22
    R posterior STS237
    R optic radiation301
    L optic radiation84
    L optic radiation124
    L optic radiation239
    L posterior bank of parieto-occipital sulcus  (extrastriate visual cortex)162
    R superior cerebellar peduncle34
    R lateral cerebellum1165
    R lateral cerebellum50
    L lateral cerebellum843
    L lateral cerebellum84
    L dorsal extrastriate cortex31
    L dorsal extrastriate cortex162
    R dorsal extrastriate cortex18
    R ventral occipital cortex128
    L ventral occipital cortex52
Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo > chimp
    L inferior frontal gyrus118
    Genu of corpus callosum10
    L head of caudate32
    R lateral inferior frontal cortex81
    R ventral posterior frontal white matter828
    L medial orbital gyrus and white matter505
    R mid-insula236
    R anterior STS186
    R superior frontal gyrus21
    L ventral anterior thalamus231
    Bilateral mid cingulate sulcus975
    R dorsal posterior insula1441
    R cerebral peduncle532
    L cerebral peduncle1037
    L putamen35
    R posterior intraparietal sulcus94
    R inferior lateral extrastriate cortex2935
    L anterior bank of lunate (V2)158
    R visual cortex55
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R ventral rim of cingulate sulcus30
    R motor cortex (precentral gyrus)797
    MD and adjacent midline thalamic nuclei2657
    R medial temporal white matter29
    R ventrolateral thalamus69
    R perirhinal cortex455
    R hippocampus51
    L fusiform gyrus555
    R intraparietal sulcus22
    R posterior STS237
    R optic radiation301
    L optic radiation84
    L optic radiation124
    L optic radiation239
    L posterior bank of parieto-occipital sulcus  (extrastriate visual cortex)162
    R superior cerebellar peduncle34
    R lateral cerebellum1165
    R lateral cerebellum50
    L lateral cerebellum843
    L lateral cerebellum84
    L dorsal extrastriate cortex31
    L dorsal extrastriate cortex162
    R dorsal extrastriate cortex18
    R ventral occipital cortex128
    L ventral occipital cortex52

Spatial extent of the regions is given in number of 0.8 mm isotropic voxels.

Table 4

Brain regions in which the Jacobian warp field of the FA images differed between species at a threshold of t > 4.0 with at least 10 contiguous voxels

Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo > chimp
    L inferior frontal gyrus118
    Genu of corpus callosum10
    L head of caudate32
    R lateral inferior frontal cortex81
    R ventral posterior frontal white matter828
    L medial orbital gyrus and white matter505
    R mid-insula236
    R anterior STS186
    R superior frontal gyrus21
    L ventral anterior thalamus231
    Bilateral mid cingulate sulcus975
    R dorsal posterior insula1441
    R cerebral peduncle532
    L cerebral peduncle1037
    L putamen35
    R posterior intraparietal sulcus94
    R inferior lateral extrastriate cortex2935
    L anterior bank of lunate (V2)158
    R visual cortex55
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R ventral rim of cingulate sulcus30
    R motor cortex (precentral gyrus)797
    MD and adjacent midline thalamic nuclei2657
    R medial temporal white matter29
    R ventrolateral thalamus69
    R perirhinal cortex455
    R hippocampus51
    L fusiform gyrus555
    R intraparietal sulcus22
    R posterior STS237
    R optic radiation301
    L optic radiation84
    L optic radiation124
    L optic radiation239
    L posterior bank of parieto-occipital sulcus  (extrastriate visual cortex)162
    R superior cerebellar peduncle34
    R lateral cerebellum1165
    R lateral cerebellum50
    L lateral cerebellum843
    L lateral cerebellum84
    L dorsal extrastriate cortex31
    L dorsal extrastriate cortex162
    R dorsal extrastriate cortex18
    R ventral occipital cortex128
    L ventral occipital cortex52
Brain regionNo. of voxels
Bonobo > chimp
    L inferior frontal gyrus118
    Genu of corpus callosum10
    L head of caudate32
    R lateral inferior frontal cortex81
    R ventral posterior frontal white matter828
    L medial orbital gyrus and white matter505
    R mid-insula236
    R anterior STS186
    R superior frontal gyrus21
    L ventral anterior thalamus231
    Bilateral mid cingulate sulcus975
    R dorsal posterior insula1441
    R cerebral peduncle532
    L cerebral peduncle1037
    L putamen35
    R posterior intraparietal sulcus94
    R inferior lateral extrastriate cortex2935
    L anterior bank of lunate (V2)158
    R visual cortex55
Chimpanzee > bonobo
    R ventral rim of cingulate sulcus30
    R motor cortex (precentral gyrus)797
    MD and adjacent midline thalamic nuclei2657
    R medial temporal white matter29
    R ventrolateral thalamus69
    R perirhinal cortex455
    R hippocampus51
    L fusiform gyrus555
    R intraparietal sulcus22
    R posterior STS237
    R optic radiation301
    L optic radiation84
    L optic radiation124
    L optic radiation239
    L posterior bank of parieto-occipital sulcus  (extrastriate visual cortex)162
    R superior cerebellar peduncle34
    R lateral cerebellum1165
    R lateral cerebellum50
    L lateral cerebellum843
    L lateral cerebellum84
    L dorsal extrastriate cortex31
    L dorsal extrastriate cortex162
    R dorsal extrastriate cortex18
    R ventral occipital cortex128
    L ventral occipital cortex52

Spatial extent of the regions is given in number of 0.8 mm isotropic voxels.

Differences in Jacobian warp fields between chimpanzees and bonobos based on analysis of diffusion-weighted images. The t-statistic image is thresholded at t > 4.0 and overlaid on the common species FA template. The figure shows four coronal sections through the FA template at different anterior–posterior levels. Regions in blue are larger in bonobos (larger Jacobians from template to subject space) and regions in yellow are larger in chimpanzees. The Y-coordinate is relative to Y = 0 at the anterior commissure.
Fig. 3

Differences in Jacobian warp fields between chimpanzees and bonobos based on analysis of diffusion-weighted images. The t-statistic image is thresholded at t > 4.0 and overlaid on the common species FA template. The figure shows four coronal sections through the FA template at different anterior–posterior levels. Regions in blue are larger in bonobos (larger Jacobians from template to subject space) and regions in yellow are larger in chimpanzees. The Y-coordinate is relative to Y = 0 at the anterior commissure.

Tractography results

To determine whether the species difference in ventral posterior frontal lobe volume identified in the FA comparison was related to the VBM results, we used tractography to delineate the pathway passing through this ROI. The pathway was tracked in native diffusion space for each of the three individual chimpanzee brains and each of the three individual bonobo brains and then warped back into FA template space to visualize the group average trajectory. The resulting tract connected the ventral anterior cingulate cortex with the amygdala bilaterally (Figure 4 and Supplementary Figure S4). Pathway volumes calculated in native space revealed that all three bonobos had a larger pathway than all three chimpanzees in both the left [t(4) = 5.06, P < 0.01) and right [t(4) = 2.82, P < 0.05] hemisphere (Table 5). Warping the average pathway into T1 space revealed that it made contact with the region of expanded amygdala volume in bonobos (Supplementary Figure S5).

Group tractography result from posterior ventral frontal ROI derived from the jacobian comparison shown in Figure 3. Tracts from all three bonobos and all three chimpanzees were registered to template space, binarized and summed. The resulting map shows voxels where at least three of six subjects had a possible connection.
Fig. 4

Group tractography result from posterior ventral frontal ROI derived from the jacobian comparison shown in Figure 3. Tracts from all three bonobos and all three chimpanzees were registered to template space, binarized and summed. The resulting map shows voxels where at least three of six subjects had a possible connection.

Table 5

Amygdala to ventral ACC pathway volumes in bonobos and chimpanzees

SpeciesLeft volume (mm3)Right volume (mm3)
Bonobo (Mambo)1676.311135.27
Bonobo (Psuke)2010.431577.59
Bonobo (Tamuli)2025.621980.17
Chimpanzee (Artifee)1104.921080.23
Chimpanzee (Jenda)749.87364.50
Chimpanzee (Polyanna)429.08358.82
SpeciesLeft volume (mm3)Right volume (mm3)
Bonobo (Mambo)1676.311135.27
Bonobo (Psuke)2010.431577.59
Bonobo (Tamuli)2025.621980.17
Chimpanzee (Artifee)1104.921080.23
Chimpanzee (Jenda)749.87364.50
Chimpanzee (Polyanna)429.08358.82
Table 5

Amygdala to ventral ACC pathway volumes in bonobos and chimpanzees

SpeciesLeft volume (mm3)Right volume (mm3)
Bonobo (Mambo)1676.311135.27
Bonobo (Psuke)2010.431577.59
Bonobo (Tamuli)2025.621980.17
Chimpanzee (Artifee)1104.921080.23
Chimpanzee (Jenda)749.87364.50
Chimpanzee (Polyanna)429.08358.82
SpeciesLeft volume (mm3)Right volume (mm3)
Bonobo (Mambo)1676.311135.27
Bonobo (Psuke)2010.431577.59
Bonobo (Tamuli)2025.621980.17
Chimpanzee (Artifee)1104.921080.23
Chimpanzee (Jenda)749.87364.50
Chimpanzee (Polyanna)429.08358.82

DISCUSSION

VBM analyses of T1 scans revealed that bonobos have more gray matter than chimpanzees in the hypothalamus, the right dorsal amygdala and the right anterior insula. DTI analyses additionally showed that the pathway connecting the amygdala with the ventral anterior cingulate cortex is larger in bonobos than chimpanzees. These species differences in brain anatomy can be plausibly linked with known species differences in temperament and behavior.

Anxiety and stress reactivity

Bonobos exhibit a stronger stress hormone response to feeding competition (Wobber et al., 2010b), and have been described as more nervous than chimpanzees (de Waal, 1997). The larger right amygdala, hypothalamus and right anterior insula of bonobos are a potential neural substrate for this difference in stress and anxiety. The amygdala difference is concentrated in its dorsal aspect, in the vicinity of the central nucleus (Amunts et al., 2005; Barger et al., 2007), a region strongly implicated in fear and anxiety that is known to activate the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and autonomic nervous system by way of connections with the hypothalamus (Davis, 1997; Ledoux, 1998). Interestingly, this nucleus is larger in human patients with generalized anxiety disorder compared with healthy controls (Etkin et al., 2009). Human subjects who are prone to anxiety have increased activity in the anterior insula during emotion processing (Paulus and Stein, 2006). The anterior insula also tracks risk (Bossaerts, 2010) and predicts risk-averse choices (Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005), and bonobos are more risk-averse than chimpanzees (Heilbronner et al., 2008).

Sexual behavior

Compared with chimpanzees, bonobos have higher rates of sexual interactions in captivity (de Waal, 1995) but not in the wild (Stanford, 1998; Hashimoto, 2002). In both contexts, bonobos exhibit a greater diversity of sexual behaviors, including all possible age–sex combinations (de Waal, 1987; Parish and de Waal, 2000). The hypothalamus and amygdala are key nodes of a network that controls sexual behavior in non-human animals (Newman, 1999; Breedlove et al., 2007). Functional neuroimaging experiments have similarly implicated the hypothalamus and amygdala in the human response to visual sexual stimuli (Karama et al., 2002; Hamann et al., 2004). Given that bonobos use sex to reduce tension (de Waal, 1987; Parish, 1996; Hare et al., 2007; Hohmann et al., 2008), the enlarged bonobo amygdala could be eliciting fear and anxiety, while simultaneously motivating sexual behavior that helps dissipate it.

Play

Bonobos are thought to have retained certain juvenile characteristics into adulthood (Shea, 1983; Wrangham and Pilbeam, 2001; Lieberman et al., 2007) (i.e. paedomorphism), including extraordinary playfulness (de Waal, 1997). In addition to sex, bonobos use play to diffuse social tension (Enomoto, 1990) and rates of play are higher among adult bonobos than adult chimpanzees (Palagi, 2006). After controlling for overall brain size and body size, both amygdala and hypothalamus size are correlated with frequency of social play across primate species (Lewis and Barton, 2006), so the larger amygdala and hypothalamus of bonobos could also relate to this difference in play frequency.

Empathy

Anecdotal observations have led to the suggestion that bonobos may be more empathic than chimpanzees (de Waal, 1997). Psychopathy, a disorder marked by lack of empathy, is associated with reduced size and function of the amygdala (Gordon et al., 2004; Yang et al., 2009), as well as reduced functional connectivity between the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) (Marsh et al., 2008). These results and others have led to the hypothesis that the pathway from the amygdala to the VMPFC is involved in perceiving distress in others and in learning to avoid behaviors that provoke such distress (Blair, 2008). Our results show that bonobos have more gray matter in the dorsal amygdala and a larger pathway linking the amygdala with VMPFC compared with chimpanzees. There is evidence that testosterone impairs the functioning of this pathway in humans (van Wingen et al., 2010), and chimpanzee males have both higher baseline testosterone metabolite levels (Sannen et al., 2004), as well as a more pronounced increase in testosterone in response to feeding competition (Wobber et al., 2010b) compared with bonobo males. Thus, species differences in pathway size may be augmented by differences in testosterone-mediated functional suppression. There is also suggestive evidence, in the form of the ratio of the second to fourth finger length (2D : 4D), that chimpanzees may have higher prenatal androgen levels than bonobos (or humans) (McIntyre et al., 2009). Prenatal testosterone levels are inversely correlated with measures of empathy in human children (Chapman et al., 2006). Thus, organizational effects of prenatal androgens could contribute to species differences in the size of the pathway linking the amygdala and VMPFC, as well as other empathy-related structures.

The anterior insula is also strongly implicated in empathy, presumably through a mechanism that involves simulation of visceral somatic states (Singer et al., 2009), and disorders characterized by empathic deficits are associated with reduced size and function of the anterior insula (Bird et al., 2010) (Veit et al., 2002; Birbaumer et al., 2005; Sterzer et al., 2007; Seeley, 2010). Bonobos have more gray matter in the anterior insula than chimpanzees, specifically within the ventral region that is consistently associated with social–emotional processing (Allman et al., 2010; Kurth et al., 2010) and that reliably coactivates with the amygdala (Mutschler et al., 2009). Thus, the neurobiological characteristics that differentiate chimpanzees from bonobos bear a striking similarity to those that differentiate patients with empathy deficits from normal controls.

Social tolerance

Bonobos exhibit highly tolerant food sharing in the wild (Kuroda, 1984; White, 1994; Fruth and Hohmann, 2002) [but see (Jaeggi et al., 2010)], and bonobos showed greater tolerance of co-feeding than chimpanzees in direct experimental comparisons (Hare et al., 2007; Hare and Kwetuenda, 2010; Wobber et al., 2010a). Bonobos may therefore be better at inhibiting selfish behaviors than chimpanzees are (Hare et al., 2007). This may be supported by their larger pathway linking ventral ACC with the amygdala, a pathway that has been implicated in emotion regulation in humans (Davidson et al., 2000; Pezawas et al., 2005). This same pathway might also be involved in controlling aggressive impulses, another behavior that differs between these two species.

Aggression

Chimpanzees exhibit more severe aggression than bonobos. Within social groups, chimpanzee aggression involves male aggression against females, male infanticide and intense male competition for dominance status. In contrast, male aggression against females is rare in bonobos (perhaps due to strong bonds among females), male infanticide has not yet been observed and reconciliation following conflict is more common and more often initiated by the aggressor than among chimpanzees (Goodall, 1986; de Waal, 1987; Parish, 1996; Wrangham and Peterson, 1996; Parish and de Waal, 2000). Chimpanzees are strongly xenophobic. Between group interactions are typically hostile and can involve lethal aggression (Wrangham, 1999). In bonobos, intergroup encounters can be hostile, but in contrast to chimpanzees, also affiliative (Idani, 1990; Kano, 1992). Wrangham and Pilbeam (2001) suggests that bonobos evolved due to selection against aggression and that they share a number of traits with domesticated versions of wild animals that also show reduced aggression. For example, both bonobos and dogs are less aggressive, as well as more juvenilized, than chimpanzees and wolves, respectively.

Evidence suggests that the pathway linking the ventral ACC and the amygdala normally functions to restrain aggression via both top–down suppression of aggressive impulses from the amygdala (Davidson et al., 2000; Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2006), and through a bottom–up relay of perceived distress in others to VMPFC that inhibits anti-social behavior (Blair, 2008). Thus, underdevelopment of this pathway, as found in chimpanzees, might be expected to augment both impulsive aggression due to lack of top–down control, as well as instrumental aggression due to lack of bottom–up biases against harming others. As mentioned above, this effect may be compounded by higher testosterone levels in male chimpanzees that inhibit functionality in this pathway, especially since testosterone is positively correlated with aggression in male chimpanzees (Muller and Wrangham, 2004).

Synthesis

Our results can be usefully interpreted within a recently proposed model of the neurobiology of human empathy and callousness (Blair, 2008; Shirtcliff et al., 2009). Callous individuals are characterized by hypoarousal of the stress system in response to their own distress. Because we normally empathize with others by simulating their distress (Singer and Lamm, 2009), callous individuals are also deficient in representing the distress of others. For example, criminal psychopaths are autonomically insensitive to the suffering and distress of others (Blair et al., 1997). These psychological deficits are paralleled by deficits in the structure and function of empathy-related areas including the amygdala and anterior insula, as well as the connections linking the amygdala to VMPFC. Further, deficits in empathy may contribute to increased aggression insofar as harming others will be less aversive to those with a reduced capacity to experience distress. In parallel, we suggest that the enlarged dorsal amygdala, anterior insula and amygdala to vACC pathway in bonobos allows them to more strongly represent distress of both self and others compared with chimpanzees. This translates into greater empathy, as well as reduced aggression. However, excessive distress and anxiety can interfere with helping behavior (Batson, 1998). In bonobos, this may be prevented in part by a stronger regulatory pathway from ventral ACC to amygdala and in part by increased rates of play, sexual behavior and reconciliation that serve to limit anxiety and distress to a level that supports prosocial behavior.

Other species differences

The above discussion is focused on species differences in the neural system involved in understanding others’ emotions. However, humans have an additional neural system for understanding others that involves making cognitive inferences about others’ thoughts. Both the medial prefrontal cortex and the temporo-parietal junction have been consistently implicated in this function (Gallagher and Frith, 2003; Saxe and Kanwisher, 2003). Thus, it is interesting that bonobos also have higher local gray matter volume estimates in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex compared with chimpanzees. Although the question of whether chimpanzees possess a theory of mind is still debated (Byrne and Whiten, 1992; Tomasello et al., 2003; Penn and Povinelli, 2007), our results suggest that further investigation of bonobo mentalizing capacities may be of interest. Indeed, a recent experimental study concluded that bonobos were more skilled than chimpanzees at tasks related to theory of mind (Herrmann et al., 2010).

Our FA analysis revealed a large area of the medial thalamus that is larger in chimpanzees than bonobos. This difference is concentrated in the region of the medial dorsal thalamic nucleus and could reflect differences in the white matters axons that penetrate the medial dorsal nucleus, or alternatively differences in the adjacent white matter pathway—the fornix. Chimpanzees also showed expansion relative to bonobos in several visual cortical regions, including portions of both the ventral and dorsal visual streams, and chimpanzees showed higher FA values along visual system white matter pathways, which might imply greater myelination or fiber density in these pathways (Pierpaoli and Basser, 1996; Beaulieu, 2002; Le Bihan, 2003). Although the functional significance of these differences is uncertain, we note that chimpanzees outperform bonobos on tasks requiring the use of tools (Herrmann et al., 2010) and also more often use tools in the wild (Schaik et al., 1999; Hohmann and Fruth, 2003). Neural specializations may have evolved to support this ability, and human tool use relies heavily on the dorsal and ventral visual streams (Frey, 2007). Finally, in both the FA and T1 analyses, chimpanzees showed evidence of expansion in the right superior temporal sulcus relative to bonobos. This region is involved in the perception of biological motion and also in understanding intentions behind actions (Perret et al., 1985; Allison et al., 2000), raising the possibility that chimpanzees may excel at this aspect of social cognition relative to bonobos.

Limitations

Access to bonobo brains is limited due to their endangered status, and our sample size is accordingly limited. We therefore cannot exclude the possibility that some of our group differences are type 1 errors due to chance sampling. This concern is mitigated to some extent by the overlapping results from the independent FA and T1 data sets, particularly with respect to consistent differences in the right anterior insula and amygdala in favor of bonobos, and consistent differences in visual cortical areas in favor of chimpanzees. Moreover, some of our results converge with earlier results, such as the finding that chimpanzees have a larger cerebellum than bonobos (Hopkins et al., 2009) and bonobos have a larger lateral amygdala nucleus after controlling for brain size (Barger et al., 2007). Our group differences could also be explained by a confounding variable. For example, rearing history can impact brain development and subsequent anatomy (Sanchez et al., 1998). Although we do not have information on the rearing history of all of the animals included in this study, we do know that two of our bonobo subjects were mother-reared and two of our chimpanzee subjects were nursery-reared by humans (Schenker et al., 2005). Although we cannot exclude this as a potential explanation for our results, the nursery-reared chimpanzees were not raised in an impoverished social environment, but instead had frequent human contact along with the opportunity for daily play with other chimpanzees. Thus, we favor heritable species differences as the most likely factor driving these neurobiological differences. Finally, given evidence for developmental delay and paedomorphosis in bonobos, it would be informative to compare the two species at earlier developmental stages to determine how these differences in adult brain anatomy arise in ontogeny.

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

The authors thank Brain Hare, Scott Lilienfeld, Jennifer Mascaro, Frans de Waal and Patricia Whitten, as well as our two anonymous reviewers, for their many helpful comments on this article. The authors also thank William Hopkins for supplying two post-mortem bonobo brains for scanning.

This work was supported by the Emory University Research Committee, the Center for Behavioral Neuroscience, the Yerkes Base Grant (NIH RR-00165), and by a visiting research fellowship to JKR from Jesus College at Oxford University.

REFERENCES

Allison
T
Puce
A
McCarthy
G
,
Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
2000
, vol.
4
(pg.
267
-
78
)
Allman
JM
Tetreault
NA
Hakeem
AY
et al.
,
The von Economo neurons in frontoinsular and anterior cingulate cortex in great apes and humans
Brain Structure and Function
,
2010
, vol.
214
(pg.
495
-
517
)
Amunts
K
Kedo
O
Kindler
M
et al.
,
Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps
Anatomical Embryology
,
2005
, vol.
210
(pg.
343
-
52
)
Andersson
JLR
Jenkinson
M
Smith
S
,
Non-linear optimisation
,
2007
 
Andersson
JLR
Jenkinson
M
Smith
S
,
Non-linear optimisation, aka Spatial normalisation
,
2007
 
Ashburner
J
Friston
KJ
,
Voxel-based morphometry - the methods
Neuroimage
,
2000
, vol.
11
(pg.
805
-
821
)
Barger
N
Stefanacci
L
Semendeferi
K
,
A comparative volumetric analysis of the amygdaloid complex and basolateral division in the human and ape brain
American Journal of Physical Anthropology
,
2007
, vol.
134
(pg.
392
-
403
)
Batson
CD
Gilbert
DT
Fiske
ST
Lindzey
G
,
Altruism and prosocial behavior
The Handbook of Social Psychology
,
1998
Boston
McGraw-Hill
(pg.
282
-
316
)
Beaulieu
C
,
The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system - a technical review
NMR Biomedical
,
2002
, vol.
15
(pg.
435
-
55
)
Becquet
C
Patterson
N
Stone
AC
Przeworski
M
Reich
D
,
Genetic structure of chimpanzee populations
PLoS Genetics
,
2007
, vol.
3
pg.
e66
Behrens
TE
Berg
HJ
Jbabdi
S
Rushworth
MF
Woolrich
MW
,
Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: what can we gain?
Neuroimage
,
2007
, vol.
34
(pg.
144
-
55
)
Birbaumer
N
Veit
R
Lotze
M
et al.
,
Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study
Archives in General Psychiatry
,
2005
, vol.
62
(pg.
799
-
805
)
Bird
G
Silani
G
Brindley
R
White
S
Frith
U
Singer
T
,
Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism
Brain
,
2010
, vol.
133
(pg.
1515
-
25
)
Blair
RJ
,
The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex: functional contributions and dysfunction in psychopathy
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences
,
2008
, vol.
363
(pg.
2557
-
65
)
Blair
RJ
Jones
L
Clark
F
Smith
M
,
The psychopathic individual: a lack of responsiveness to distress cues?
Psychophysiology
,
1997
, vol.
34
(pg.
192
-
8
)
Bossaerts
P
,
Risk and risk prediction error signals in anterior insula
Brain Structure and Function
,
2010
, vol.
214
(pg.
645
-
53
)
Breedlove
SM
Rosenzweig
MR
Watson
NV
,
Sex: evolutionary, hormonal, and neural bases
In: Biological Psychology: An Introduction to Behavioral, Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience
,
2007
5th edn
Sunderland, MA
Sinaur Associates
(pg.
354
-
84
)
Byrne
RW
Whiten
A
,
Cognitive evolution in primates: evidence from tactical deception
Man
,
1992
, vol.
27
(pg.
609
-
27
)
Chapman
E
Baron-Cohen
S
Auyeung
B
Knickmeyer
R
Taylor
K
Hackett
G
,
Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the "reading the mind in the eyes" test
Social Neurosciences
,
2006
, vol.
1
(pg.
135
-
48
)
Davidson
RJ
Putnam
KM
Larson
CL
,
Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation - A possible prelude to violence
Science
,
2000
, vol.
289
(pg.
591
-
4
)
Davis
M
,
Neurobiology of fear responses: the role of the amygdala
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
,
1997
, vol.
9
(pg.
382
-
402
)
de Waal
FB
,
Tension regulation and nonreproductive functions of sex in captive bonobos Pan paniscus
National Geogrpahic Research
,
1987
, vol.
3
(pg.
318
-
35
)
de Waal
FB
Abramson
P
Pinkerton
S
,
Sex as an alternative to aggression in the bonobo
Sexual Nature, Sexual Culture
,
1995
Chicago
University of Chicago Press
(pg.
37
-
56
)
de Waal
FB
Bonobo: the Forgotten Ape
,
1997
Berkeley
University of California Press
Enomoto
T
,
Social play and sexual behavior of the bonobo (Pan paniscus) with special reference to flexibility
Primates
,
1990
, vol.
31
(pg.
469
-
80
)
Etkin
A
Prater
KE
Schatzberg
AF
Menon
V
Greicius
MD
,
Disrupted amygdalar subregion functional connectivity and evidence of a compensatory network in generalized anxiety disorder
Archives in Genenal Psychiatry
,
2009
, vol.
66
(pg.
1361
-
72
)
Frey
SH
,
What puts the how in where? Tool use and the divided visual streams hypothesis
Cortex
,
2007
, vol.
43
(pg.
368
-
75
)
Fruth
B
Hohmann
G
Boesch
C
Hohmann
G
Marchant
LF
,
How bonobos handle hunts and harvests: why share food?
Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos
,
2002
New York
Cambridge University Press
(pg.
231
-
243
)
Gallagher
HL
Frith
CD
,
Functional imaging of “theory of mind”
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
2003
, vol.
7
(pg.
77
-
83
)
Good
CD
Johnsrude
I
Ashburner
J
Henson
RN
Friston
KJ
Frackowiak
RS
,
Cerebral asymmetry and the effects of sex and handedness on brain structure: a voxel-based morphometric analysis of 465 normal adult human brains
Neuroimage
,
2001
, vol.
14
(pg.
685
-
700
)
Goodall
J
The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior
,
1986
Cambridge, MA
Harvard University Press
Goodman
M
Tagle
DA
Fitch
DH
et al.
,
Primate evolution at the DNA level and a classification of hominoids
Journal of Molecular Evolution
,
1990
, vol.
30
(pg.
260
-
6
)
Gordon
HL
Baird
AA
End
A
,
Functional differences among those high and low on a trait measure of psychopathy
Biological Psychiatry
,
2004
, vol.
56
(pg.
516
-
21
)
Hamann
S
Herman
RA
Nolan
CL
Wallen
K
,
Men and women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli
Nature Neuroscience
,
2004
, vol.
7
(pg.
411
-
6
)
Hare
B
Kwetuenda
S
,
Bonobos voluntarily share their own food with others
Current Biology
,
2010
, vol.
20
(pg.
R230
-
1
)
Hare
B
Melis
AP
Woods
V
Hastings
S
Wrangham
R
,
Tolerance allows bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task
Current Biology
,
2007
, vol.
17
(pg.
619
-
23
)
Hashimoto
TFTaC
Boesch
GH
Marchant
L
,
Why female bonobos have a lower copulation rate during estrus than chimpanzees
Behavioral Diversity of Chimpanzees and Bonobos
,
2002
Cambridge
Cambridge University Press
(pg.
156
-
67
)
Heilbronner
SR
Rosati
AG
Stevens
JR
Hare
B
Hauser
MD
,
A fruit in the hand or two in the bush? Divergent risk preferences in chimpanzees and bonobos
Biological Letter
,
2008
, vol.
4
(pg.
246
-
9
)
Herndon
JG
Tigges
J
Anderson
DC
Klumpp
SA
McClure
HM
,
Brain weight throughout the life span of the chimpanzee
Journal of Computer Neurology
,
1999
, vol.
409
(pg.
567
-
72
)
Herrmann
E
Hare
B
Call
J
Tomasello
M
,
Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees
PLoS ONE
,
2010
, vol.
5
pg.
e12438
Hey
J
,
The divergence of chimpanzee species and subspecies as revealed in multipopulation isolation-with-migration analyses
Molecular Biology Evolution
,
2010
, vol.
27
(pg.
921
-
33
)
Hohmann
G
Fruth
B
,
Culture in bonobos? Between-species and within-secies variation in behavior
Current Anthropology
,
2003
, vol.
44
(pg.
563
-
71
)
Hohmann
G
Mundry
R
Deschner
T
,
The relationship between socio-sexual behavior and salivary cortisol in bonobos: Tests of the tension regulation hypothesis
American Journal of Primatology
,
2008
, vol.
71
(pg.
223
-
32
)
Hopkins
WD
Lyn
H
Cantalupo
C
,
Volumetric and lateralized differences in selected brain regions of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus)
Americal Journal of Primatology
,
2009
, vol.
71
(pg.
988
-
97
)
Idani
G
,
Relations between unit-groups of bonobos at Wamba: encounters and temporary fusions
African Study Monographs
,
1990
, vol.
11
(pg.
153
-
86
)
Jaeggi
AV
Stevens
JM
Van Schaik
CP
,
Tolerant food sharing and reciprocity is precluded by despotism among bonobos but not chimpanzees
American Journal of Physical Anthropology
,
2010
, vol.
143
(pg.
41
-
51
)
Jenkinson
M
Smith
S
,
A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images
Medical Image Analysis
,
2001
, vol.
5
(pg.
143
-
56
)
Jenkinson
M
Bannister
P
Brady
M
Smith
S
,
Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images
Neuroimage
,
2002
, vol.
17
(pg.
825
-
41
)
Kano
T
The Last Ape: Pygmy Chimpanzee Behavior and Ecology
,
1992
Stanford, CA
Stanford University Press
Karama
S
Lecours
AR
Leroux
JM
et al.
,
Areas of brain activation in males and females during viewing of erotic film excerpts
Human Brain Mapping
,
2002
, vol.
16
(pg.
1
-
13
)
Kuhnen
CM
Knutson
B
,
The neural basis of financial risk taking
Neuron
,
2005
, vol.
47
(pg.
763
-
70
)
Kuroda
S
Susman
RL
,
Interaction over food among pygmy chimpanzees
The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior
,
1984
New York
Plenum Press
(pg.
301
-
24
)
Kurth
F
Zilles
K
Fox
PT
Laird
AR
Eickhoff
SB
,
A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis
Brain Structure and Function
,
2010
, vol.
214
(pg.
519
-
34
)
Le Bihan
D
,
Looking into the functional architecture of the brain with diffusion MRI
Nature Review of Neuroscience
,
2003
, vol.
4
(pg.
469
-
80
)
Ledoux
J
,
Fear and the brain: where have we been, and where are we going?
Biological Psychiatry
,
1998
, vol.
44
(pg.
1229
-
38
)
Lewis
KP
Barton
RA
,
Amygdala size and hypothalamus size predict social play frequency in nonhuman primates: a comparative analysis using independent contrasts
Journal of Computer Psychology
,
2006
, vol.
120
(pg.
31
-
7
)
Lieberman
DE
Carlo
J
de Leon
MP
Zollikofer
CPE
,
A geometric morphometric analysis of heterochrony in the cranium of chimpanzees and bonobos
Journal of Human Evolution
,
2007
, vol.
52
(pg.
647
-
62
)
Marsh
AA
Finger
EC
Mitchell
DG
et al.
,
Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
2008
, vol.
165
(pg.
712
-
20
)
McIntyre
MH
Herrmann
E
Wobber
V
et al.
,
Bonobos have a more human-like second-to-fourth finger length ratio (2D:4D) than chimpanzees: a hypothesized indication of lower prenatal androgens
Journal of Human Evolution
,
2009
, vol.
56
(pg.
361
-
5
)
Meyer-Lindenberg
A
Buckholtz
JW
Kolachana
B
et al.
,
Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
2006
, vol.
103
(pg.
6269
-
74
)
Muller
MN
Wrangham
RW
,
Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the “challenge hypothesis”
Animal Behavior
,
2004
, vol.
67
(pg.
113
-
23
)
Mutschler
I
Wieckhorst
B
Kowalevski
S
et al.
,
Functional organization of the human anterior insular cortex
Neuroscience Letter
,
2009
, vol.
457
(pg.
66
-
70
)
Newman
SW
,
The medial extended amygdala in male reproductive behavior. A node in the mammalian social behavior network
Annual New York Academy of Sciences
,
1999
, vol.
877
(pg.
242
-
57
)
Palagi
E
,
Social play in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): implications for natural social systems and interindividual relationships
American Journal of Physical Anthropology
,
2006
, vol.
129
(pg.
418
-
26
)
Parish
AR
,
Female relationships in bonobos (Pan paniscus)
Human Nature
,
1996
, vol.
7
(pg.
61
-
96
)
Parish
AR
de Waal
FB
,
The other “closest living relative”. How bonobos (Pan paniscus) challenge traditional assumptions about females, dominance, intra- and intersexual interactions, and hominid evolution
Annual New York Academy of Sciences
,
2000
, vol.
907
(pg.
97
-
113
)
Paulus
MP
Stein
MB
,
An insular view of anxiety
Biology of Psychiatry
,
2006
, vol.
60
(pg.
383
-
7
)
Penn
DC
Povinelli
DJ
,
On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind'
Philosophical Transactions of Royal Society of London, Series B Biological Sciences
,
2007
, vol.
362
(pg.
731
-
44
)
Perret
DI
Smith
PA
Potter
DD
et al.
,
Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction
Proceedings of Royal Soceity of London, Series B Biological Science
,
1985
, vol.
223
(pg.
293
-
317
)
Pezawas
L
Meyer-Lindenberg
A
Drabant
EM
et al.
,
5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression
Nature of Neuroscience
,
2005
, vol.
8
(pg.
828
-
34
)
Pierpaoli
C
Basser
PJ
,
Toward a quantitative assessment of diffusion anisotropy
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine
,
1996
, vol.
36
(pg.
893
-
906
)
Rilling
JK
Insel
TR
,
The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging
Journal of Human Evolution
,
1999
, vol.
37
(pg.
191
-
223
)
Rilling
JK
Seligman
RA
,
A quantitative morphometric comparative analysis of the primate temporal lobe
Journal of Human Evolution
,
2002
, vol.
42
(pg.
505
-
33
)
Rueckert
D
Sonoda
LI
Hayes
C
Hill
DL
Leach
MO
Hawkes
DJ
,
Nonrigid registration using free-form deformations: application to breast MR images
IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging
,
1999
, vol.
18
(pg.
712
-
21
)
Sanchez
MM
Hearn
EF
Do
D
Rilling
JK
Herndon
JG
,
Differential rearing affects corpus callosum size and cognitive function of rhesus monkeys
Brain Research
,
1998
, vol.
812
(pg.
38
-
49
)
Sannen
A
Van Elsacker
L
Heistermann
M
Eens
M
,
Urinary testosterone-metabolite levels and dominance rank in male and female bonobos (Pan paniscus)
Primates
,
2004
, vol.
45
(pg.
89
-
96
)
Saxe
R
Kanwisher
N
,
People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”
Neuroimage
,
2003
, vol.
19
(pg.
1835
-
42
)
Schaik
CPv
Deaner
RO
Merrill
MY
,
The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture
Journal of Human Evolution
,
1999
, vol.
36
(pg.
719
-
41
)
Schenker
NM
Desgouttes
AM
Semendeferi
K
,
Neural connectivity and cortical substrates of cognition in hominoids
Journal of Human Evolution
,
2005
, vol.
49
(pg.
547
-
69
)
Seeley
WW
,
Anterior insula degeneration in frontotemporal dementia
Brain Structure and Function
,
2010
, vol.
214
(pg.
465
-
75
)
Shea
BT
,
Paedomorphosis and neoteny in the pygmy chimpanzee
Science
,
1983
, vol.
222
(pg.
521
-
2
)
Shirtcliff
EA
Vitacco
MJ
Graf
AR
Gostisha
AJ
Merz
JL
Zahn-Waxler
C
,
Neurobiology of empathy and callousness: implications for the development of antisocial behavior
Behavior Science Law
,
2009
, vol.
27
(pg.
137
-
71
)
Singer
T
Critchley
HD
Preuschoff
K
,
A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
2009
Singer
T
Lamm
C
,
The social neuroscience of empathy
Annual New York Academy Sciences
,
2009
, vol.
1156
(pg.
81
-
96
)
Smith
SM
,
Fast robust automated brain extraction
Human Brain Mapping
,
2002
, vol.
17
(pg.
143
-
55
)
Smith
SM
Jenkinson
M
Johansen-Berg
H
et al.
,
Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data
Neuroimage
,
2006
, vol.
31
(pg.
1487
-
505
)
Smith
SM
Jenkinson
M
Woolrich
MW
et al.
,
Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL
Neuroimage
,
2004
, vol.
23
Suppl 1
(pg.
S208
-
19
)
Stanford
CB
,
The social behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos
Current Anthropology
,
1998
, vol.
39
(pg.
399
-
420
)
Stephan
H
Frahm
H
Baron
G
,
New and revised data on volumes of brain structures in insectivores and primates
Folia Primatologica
,
1981
, vol.
35
(pg.
1
-
29
)
Sterzer
P
Stadler
C
Poustka
F
Kleinschmidt
A
,
A structural neural deficit in adolescents with conduct disorder and its association with lack of empathy
Neuroimage
,
2007
, vol.
37
(pg.
335
-
42
)
Tomasello
M
Call
J
Hare
B
,
Chimpanzees understand psychological states – the question is which ones and to what extent
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
,
2003
, vol.
7
(pg.
153
-
6
)
van Wingen
G
Mattern
C
Verkes
RJ
Buitelaar
J
Fernandez
G
,
Testosterone reduces amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex coupling
Psychoneuroendocrinology
,
2010
, vol.
35
(pg.
105
-
13
)
Veit
R
Flor
H
Erb
M
et al.
,
Brain circuits involved in emotional learning in antisocial behavior and social phobia in humans
Neuroscience Letters
,
2002
, vol.
328
(pg.
233
-
6
)
White
F
Roeder
JJ
Thierry
B
Anderson
JR
Herrenschmidt
N
,
Food sharing in wild pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus)
Current Primatology: Social Development, Learning and Behavior
,
1994
, vol.
Vol. II
Strasbourg
Universite Louis Pasteur
(pg.
1
-
10
)
Wobber
V
Hare
B
Maboto
J
Lipson
S
Wrangham
R
Ellison
PT
,
Differential changes in steroid hormones before competition in bonobos and chimpanzees
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
2010
, vol.
107
(pg.
12457
-
62
)
Wobber
V
Wrangham
R
Hare
B
,
Bonobos exhibit delayed development of social behavior and cognition relative to chimpanzees
Current Biology
,
2010
, vol.
20
(pg.
226
-
30
)
Wrangham
R
Pilbeam
D
Galdikas
BMF
Briggs
NE
Sheeran
LK
Shapiro
GL
Goodall
J
,
African apes as time machines
All Apes Great and Small
,
2001
New York
Plenum
(pg.
5
-
17
)
Wrangham
RW
,
The evolution of coalitionary killing
Yearbook of Physical Anthropology
,
1999
, vol.
42
(pg.
1
-
30
)
Wrangham
RW
Peterson
D
Demonic Males: Apes and the Origins of Human Violence
,
1996
Boston
Houghton Mifflin
Yang
Y
Raine
A
Narr
KL
Colletti
P
Toga
AW
,
Localization of deformations within the amygdala in individuals with psychopathy
Archives in General Psychiatry
,
2009
, vol.
66
(pg.
986
-
94
)
Zhang
Y
Brady
M
Smith
S
,
Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm
IEEE Transactions of Medical Imaging
,
2001
, vol.
20
(pg.
45
-
57
)
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]

Supplementary data