Abstract

Study Objectives

To examine cross-sectionally and prospectively whether informal caregiving is related to sleep disturbance among caregivers in paid work.

Methods

Participants (N = 21604) in paid work from the Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health. Sleeping problems were measured with a validated scale of sleep disturbance (Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire). Random-effects modeling was used to examine the cross-sectional association between informal caregiving (self-reports: none, up to 5 hours per week, over 5 hours per week) and sleep disturbance. Potential sociodemographic and health confounders were controlled for and interactions between caregiving and gender included. Longitudinal random-effects modeling of the effects of changes in reported informal caregiving upon sleep disturbance and change in sleep disturbance was performed.

Results

In multivariate analyses controlling for sociodemographics, health factors, and work hours, informal caregiving was associated cross-sectionally with sleep disturbance in a dose–response relationship (compared with no caregiving, up to 5 hours of caregiving: β = 0.03; 95% CI: 0.01, 0.06, and over 5 hours: β = 0.08; 95% CI: 0.02, 0.13), results which varied by gender. Cessation of caregiving was associated with reductions in sleep disturbance (β = −0.08; 95% CI: −0.13, −0.04).

Conclusions

This study provides evidence for a causal association of provision of informal care upon self-reported sleep disturbance. Even low-intensity care provision was related to sleep disturbance among this sample of carers in paid work. The results highlight the importance of addressing sleep disturbance in caregivers.

Statement of Significance

Sleep disturbances are a common health problem. One likely cause of sleep disturbance is provision of informal care to an elderly, ill, or disabled person. In this prospective analysis of 21 604 participants in paid work, caregiving was associated with self-reported sleep disturbance, particularly at higher numbers of hours of informal care. The effects of providing informal care at different intensities varied between women and men. In analyses of change in caregiving, providing caregiving at the first but not the second wave was associated with reduction in sleep disturbance. These findings support the hypothesis that provision of informal care affects the carer’s sleep quality. Further large-scale studies to examine the mechanisms of sleep disturbance among in-work carers are required.

INTRODUCTION

Sleeping problems are common: in Swedish adults aged 18–84, insomnia disorder, that is, insomnia symptoms and daytime consequences, has an estimated prevalence of around 10.5% and symptoms of insomnia are reported by one-quarter of the population.1 Evidence is growing that people with insomnia symptoms have a raised risk of developing physical illness,2 specifically cardiometabolic disease, occupational injuries, and all-cause mortality.3,4 In terms of mental illness, disturbed sleep and tiredness are common symptoms of mood disorders such as depression.5 Consequently, addressing the causes of insomnia may contribute to improvements in population health.6

One likely important cause of sleep disturbance is provision of informal care to an elderly, ill, or disabled person. Informal care is assistance provided by people from the intimate environment of the dependent person, who do not receive any training or economic compensation. It is common: in the United States, 16.6% of adults7 and in Sweden, 10.9% of adults8 are currently informal carers, rates that are expected to increase in line with population aging and cut-backs in publicly funded care.9 Informal caregiving has been associated, albeit inconsistently, with a range of poor health outcomes among carers in large cross-sectional and prospective studies,10–13 depending on the characteristics of the carer and person cared for, type of health outcome, type of care provided, and duration of follow-up.14,15 However, some studies find that caregiving may be neutral or benefit carers’ health, e.g., by providing an additional rewarding role (role enhancement) and improving the quality of family relationships.16–20 Among other factors, it is probable that the intensity and duration of care provision determine whether and to what degree caring has a positive or negative impact on the caregiver.

Although it has been argued that various aspects of the caregiving role impact on sleep specifically,21 recent reviews of the evidence note that few adequately powered, community-based, longitudinal studies have examined the impacts of caregiving on sleep.22–24 Existing studies point to poorer sleep among caregivers, but apart from certain exceptions, tend to be cross-sectional, use small samples which make effects hard to detect, or only examine specific groups of carers without comparing them to the general population. Important confounders such as the caregiver’s education level or health may not be controlled for, factors which lead to both selection into the caregiving role and sleep disturbance.17 Stronger evidence would also be provided by prospective designs linking changing provision of care to changes in sleep, but almost no research has so far done so.25,26

Although recent reviews have argued for greater focus on subpopulations of carers in terms of factors such as age,27 or care recipient characteristics,28 to the best of our knowledge, prior research has not examined the impact of caregiving on sleep in the important and large population of caregivers who are simultaneously engaged in paid work. This is despite evidence that difficulties combining work tasks with family obligations can generate stress and health problems.29,30 Specifically, the demands of paid work may conflict with requirements to provide informal care, which may lead to role overload, in which an individual with a finite amount of time and energy experiences strain from trying to fulfil multiple roles.16

Consequently, this study focuses on caregivers and noncaregivers who are in paid work, and uses a large community-based prospective dataset to examine both the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of caregiving with sleep disturbance. Important health and demographic confounders of the caregiving-sleep relationship are controlled for. Firstly, we examine whether greater intensity of caring, operationalized using weekly hours of care provided, is associated with sleep disturbance in cross-sectional analyses. We hypothesize that providing informal care will be associated with sleep disturbance, although the benefits of “role enhancement” may mean that carers providing low-intensity caregiving experience lower levels of sleep disturbance than noncaregivers. Since women tend to provide more informal care than men,7 and reports of insomnia symptoms are more frequent for women than for men,1 gender interactions are included in these analyses in case the relationship between caregiving and sleep disturbance differs by gender. Secondly, in longitudinal analyses, we examine the effects of changes in caregiving upon sleep disturbance and upon change in sleep disturbance. We hypothesize that commencing informal caregiving will be associated with greater sleep disturbance and cessation of caregiving with reduced sleep disturbance. To the best of our knowledge, only one study has previously examined the relationship between starting and stopping provision of informal care on sleeping problems, in a small sample of spousal Alzheimer caregivers.26 In short, the longitudinal analyses performed in this study may offer additional evidence for a causal relationship between informal caregiving and sleep disturbance in a large sample of in-work carers.

METHODS

Study Population

Participants in the Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health (SLOSH) formed the study population. SLOSH is a biennial postal survey, which follows a subsample of gainfully employed people aged 16–64 from the Swedish Labour Force Survey who were recruited into the Swedish Work Environment Surveys (SWES) 2003–2011.31 Both SLOSH and the present study have been approved by the Regional Research Ethics Board in Stockholm. All participants provided informed consent.

SLOSH participants respond to one of the two versions of the postal questionnaire: an in-work questionnaire for those currently in paid work at least 30% of full-time and another for those working less or who are outside the labor force whether permanently or temporarily. The present study is based on information provided in the in-work questionnaires only from 2010, 2012, 2014, and 2016. Response rates to the SLOSH questionnaires were 57% in 2010 and 2012, 53% in 2014, and 51% in 2016. In total, 21604 participants responding to at least one in-work questionnaire were included in the study, providing 42928 observations for the analysis. The change analyses required participants to respond to the in-work questionnaire in at least two consecutive waves, a requirement which reduced the sample to 12253 people.

Sleep Disturbance

Self-reported sleep disturbance was measured at each wave using a validated measure of disturbed sleep composed of four questions from the Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire.32–34 The measure contains the core symptoms of insomnia; specifically participants were asked how often they had been disturbed in the previous 3 months by difficulties falling asleep, repeated awakenings with difficulties going back to sleep, premature (final) awakening, or disturbed or restless sleep, with response options ranging from 0 (“never”) to 5 (“always/five times a week”). Item nonresponse rates were low: 1.1% for one missing item, 0.2% for each of two and three missing items, and 1.1% for four missing items. An average sleep disturbance score was calculated for participants who provided responses to at least three of the four items. Change in sleep disturbance from one wave to the next was calculated by subtracting the score in the earlier wave from the score in the next wave.

Provision of Informal Care

At each study wave, questions about participants’ time use in a typical work week were used to identify how much care in hours participants provided for a relative other than a child or grandchild. Original response categories were 0 hour (85.8% of sample members), 1–5 hours (12.4%), 6–10 hours (1.2%), 11–15 hours (0.3%), and >15 hours (0.4%). The last three categories were collapsed into >5 hours per week since relatively few respondents provided high numbers of hours of informal care. A dichotomized variable of caregiving was used in analyses examining change between consecutive waves: no care-giving and provision of any care (at least 1 hour per week). This generated the combinations: no caregiving at either wave; no caregiving at the first wave, caregiving at the second wave; caregiving at the first wave but not at the second; and caregiving at both waves.

Covariates

Selection of covariates was performed by drawing directed acyclic graphs based on existing knowledge of factors which might affect both sleep disturbance and possibilities to provide informal care.35 The sociodemographic variables of gender, age, marital status, and education were included in the analyses as possible confounders of the relationship between caring and sleep disturbance. Information regarding gender, age, education, and marital status (0 = married/cohabiting and 1 = nonmarried/divorced/widowed/single) was obtained through linkage using individual person numbers to Statistics Sweden’s Longitudinal Individual Data Base (LISA) administrative register. Age was centered at its mean of 50 years. In order to model nonlinear relationships between age and sleep disturbance, squared and cubic functions of age were generated. Highest education level was grouped into the following categories: compulsory schooling; 2 years upper secondary/vocational training or 4 years upper secondary; university or equivalent shorter than 3 years; and at least 3 years of university or equivalent.

Two variables recorded participants’ perceptions of whether their lives were affected by (1) any of a range of chronic conditions and (2) physical pain or discomfort. These variables were included as confounders because such health problems might prevent carers from providing help to relatives and are also likely to increase sleep disturbance.36–38 Specifically, participants indicated whether they had any of the following chronic conditions (hypertension, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, rheumatic disorder, musculoskeletal disorder, mental illness, asthma, obstructive pulmonary disease, migraine, physical disability, cancer, and other illness) and in a separate question whether they had experienced pain or discomfort over the previous 3 months (specifically headache, pain in the neck or shoulders, pain in the lower back, or other pain). For both variables, participants could respond: no; yes, but this does not affect their life at all; yes, this affects their life a little; or yes, this affects their life a lot. The two variables were dichotomized such that reports that chronic disease or pain/discomfort affected the participant’s life either a little or a lot were coded as 1, otherwise they were coded as 0.

Self-rated health and reports of depressive symptoms were included in the analyses as both potential confounders and mediators of the relationship between informal caregiving and sleep disturbance. To measure self-rated health, respondents were asked: “How would you rate your general state of health” with the responses: very good, good, neither good nor bad, quite poor, or very poor, a single-item measure of general physical and mental health which predicts mortality.39,40 A brief six-item version of the Symptom Checklist Core Depression Scale (SCL-CD6) was used to measure depressive symptomatology.41–43 Questions in this scale evaluate how much (from 0 = not at all to 4 = very much) participants were troubled by lethargy or low energy, feeling blue, blaming oneself, excessive worrying, feelings of no interest in things, and feeling that everything is an effort. An overall score (0–24) was obtained by summing responses to all items.

Self-rated health and depressive symptoms were considered to be potential confounders of the informal caring–sleep relationship because poor self-rated health and depressive symptoms might prevent carers from providing help to relatives and also be likely to increase sleep disturbance. However, self-rated health and depressive symptoms might also mediate the relationship from informal caregiving to sleep disturbance; i.e., they may be on the causal path. Previous research has highlighted the importance of physical fatigue and tiredness to how people evaluate their self-rated health.40,44 Furthermore, fatigue and sleep disturbance are common symptoms of depressive episodes in the ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioral disorders.

Paid work intensity was measured in four categories indicating time spent weekly in paid work created from participants’ reports (<10 hours, 10–19 hours, 20–29 hours, and ≥30 hours). This covariate could be both a confounder or mediator of the informal caring–sleep relationship: a confounder because time in paid work could affect both the number of hours dedicated to informal caregiving and sleep disturbance, and a mediator because hours spent providing informal care could lead to a decrease in hours of paid work, which in turn may affect sleep disturbance if, for example, this generated financial difficulties.45

Statistical Methods

Statistical analysis was carried out in Stata 14.2 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, USA). We performed random intercept (random effect) modeling, which allows the use of full information provided by longitudinal data, while accounting for the dependency of repeated measures within individuals.46 These models use both the between- and within-subject components of the variability that are present in longitudinal data.

Two main analyses were performed, which both present results from the random intercept models as regression coefficients with 95% confidence intervals. First, cross-sectional associations between intensity of informal caregiving (no caregiving, up to 5 hours, and over 5 hours) and sleep disturbance were examined. The reference group contained participants who provided no informal care. These bivariate associations are presented as Model 1. In Model 2, variables were included that were considered to be potential confounders (sociodemographics, physical pain, and chronic disease) because they might affect both sleep disturbance and possibilities to provide informal care. In Model 3, additional variables (self-rated health, depressive symptoms, and hours in paid work) were included which might be both confounders and mediators of the informal caring–sleep relationship. In other words, in addition to being likely confounders, these factors may also lie on the causal pathway from informal caregiving to sleep disturbance. Consequently, adjusting on them may remove part or all of any effect of informal caregiving on sleep disturbance (overadjustment). Potential interactions between gender and care-giving intensity were also examined (Model 4).

Second, we examined changes in informal caregiving (whether providing any care or not) between two consecutive waves in relation to sleep disturbance at the second of the two waves, as well as in relation to change in sleep disturbance between the waves. All of the changes between the four available waves were simultaneously modeled in random intercept models. Covariates were taken from the second of the two waves. The reference group contained participants providing no informal care in either of the two waves.

RESULTS

Descriptive and bivariate statistics of the total study sample (N = 21604) are presented in Table 1 using information from the baseline survey year for each individual. Although most of the sample (18531 participants) did not report providing informal care at baseline, 2675 participants (12.4%) reported providing informal care up to 5 hours per week, and 398 participants (1.8%) reported higher number of hours of care. Caregivers were more likely to be female, be older, have a lower education level, report that their life was affected by physical pain or discomfort or by chronic illness, report poorer self-rated health, depressive symptoms, and sleep disturbance, and be in paid work under 20 hours per week. In sensitivity analyses (not shown), caregivers had poorer physical and mental health as well as greater sleep disturbance even after controlling for the average older age of caregivers.

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Associations Between Caregiving Status and Other Variables at Baseline (Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health, N = 21604).

Percent or mean (SD)
VariablesFull sampleNoncarersCaregiving ≤ 5 h per weekCaregiving > 5 h per week
All100.0%85.8%12.4%1.8%
Gender (p < .001)
 Men44.5%45.5%39.7%29.9%
 Women55.6%54.6%60.3%70.1%
Age (p < .001)49.5 (10.4)48.9 (10.6)52.8 (8.5)54.2 (8.5)
Education (p = .003)
 Compulsory11.0%10.9%10.7%15.1%
 High school46.1%45.8%48.2%45.0%
 University < 3 y14.7%14.7%14.8%17.1%
 University ≥ 3y28.3%28.7%26.3%22.9%
Marital status (p < .001)
 Cohabiting/married55.9%55.1%61.3%56.0%
 Other44.1%44.9%38.7%44.0%
Life affected by pain or discomfort (p < .001)
 No41.4%42.1%36.6%40.0%
 Yes58.6%57.9%63.4%60.1%
Life affected by chronic illness (p < .001)
 No56.4%57.6%49.6%47.5%
 Yes43.6%42.4%50.4%52.5%
Self-rated health (p < .001)
 Very good26.6%27.4%21.6%22.4%
 Good53.6%53.5%55.0%51.5%
 Neither good nor bad14.2%13.9%16.0%18.3%
 Poor5.3%5.0%7.1%7.3%
 Very poor.3%.4%.3%.5%
Depressive symptoms (p < .001)5.4 (5.1)5.2 (5.0)6.1 (5.4)6.5 (5.7)
Time spent in paid work weekly (p < .001)
 <10 h9.4%9.2%10.4%12.3%
 10–19 h16.0%15.7%17.6%19.1%
 20–29 h15.3%14.9%18.1%14.8%
 ≥30 h59.3%60.2%54.0%53.8%
Sleep disturbance score (p < .001)1.61 (1.05)1.58 (1.04)1.79 (1.10)1.89 (1.21)
Percent or mean (SD)
VariablesFull sampleNoncarersCaregiving ≤ 5 h per weekCaregiving > 5 h per week
All100.0%85.8%12.4%1.8%
Gender (p < .001)
 Men44.5%45.5%39.7%29.9%
 Women55.6%54.6%60.3%70.1%
Age (p < .001)49.5 (10.4)48.9 (10.6)52.8 (8.5)54.2 (8.5)
Education (p = .003)
 Compulsory11.0%10.9%10.7%15.1%
 High school46.1%45.8%48.2%45.0%
 University < 3 y14.7%14.7%14.8%17.1%
 University ≥ 3y28.3%28.7%26.3%22.9%
Marital status (p < .001)
 Cohabiting/married55.9%55.1%61.3%56.0%
 Other44.1%44.9%38.7%44.0%
Life affected by pain or discomfort (p < .001)
 No41.4%42.1%36.6%40.0%
 Yes58.6%57.9%63.4%60.1%
Life affected by chronic illness (p < .001)
 No56.4%57.6%49.6%47.5%
 Yes43.6%42.4%50.4%52.5%
Self-rated health (p < .001)
 Very good26.6%27.4%21.6%22.4%
 Good53.6%53.5%55.0%51.5%
 Neither good nor bad14.2%13.9%16.0%18.3%
 Poor5.3%5.0%7.1%7.3%
 Very poor.3%.4%.3%.5%
Depressive symptoms (p < .001)5.4 (5.1)5.2 (5.0)6.1 (5.4)6.5 (5.7)
Time spent in paid work weekly (p < .001)
 <10 h9.4%9.2%10.4%12.3%
 10–19 h16.0%15.7%17.6%19.1%
 20–29 h15.3%14.9%18.1%14.8%
 ≥30 h59.3%60.2%54.0%53.8%
Sleep disturbance score (p < .001)1.61 (1.05)1.58 (1.04)1.79 (1.10)1.89 (1.21)

SD = standard deviation.

p-Values for the relationship between caregiving status and other variables were obtained using a chi-square test for categorical variables, and one-way ANOVAs for testing differences among means across caregiving status groups.

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Associations Between Caregiving Status and Other Variables at Baseline (Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health, N = 21604).

Percent or mean (SD)
VariablesFull sampleNoncarersCaregiving ≤ 5 h per weekCaregiving > 5 h per week
All100.0%85.8%12.4%1.8%
Gender (p < .001)
 Men44.5%45.5%39.7%29.9%
 Women55.6%54.6%60.3%70.1%
Age (p < .001)49.5 (10.4)48.9 (10.6)52.8 (8.5)54.2 (8.5)
Education (p = .003)
 Compulsory11.0%10.9%10.7%15.1%
 High school46.1%45.8%48.2%45.0%
 University < 3 y14.7%14.7%14.8%17.1%
 University ≥ 3y28.3%28.7%26.3%22.9%
Marital status (p < .001)
 Cohabiting/married55.9%55.1%61.3%56.0%
 Other44.1%44.9%38.7%44.0%
Life affected by pain or discomfort (p < .001)
 No41.4%42.1%36.6%40.0%
 Yes58.6%57.9%63.4%60.1%
Life affected by chronic illness (p < .001)
 No56.4%57.6%49.6%47.5%
 Yes43.6%42.4%50.4%52.5%
Self-rated health (p < .001)
 Very good26.6%27.4%21.6%22.4%
 Good53.6%53.5%55.0%51.5%
 Neither good nor bad14.2%13.9%16.0%18.3%
 Poor5.3%5.0%7.1%7.3%
 Very poor.3%.4%.3%.5%
Depressive symptoms (p < .001)5.4 (5.1)5.2 (5.0)6.1 (5.4)6.5 (5.7)
Time spent in paid work weekly (p < .001)
 <10 h9.4%9.2%10.4%12.3%
 10–19 h16.0%15.7%17.6%19.1%
 20–29 h15.3%14.9%18.1%14.8%
 ≥30 h59.3%60.2%54.0%53.8%
Sleep disturbance score (p < .001)1.61 (1.05)1.58 (1.04)1.79 (1.10)1.89 (1.21)
Percent or mean (SD)
VariablesFull sampleNoncarersCaregiving ≤ 5 h per weekCaregiving > 5 h per week
All100.0%85.8%12.4%1.8%
Gender (p < .001)
 Men44.5%45.5%39.7%29.9%
 Women55.6%54.6%60.3%70.1%
Age (p < .001)49.5 (10.4)48.9 (10.6)52.8 (8.5)54.2 (8.5)
Education (p = .003)
 Compulsory11.0%10.9%10.7%15.1%
 High school46.1%45.8%48.2%45.0%
 University < 3 y14.7%14.7%14.8%17.1%
 University ≥ 3y28.3%28.7%26.3%22.9%
Marital status (p < .001)
 Cohabiting/married55.9%55.1%61.3%56.0%
 Other44.1%44.9%38.7%44.0%
Life affected by pain or discomfort (p < .001)
 No41.4%42.1%36.6%40.0%
 Yes58.6%57.9%63.4%60.1%
Life affected by chronic illness (p < .001)
 No56.4%57.6%49.6%47.5%
 Yes43.6%42.4%50.4%52.5%
Self-rated health (p < .001)
 Very good26.6%27.4%21.6%22.4%
 Good53.6%53.5%55.0%51.5%
 Neither good nor bad14.2%13.9%16.0%18.3%
 Poor5.3%5.0%7.1%7.3%
 Very poor.3%.4%.3%.5%
Depressive symptoms (p < .001)5.4 (5.1)5.2 (5.0)6.1 (5.4)6.5 (5.7)
Time spent in paid work weekly (p < .001)
 <10 h9.4%9.2%10.4%12.3%
 10–19 h16.0%15.7%17.6%19.1%
 20–29 h15.3%14.9%18.1%14.8%
 ≥30 h59.3%60.2%54.0%53.8%
Sleep disturbance score (p < .001)1.61 (1.05)1.58 (1.04)1.79 (1.10)1.89 (1.21)

SD = standard deviation.

p-Values for the relationship between caregiving status and other variables were obtained using a chi-square test for categorical variables, and one-way ANOVAs for testing differences among means across caregiving status groups.

In the random effects model, presented in Table 2, informal care provision was associated with sleep disturbance before (Model 1) and after adjustment for possible socioeconomic and health confounders (Model 2). Specifically, after adjustment, compared with participants who were not providing informal care, those who were providing care up to 5 hours per week reported higher levels of sleep disturbance (β = 0.07; 95% CI: 0.04, 0.09), whereas those who were providing over 5 hours of care reported highest levels of sleep disturbance (β = 0.17; 95% CI: 0.11, 0.23). In Model 3, we adjusted for depressive symptoms, self-rated health and hours in paid work, and variables which could be both confounders and mediators of the association between caregiving and sleep. Following this adjustment, caregiving was still associated with higher levels of reported sleep disturbance (up to 5 hours of care: β = 0.03; 95% CI: 0.01, 0.06 and over 5 hours of care: β = 0.08; 95% CI: 0.02, 0.13).

Table 2

Sleep Disturbance in Relation to Level of Informal Caregiving (Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health, N = 21604, Random Effects Models).

Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4
β95% CIβ95% CIβ95% CIβ95% CI
Caregiving (ref: not caring)
 Caregiving ≤ 5 h per week0.10***(0.08, 0.13)0.07***(0.04, 0.09)0.03**(0.01, 0.06)0.01(−0.03, 0.05)
 Caregiving > 5 h per week0.21***(0.15, 0.27)0.17***(0.11, 0.23)0.08**(0.02, 0.13)0.20***(0.10, 0.31)
Gender (ref: male)
 Female0.21***(0.18, 0.23)0.16***(0.14, 0.18)0.16***(0.14, 0.18)
Gender x caregiving (ref: not caring and male)
 Care ≤ 5 hpw and female0.03(−0.01, 0.08)
 Care > 5 hpw and female−0.18**(−0.30, −0.06)
Age0.01***(0.01, 0.01)0.01***(0.01, 0.01)0.01***(0.01, 0.01)
Age-squared−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)
Age-cubed−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)
Education (ref: compulsory)
 High school−0.01(−0.05, 0.04)−0.01(−0.05; .03)−0.01(−0.05, 0.03)
 Uni < 3 y0.04(−0.01, 0.09)0.04(−0.01, 0.08)0.04(−0.01, 0.08)
 Uni ≥ 3 y0.06*(0.01, 0.10)0.04*(0.00, 0.08)0.04*(0.00, 0.08)
Marital status (ref: married)
Other0.02(−0.00, 0.04)−0.02(−0.04, 0.00)−0.02(−0.04, 0.00)
Pain (ref: none)
 Yes, life affected0.27***(0.25, 0.29)0.13***(0.11, 0.15)0.13***(0.11, 0.15)
Chronic disease (ref: none)
 Yes, life affected0.18***(0.16, 0.20)0.06***(0.04, 0.08)0.06***(0.04, 0.08)
Self-rated health (ref: very good)
 Good0.17***(0.15, 0.19)0.17***(0.15, 0.19)
 Neither good nor bad0.38***(0.35, 0.40)0.37***(0.35, 0.40)
 Poor0.53***(0.49, 0.57)0.53***(0.48, 0.57)
 Very poor0.68***(0.54, 0.81)0.68***(0.55, 0.81)
Depressive symptoms0.07***(0.07, 0.07)0.07***(0.07, 0.07)
Time spent in paid work weekly (ref: ≥30 h)
 <10 h0.02(−0.01, 0.05)0.02(−0.01, 0.05)
 10–19 h−0.01(−0.03, 0.02)−0.01(−0.03, 0.02)
 20–29 h0.01(−0.01, 0.03)0.01(−0.01, 0.03)
Constant1.61***(1.59, 1.62)1.26***(1.22, 1.31)0.91***(0.87, 0.95)0.91***(0.87, 0.95)
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4
β95% CIβ95% CIβ95% CIβ95% CI
Caregiving (ref: not caring)
 Caregiving ≤ 5 h per week0.10***(0.08, 0.13)0.07***(0.04, 0.09)0.03**(0.01, 0.06)0.01(−0.03, 0.05)
 Caregiving > 5 h per week0.21***(0.15, 0.27)0.17***(0.11, 0.23)0.08**(0.02, 0.13)0.20***(0.10, 0.31)
Gender (ref: male)
 Female0.21***(0.18, 0.23)0.16***(0.14, 0.18)0.16***(0.14, 0.18)
Gender x caregiving (ref: not caring and male)
 Care ≤ 5 hpw and female0.03(−0.01, 0.08)
 Care > 5 hpw and female−0.18**(−0.30, −0.06)
Age0.01***(0.01, 0.01)0.01***(0.01, 0.01)0.01***(0.01, 0.01)
Age-squared−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)
Age-cubed−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)
Education (ref: compulsory)
 High school−0.01(−0.05, 0.04)−0.01(−0.05; .03)−0.01(−0.05, 0.03)
 Uni < 3 y0.04(−0.01, 0.09)0.04(−0.01, 0.08)0.04(−0.01, 0.08)
 Uni ≥ 3 y0.06*(0.01, 0.10)0.04*(0.00, 0.08)0.04*(0.00, 0.08)
Marital status (ref: married)
Other0.02(−0.00, 0.04)−0.02(−0.04, 0.00)−0.02(−0.04, 0.00)
Pain (ref: none)
 Yes, life affected0.27***(0.25, 0.29)0.13***(0.11, 0.15)0.13***(0.11, 0.15)
Chronic disease (ref: none)
 Yes, life affected0.18***(0.16, 0.20)0.06***(0.04, 0.08)0.06***(0.04, 0.08)
Self-rated health (ref: very good)
 Good0.17***(0.15, 0.19)0.17***(0.15, 0.19)
 Neither good nor bad0.38***(0.35, 0.40)0.37***(0.35, 0.40)
 Poor0.53***(0.49, 0.57)0.53***(0.48, 0.57)
 Very poor0.68***(0.54, 0.81)0.68***(0.55, 0.81)
Depressive symptoms0.07***(0.07, 0.07)0.07***(0.07, 0.07)
Time spent in paid work weekly (ref: ≥30 h)
 <10 h0.02(−0.01, 0.05)0.02(−0.01, 0.05)
 10–19 h−0.01(−0.03, 0.02)−0.01(−0.03, 0.02)
 20–29 h0.01(−0.01, 0.03)0.01(−0.01, 0.03)
Constant1.61***(1.59, 1.62)1.26***(1.22, 1.31)0.91***(0.87, 0.95)0.91***(0.87, 0.95)

CI = confidence interval.

*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Table 2

Sleep Disturbance in Relation to Level of Informal Caregiving (Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health, N = 21604, Random Effects Models).

Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4
β95% CIβ95% CIβ95% CIβ95% CI
Caregiving (ref: not caring)
 Caregiving ≤ 5 h per week0.10***(0.08, 0.13)0.07***(0.04, 0.09)0.03**(0.01, 0.06)0.01(−0.03, 0.05)
 Caregiving > 5 h per week0.21***(0.15, 0.27)0.17***(0.11, 0.23)0.08**(0.02, 0.13)0.20***(0.10, 0.31)
Gender (ref: male)
 Female0.21***(0.18, 0.23)0.16***(0.14, 0.18)0.16***(0.14, 0.18)
Gender x caregiving (ref: not caring and male)
 Care ≤ 5 hpw and female0.03(−0.01, 0.08)
 Care > 5 hpw and female−0.18**(−0.30, −0.06)
Age0.01***(0.01, 0.01)0.01***(0.01, 0.01)0.01***(0.01, 0.01)
Age-squared−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)
Age-cubed−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)
Education (ref: compulsory)
 High school−0.01(−0.05, 0.04)−0.01(−0.05; .03)−0.01(−0.05, 0.03)
 Uni < 3 y0.04(−0.01, 0.09)0.04(−0.01, 0.08)0.04(−0.01, 0.08)
 Uni ≥ 3 y0.06*(0.01, 0.10)0.04*(0.00, 0.08)0.04*(0.00, 0.08)
Marital status (ref: married)
Other0.02(−0.00, 0.04)−0.02(−0.04, 0.00)−0.02(−0.04, 0.00)
Pain (ref: none)
 Yes, life affected0.27***(0.25, 0.29)0.13***(0.11, 0.15)0.13***(0.11, 0.15)
Chronic disease (ref: none)
 Yes, life affected0.18***(0.16, 0.20)0.06***(0.04, 0.08)0.06***(0.04, 0.08)
Self-rated health (ref: very good)
 Good0.17***(0.15, 0.19)0.17***(0.15, 0.19)
 Neither good nor bad0.38***(0.35, 0.40)0.37***(0.35, 0.40)
 Poor0.53***(0.49, 0.57)0.53***(0.48, 0.57)
 Very poor0.68***(0.54, 0.81)0.68***(0.55, 0.81)
Depressive symptoms0.07***(0.07, 0.07)0.07***(0.07, 0.07)
Time spent in paid work weekly (ref: ≥30 h)
 <10 h0.02(−0.01, 0.05)0.02(−0.01, 0.05)
 10–19 h−0.01(−0.03, 0.02)−0.01(−0.03, 0.02)
 20–29 h0.01(−0.01, 0.03)0.01(−0.01, 0.03)
Constant1.61***(1.59, 1.62)1.26***(1.22, 1.31)0.91***(0.87, 0.95)0.91***(0.87, 0.95)
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4
β95% CIβ95% CIβ95% CIβ95% CI
Caregiving (ref: not caring)
 Caregiving ≤ 5 h per week0.10***(0.08, 0.13)0.07***(0.04, 0.09)0.03**(0.01, 0.06)0.01(−0.03, 0.05)
 Caregiving > 5 h per week0.21***(0.15, 0.27)0.17***(0.11, 0.23)0.08**(0.02, 0.13)0.20***(0.10, 0.31)
Gender (ref: male)
 Female0.21***(0.18, 0.23)0.16***(0.14, 0.18)0.16***(0.14, 0.18)
Gender x caregiving (ref: not caring and male)
 Care ≤ 5 hpw and female0.03(−0.01, 0.08)
 Care > 5 hpw and female−0.18**(−0.30, −0.06)
Age0.01***(0.01, 0.01)0.01***(0.01, 0.01)0.01***(0.01, 0.01)
Age-squared−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)
Age-cubed−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)−0.00***(−0.00, −0.00)
Education (ref: compulsory)
 High school−0.01(−0.05, 0.04)−0.01(−0.05; .03)−0.01(−0.05, 0.03)
 Uni < 3 y0.04(−0.01, 0.09)0.04(−0.01, 0.08)0.04(−0.01, 0.08)
 Uni ≥ 3 y0.06*(0.01, 0.10)0.04*(0.00, 0.08)0.04*(0.00, 0.08)
Marital status (ref: married)
Other0.02(−0.00, 0.04)−0.02(−0.04, 0.00)−0.02(−0.04, 0.00)
Pain (ref: none)
 Yes, life affected0.27***(0.25, 0.29)0.13***(0.11, 0.15)0.13***(0.11, 0.15)
Chronic disease (ref: none)
 Yes, life affected0.18***(0.16, 0.20)0.06***(0.04, 0.08)0.06***(0.04, 0.08)
Self-rated health (ref: very good)
 Good0.17***(0.15, 0.19)0.17***(0.15, 0.19)
 Neither good nor bad0.38***(0.35, 0.40)0.37***(0.35, 0.40)
 Poor0.53***(0.49, 0.57)0.53***(0.48, 0.57)
 Very poor0.68***(0.54, 0.81)0.68***(0.55, 0.81)
Depressive symptoms0.07***(0.07, 0.07)0.07***(0.07, 0.07)
Time spent in paid work weekly (ref: ≥30 h)
 <10 h0.02(−0.01, 0.05)0.02(−0.01, 0.05)
 10–19 h−0.01(−0.03, 0.02)−0.01(−0.03, 0.02)
 20–29 h0.01(−0.01, 0.03)0.01(−0.01, 0.03)
Constant1.61***(1.59, 1.62)1.26***(1.22, 1.31)0.91***(0.87, 0.95)0.91***(0.87, 0.95)

CI = confidence interval.

*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Gender interacted significantly with caring (Table 2, Model 4 and Figure 1). Men who did not provide care and who provided up to 5 hours of care had similar levels of sleep disturbance. Both groups had significantly lower sleep disturbance than men who provided over 5 hours of care. Women who provided up to 5 hours of informal care weekly reported slightly more sleep disturbance than women who provided no care; differences between those groups and women providing more than 5 hours of care were not discernible (Figure 1).

Predictive margins of sleep disturbance in relation to hours per week spent caring and gender (Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health, N = 21604, random effects models adjusted for socioeconomic and health covariates as well as time in paid work).
Figure 1

Predictive margins of sleep disturbance in relation to hours per week spent caring and gender (Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health, N = 21604, random effects models adjusted for socioeconomic and health covariates as well as time in paid work).

In Table 3, random effects models of change in caregiving on both sleep disturbance and change in sleep disturbance are presented. The left-hand set of analyses present change in caregiving from one wave to the next on sleep disturbance at the second of the two waves. Compared with participants who did not provide informal care in either wave, those who provided care during at least one time point had greater sleep disturbance both before (Model 1) and after adjustment (Model 2) for a range of possible confounders at the current wave. Specifically, compared with participants who did not provide informal care in either wave, those who provided care at both waves had significantly greater sleep disturbance (β = 0.10; 95% CI: 0.04, 0.15), as did those who provided care only at the second of the two waves (β = 0.07; 95% CI: 0.03, 0.12), and those who provided care at the first, but not at the second wave (β = 0.05; 95% CI: 0.00, 0.10). Results from Wald tests showed that differences in sleep disturbance among these three categories in which participants provided care in at least one wave were not significant at the 5% level. Only the coefficient for high sleep disturbance for those who were caring in both waves remained significant at the 95% level after including variables in Model 3 (self-rated health, depressive symptoms, and time spent in paid work), which may be both confounders and mediators of the caregiving–sleep relationship.

Table 3

Sleep Disturbance in Relation to Changes in Informal Caregiving (Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health, N = 12253, Random Effects Models).

Caregiving at t–1 and t1 on sleep disturbance at t1, β (CI)aCaregiving at t−1 and t1 on change in sleep disturbance from t−1 to t1, β (CI)b
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 1Model 2Model 3
Caregiving (ref: no caregiving at either t−1 or t1)
No caregiving at t−1, caregiving at t10.13*** (0.08, 0.18)0.07** (0.03, 0.12)0.03 (−0.01, 0.08)0.03 (−0.02, 0.08)0.02 (−0.03, 0.07)0.01 (−0.04, 0.06)
Caregiving at t−1, no caregiving at t10.10*** (0.05, 0.14)0.05* (0.00, 0.10)0.02 (−0.02, 0.06)−0.07** (−0.11, −0.02)−0.07** (−0.12, −0.02)−0.08*** (−0.13, −0.04)
Caregiving at t−1 and t10.18*** (0.12, 0.23)0.10*** (0.04, 0.15)0.05* (0.00, 0.09)0.00 (−0.04, 0.04)−0.00 (−0.05, 0.04)−0.02 (−0.06, 0.02)
Caregiving at t–1 and t1 on sleep disturbance at t1, β (CI)aCaregiving at t−1 and t1 on change in sleep disturbance from t−1 to t1, β (CI)b
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 1Model 2Model 3
Caregiving (ref: no caregiving at either t−1 or t1)
No caregiving at t−1, caregiving at t10.13*** (0.08, 0.18)0.07** (0.03, 0.12)0.03 (−0.01, 0.08)0.03 (−0.02, 0.08)0.02 (−0.03, 0.07)0.01 (−0.04, 0.06)
Caregiving at t−1, no caregiving at t10.10*** (0.05, 0.14)0.05* (0.00, 0.10)0.02 (−0.02, 0.06)−0.07** (−0.11, −0.02)−0.07** (−0.12, −0.02)−0.08*** (−0.13, −0.04)
Caregiving at t−1 and t10.18*** (0.12, 0.23)0.10*** (0.04, 0.15)0.05* (0.00, 0.09)0.00 (−0.04, 0.04)−0.00 (−0.05, 0.04)−0.02 (−0.06, 0.02)

CI = confidence interval.

*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Model 1 was unadjusted.

Model 2 included gender, age, age-squared, age-cubed, education level, marital status, pain, and chronic disease at the second of the two waves.

Model 3 additionally contained self-rated health, depressive symptoms, and hours in paid work at the second of the two waves.

aThe outcome is sleep disturbance at t1.

bThe outcome is change in sleep disturbance between t−1 and t1.

Table 3

Sleep Disturbance in Relation to Changes in Informal Caregiving (Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health, N = 12253, Random Effects Models).

Caregiving at t–1 and t1 on sleep disturbance at t1, β (CI)aCaregiving at t−1 and t1 on change in sleep disturbance from t−1 to t1, β (CI)b
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 1Model 2Model 3
Caregiving (ref: no caregiving at either t−1 or t1)
No caregiving at t−1, caregiving at t10.13*** (0.08, 0.18)0.07** (0.03, 0.12)0.03 (−0.01, 0.08)0.03 (−0.02, 0.08)0.02 (−0.03, 0.07)0.01 (−0.04, 0.06)
Caregiving at t−1, no caregiving at t10.10*** (0.05, 0.14)0.05* (0.00, 0.10)0.02 (−0.02, 0.06)−0.07** (−0.11, −0.02)−0.07** (−0.12, −0.02)−0.08*** (−0.13, −0.04)
Caregiving at t−1 and t10.18*** (0.12, 0.23)0.10*** (0.04, 0.15)0.05* (0.00, 0.09)0.00 (−0.04, 0.04)−0.00 (−0.05, 0.04)−0.02 (−0.06, 0.02)
Caregiving at t–1 and t1 on sleep disturbance at t1, β (CI)aCaregiving at t−1 and t1 on change in sleep disturbance from t−1 to t1, β (CI)b
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 1Model 2Model 3
Caregiving (ref: no caregiving at either t−1 or t1)
No caregiving at t−1, caregiving at t10.13*** (0.08, 0.18)0.07** (0.03, 0.12)0.03 (−0.01, 0.08)0.03 (−0.02, 0.08)0.02 (−0.03, 0.07)0.01 (−0.04, 0.06)
Caregiving at t−1, no caregiving at t10.10*** (0.05, 0.14)0.05* (0.00, 0.10)0.02 (−0.02, 0.06)−0.07** (−0.11, −0.02)−0.07** (−0.12, −0.02)−0.08*** (−0.13, −0.04)
Caregiving at t−1 and t10.18*** (0.12, 0.23)0.10*** (0.04, 0.15)0.05* (0.00, 0.09)0.00 (−0.04, 0.04)−0.00 (−0.05, 0.04)−0.02 (−0.06, 0.02)

CI = confidence interval.

*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Model 1 was unadjusted.

Model 2 included gender, age, age-squared, age-cubed, education level, marital status, pain, and chronic disease at the second of the two waves.

Model 3 additionally contained self-rated health, depressive symptoms, and hours in paid work at the second of the two waves.

aThe outcome is sleep disturbance at t1.

bThe outcome is change in sleep disturbance between t−1 and t1.

The right-hand set of analyses present change in caregiving from one wave to the next upon change in sleep disturbance over the same period. Compared with participants who did not provide informal care in either wave, providing caregiving at the previous but not the current wave was associated with a reduction in sleep disturbance over the same time period, results which changed little after adjustment for possible confounders and mediators (Model 2: β = −0.07; 95% CI: −0.12, −0.02 and Model 3: β = −0.08; 95% CI: −0.13, −0.04). Results from Wald tests showed that this group who provided care only at the first wave also reported a reduction in sleep disturbance in comparison to the other two caregiving groups ( compared with caregiving only at the second wave: χ2: 7.85, p = .005; compared with caregiving at both waves: χ2: 4.45, p = .035). Performing caregiving in both waves and providing care only at the current wave were not associated with change in sleep disturbance compared with the reference group composed of participants who did not provide care at either wave.

DISCUSSION

In this large sample of Swedish people in paid work, providing informal care predicted higher levels of self-reported sleep disturbance, particularly when more than 5 hours of care per week were provided, associations which were robust to inclusion of demographic and health covariates as well as time in paid work. These results correspond to previous research suggesting that informal caregiving impacts on self-reported and objective sleep22–24,47 and that providing informal care for a higher number of hours is more strongly associated with sleep disturbance.48,49 Studies have shown that provision of nighttime care is associated with poorer self-reported sleep,49 as is (albeit inconsistently) when carers and care recipients are coresident.50–52 It might be expected that providing relatively low intensity of informal care (up to 5 hours a week) would be associated with better sleep through the mechanism of role enhancement. However, in this Swedish working sample, even low intensity caregiving was associated with self-reported sleep disturbance, suggesting that in-work carers may be experiencing stress generated by role overload in managing the competing commitments of paid work and informal caregiving.

In terms of the stress process model, in which primary stressors generated by informal caregiving lead to problems in other domains, such as job-caregiving conflicts,53 such role overload may be generated due to the difficulty of apportioning time and energy to two relatively inflexible activities.54 In-work carers have partial control at best over the timing of caregiving and paid work activities, leading to scheduling difficulties (e.g., providing care at unpredictable times, at night, and during the working week), and the difficulty of catching up on sleep during the day due to the fixed timing of most jobs. Informal caregiving, unlike other unremunerated activities, is often performed out of a sense of moral obligation, particularly when the care recipient is a close family member, leaving some caregivers with little or no choice about engaging in caregiving.55 The impacts of caregiving upon sleep disturbance extend beyond the time taken for care tasks: caregivers may be on 24-hour call21,56 and the emotional labor involved in caregiving can generate substantial worry and distress. Accordingly, introduction of depressive symptoms and self-rated health into the models attenuated the relationship between provision of informal care and sleep disturbance. This result accords with prior research which has highlighted the comorbidity of insomnia symptoms with depression24 and self-rated health,57 and found cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between sleep and affect in caregivers.25,58,59

In contrast to a previous study which failed to find gender differences in the impact of caregiving upon sleeping problems,50 we found significant interactions between gender and informal caring in relation to sleep disturbance, specifically similar levels of sleep disturbance in men providing no caregiving and up to 5 hours of caregiving and much higher sleep disturbance among men providing more than 5 hours of care. Women had higher levels of sleep disturbance overall; differences among women by caregiving status were smaller than among men. It is possible that gender differences in sleep disturbance at moderate levels of caregiving are due to different tasks being performed by male and female carers, e.g., more personal care and routine tasks being performed by female carers.60,61 Higher intensity caregiving is more often spousal care, where gender differences in care tasks are smaller.60 It would be valuable for future research to examine gender differences in large samples, where information about the carer burden, who is cared for and the tasks performed, is available, in order to examine possible mechanisms generating gendered differences in the impact of caregiving upon sleep disturbance.

Relatively little prior research has examined the relationship between starting and stopping provision of informal care and sleep disturbance, an approach which provides additional evidence for the direction of effects. We found that, compared with not providing informal care in either wave, patterns of caregiving between two study waves that would correspond to beginning, terminating, or continuing to provide informal care were associated with higher sleep disturbance at the second of those study waves. The finding that reporting providing care only at the first wave was associated with sleep disturbance at the second wave is in line with qualitative research suggesting that sleep disturbance extends beyond the end of informal care provision.62

In analyses relating changes in sleep disturbance from one wave to the next to patterns of caregiving over the same period, individuals who provided informal care only at the first wave (which would correspond to ceasing caregiving) experienced improved self-reported sleep compared with the reference group of participants not providing informal care at either wave. These results point to cessation of caregiving generating reductions in sleep disturbance, even if not to the level of those who had not provided informal care at either wave. They stand in contrast to the sole earlier study examining transitions out of caregiving, which used a small sample of spousal Alzheimer caregivers and differentiated caregiving ceasing as a result of spousal institutionalization or death.26 That study, by von Känel et al., found no impact on self-reported or objective sleep measures of spousal institutionalization and a negative impact of spousal death on nighttime wakening and sleep time (measured with actigraph data), but no effect on self-reported sleep complaints.

Strengths and Limitations

The major strengths of this study are its use of a validated measure of sleep disturbance with good measurement properties in a longitudinal analysis of a large sample with a wide range of demographic and health covariates. In addition, the random effects model is a robust approach to dealing with unbalanced panels and missing data due to attrition, which is generally found in longitudinal analyses. Rather than having to assume that data are missing completely at random, random effects models make the less restrictive missing at random assumption, i.e., the propensity for data to be missing is not related to the missing data after accounting for relationships with observed data.63–65 However, the study has certain limitations: first, although the original sample of the SLOSH study was drawn from a representative sample of the Swedish population within the age-range 16–64 years (i.e., the Swedish Labour Force Survey), the findings are not generalizable to those working less than 30% of full-time or outside this age range. In addition, selective attrition from the SLOSH study has taken place, with SLOSH respondents more likely than nonrespondents to be older, female, married, Swedish-born, with university qualifications, and to work in the government sector. Second, it is possible that individuals with disturbed sleep are less likely to provide informal care, as a result of the impact of health conditions associated with disturbed sleep (e.g., depression and chronic pain). Although this would have a conservative effect on the results, weakening the caregiving–sleep disturbance associations, such reverse causation has been reduced by taking such health variables into account. Third, although we were able to control for a wide range of demographic and health variables, there may be residual confounding related to unobserved characteristics which could generate a spurious positive association between participation in informal care and sleep disturbance. Consequently, we performed a sensitivity analysis using fixed effect modeling, which partials out unobserved time-invariant individual differences as well as baseline differences in sleep disturbance by utilizing only information on changes in dependent and independent variables within individuals. The results are presented in Supplementary Table S1 and confirm the conclusions presented in the main analyses. Fourth, measures of care-giving were only provided at each wave; therefore, it was not possible to ascertain what was happening between the waves, e.g., whether a carer providing care at two consecutive waves had been providing care continuously during the 2-year period between those waves. Last, it was only possible to examine associations with weekly hours of care provision because the SLOSH study lacks information about the nature of tasks performed and characteristics of the care recipient (e.g., whether co-resident, nature of disability, or illness), an important avenue for future research in large, community-based samples.

Concluding Remarks

This study has found that among workers, providing informal care is an independent predictor of sleep disturbance after controlling for demographic and health variables. Even low-intensity caregiving, measured in hours per week, was associated with self-reported sleep disturbance. Sleep disturbance among carers is an under-recognized and under-treated problem,24 which is related to a variety of serious health outcomes, including major depressive disorder and physical health complaints. The data are from Sweden, a country with a welfare model aiming to minimize conflict between paid work and caring commitments among informal carers, although recent years have seen cuts to formal care services.66 A promising avenue for further research may be to investigate the caregiving–sleep disturbance association in diverse welfare and social care contexts.67 That a caregiving–sleep association was observed in a country with relatively comprehensive formal care provision suggests the need for policy actors everywhere to consider measures to support employed caregivers, concerning both government and employers, as well as to protect formal care services. Since it appears that informal carers are at a high risk of sleep disturbance, improved recognition and management by healthcare staff of sleeping problems in carers may also improve carers’ health and quality of life.

FUNDING

LBS was supported by the British cross-research council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing (LLHW) program under Extending Working Lives as part of an interdisciplinary consortium on Wellbeing, Health, Retirement, and the Lifecourse (WHERL) (ES/L002825/1). LGP received funding from the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (FAS, 2012-1743). SLOSH was supported by FAS (2005-0734) and the Swedish Research Council (VR, 2009-6192 and 2013-1645). The work was carried out within the framework of the Stockholm Stress Center, a FORTE Centre of Excellence (FORTE, 2009-1758).

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT

None declared.

WORK PERFORMED

Stress Research Institute, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We gratefully acknowledge advice from Göran Kecklund during the preparation of this manuscript.

REFERENCES

1.

Mallon
L
,
Broman
JE
,
Akerstedt
T
,
Hetta
J
.
Insomnia in Sweden: a population-based survey
.
Sleep Disord
.
2014
;
2014
:
Article ID 843126
,
1
7
.
doi:10.1155/2014/843126
.

2.

Hall
MA
,
Fernandez-Mendoza
J
,
Kline
CE
,
Vgontzas
AN
.
Insomnia and health
. In:
Kryger
M
,
Roth
T
,
Dement
WC
, eds.
Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine
. 6th ed.
Philadelphia, PA
:
Elsevier
;
2017
:
794
803
.

3.

Kecklund
G
,
Axelsson
J
.
Health consequences of shift work and insufficient sleep
.
BMJ
.
2016
;
355
:
i5210
.

4.

Luyster
FS
,
Strollo
PJ
Jr
,
Zee
PC
,
Walsh
JK
;
Boards of Directors of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and the Sleep Research Society
.
Sleep: a health imperative
.
Sleep
.
2012
;
35
(
6
):
727
734
.

5.

World Health Organization
.
The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines
. Vol
1
.
Geneva, Switzerland
:
World Health Organization
;
1992
.

6.

Rod
NH
,
Vahtera
J
,
Westerlund
H
et al.
Sleep disturbances and cause-specific mortality: results from the GAZEL cohort study
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
2011
;
173
(
3
):
300
309
.

7.

National Alliance for Caregiving
.
AARP Public Policy Institute
.
Caregiving in the U.S
.
2015
.
Washington, DC
:
National Alliance for Caregiving and AARP
;
2015
.

8.

Berglund
E
,
Lytsy
P
,
Westerling
R
.
Health and wellbeing in informal caregivers and non-caregivers: a comparative cross-sectional study of the Swedish general population
.
Health Qual Life Outcomes
.
2015
;
13
:
109
.
doi:10.1186/s12955-015-0309-2

9.

Agree
E
,
Glaser
K
.
Demography of informal caregiving
. In:
Uhlenberg
P
, ed.
International Handbook of Population Aging
. Vol
1
.
Netherlands
:
Springer
;
2009
:
647
668
.

10.

Buyck
JF
,
Ankri
J
,
Dugravot
A
et al.
Informal caregiving and the risk for coronary heart disease: the Whitehall II study
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
.
2013
;
68
(
10
):
1316
1323
.

11.

Choi
KS
,
Stewart
R
,
Dewey
M
.
Participation in productive activities and depression among older Europeans: survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe (SHARE)
.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2013
;
28
(
11
):
1157
1165
.

12.

Legg
L
,
Weir
CJ
,
Langhorne
P
,
Smith
LN
,
Stott
DJ
.
Is informal caregiving independently associated with poor health? A population-based study
.
J Epidemiol Community Health
.
2013
;
67
(
1
):
95
97
.

13.

Vlachantoni
A
,
Robards
J
,
Falkingham
J
,
Evandrou
M
.
Trajectories of informal care and health
.
SSM – Popul Health
.
2016
;
2
:
495
501
.
doi:10.1016/j.ssmph.2016.05.009

14.

Vlachantoni
A
,
Evandrou
M
,
Falkingham
J
,
Robards
J
.
Informal care, health and mortality
.
Maturitas
.
2013
;
74
(
2
):
114
118
.

15.

Pinquart
M
,
Sörensen
S
.
Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers in psychological health and physical health: a meta-analysis
.
Psychol Aging
.
2003
;
18
(
2
):
250
267
.

16.

Rozario
PA
,
Morrow-Howell
N
,
Hinterlong
JE
.
Role enhancement or role strain assessing the impact of multiple productive roles on older caregiver well-being
.
Res Aging
.
2004
;
26
(
4
):
413
428
.

17.

Schulz
R
,
Sherwood
PR
.
Physical and mental health effects of family caregiving
.
Am J Nurs
.
2008
;
108
(
9 Suppl
):
23
7
.

18.

Broese van Groenou
MI
,
de Boer
A
,
Iedema
J
.
Positive and negative evaluation of caregiving among three different types of informal care relationships
.
Eur J Ageing
.
2013
;
10
(
4
):
301
311
.

19.

O’Reilly
D
,
Connolly
S
,
Rosato
M
,
Patterson
C
.
Is caring associated with an increased risk of mortality? A longitudinal study
.
Soc Sci Med
.
2008
;
67
(
8
):
1282
1290
.

20.

O’Reilly
D
,
Rosato
M
,
Maguire
A
.
Caregiving reduces mortality risk for most caregivers: a census-based record linkage study
.
Int J Epidemiol
.
2015
;
44
(
6
):
1959
1969
.

21.

Arber
S
,
Venn
S
.
Caregiving at night: understanding the impact on carers
.
J Aging Stud
.
2011
;
25
(
2
):
155
165
.

22.

Kotronoulas
G
,
Wengstrom
Y
,
Kearney
N
.
Sleep patterns and sleep-impairing factors of persons providing informal care for people with cancer: a critical review of the literature
.
Cancer Nurs
.
2013
;
36
(
1
):
E1
15
.

23.

Fonareva
I
,
Oken
BS
.
Physiological and functional consequences of caregiving for relatives with dementia
.
Int Psychogeriatr
.
2014
;
26
(
5
):
725
747
.

24.

Peng
HL
,
Chang
YP
.
Sleep disturbance in family caregivers of individuals with dementia: a review of the literature
.
Perspect Psychiatr Care
.
2013
;
49
(
2
):
135
146
.

25.

Kochar
J
,
Fredman
L
,
Stone
KL
,
Cauley
JA
;
Study of Osteoporotic Fractures
.
Sleep problems in elderly women caregivers depend on the level of depressive symptoms: results of the caregiver–study of osteoporotic fractures
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2007
;
55
(
12
):
2003
2009
.

26.

von Känel
R
,
Mausbach
BT
,
Ancoli-Israel
S
et al.
Sleep in spousal Alzheimer caregivers: a longitudinal study with a focus on the effects of major patient transitions on sleep
.
Sleep
.
2012
;
35
(
2
):
247
255
.

27.

Etcher
L
.
Sleep disruption in older informal caregivers
.
Home Healthc Nurse
.
2014
;
32
(
7
):
415
419
.

28.

McCurry
SM
,
Song
Y
,
Martin
JL
.
Sleep in caregivers: what we know and what we need to learn
.
Curr Opin Psychiatry
.
2015
;
28
(
6
):
497
503
.

29.

Melchior
M
,
Berkman
LF
,
Niedhammer
I
,
Zins
M
,
Goldberg
M
.
The mental health effects of multiple work and family demands. A prospective study of psychiatric sickness absence in the French GAZEL study
.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol
.
2007
;
42
(
7
):
573
582
.

30.

Razavi
T
,
Clark
C
,
Stansfeld
SA
.
Work-family conflict as a predictor of common mental disorders in the 1958 British birth cohort
.
Longitud Life Course Stud
.
2015
;
6
(
3
):
264
278
.

31.

Magnusson Hanson
LL
,
Theorell
T
,
Oxenstierna
G
,
Hyde
M
,
Westerlund
H
.
Demand, control and social climate as predictors of emotional exhaustion symptoms in working Swedish men and women
.
Scand J Public Health
.
2008
;
36
(
7
):
737
743
.

32.

Nordin
M
,
Åkerstedt
T
,
Nordin
S
.
Psychometric evaluation and normative data for the Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire
.
Sleep Biol Rhythms
.
2013
;
11
(
4
):
216
226
.

33.

Akerstedt
T
,
Knutsson
A
,
Westerholm
P
,
Theorell
T
,
Alfredsson
L
,
Kecklund
G
.
Sleep disturbances, work stress and work hours: a cross-sectional study
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2002
;
53
(
3
):
741
748
.

34.

Hanson
LL
,
Åkerstedt
T
,
Näswall
K
,
Leineweber
C
,
Theorell
T
,
Westerlund
H
.
Cross-lagged relationships between workplace demands, control, support, and sleep problems
.
Sleep
.
2011
;
34
(
10
):
1403
1410
.

35.

Greenland
S
,
Pearl
J
,
Robins
JM
.
Causal diagrams for epidemiologic research
.
Epidemiology
.
1999
;
10
(
1
):
37
48
.

36.

Foley
D
,
Ancoli-Israel
S
,
Britz
P
,
Walsh
J
.
Sleep disturbances and chronic disease in older adults: results of the 2003 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Survey
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2004
;
56
(
5
):
497
502
.

37.

McCann
JJ
,
Hebert
LE
,
Bienias
JL
,
Morris
MC
,
Evans
DA
.
Predictors of beginning and ending caregiving during a 3-year period in a biracial community population of older adults
.
Am J Public Health
.
2004
;
94
(
10
):
1800
1806
.

38.

Lee
C
,
Gramotnev
H
.
Transitions into and out of caregiving: health and social characteristics of mid-age Australian women
.
Psychol Health
.
2007
;
22
(
2
):
193
209
.

39.

Quesnel-Vallée
A
.
Self-rated health: caught in the crossfire of the quest for ‘true’ health
?
Int J Epidemiol
.
2007
;
36
(
6
):
1161
1164
.

40.

Singh-Manoux
A
,
Martikainen
P
,
Ferrie
J
,
Zins
M
,
Marmot
M
,
Goldberg
M
.
What does self rated health measure? Results from the British Whitehall II and French Gazel cohort studies
.
J Epidemiol Community Health
.
2006
;
60
(
4
):
364
372
.

41.

Derogatis
LR
,
Lipman
RS
,
Rickels
K
,
Uhlenhuth
EH
,
Covi
L
.
The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL): a self-report symptom inventory
.
Behav Sci
.
1974
;
19
(
1
):
1
15
.

42.

Lipman
RS
.
Depression scales derived from the Hopkins Symptom Checklist
. In:
Sartorius
N
,
Ban
TA
, eds.
Assessment of Depression
.
Berlin, Heidelberg
:
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
;
1986
:
232
248
.
doi:10.1007/978-3-642-70486-4_22
.

43.

Magnusson Hanson
LL
,
Westerlund
H
,
Leineweber
C
et al.
The Symptom Checklist-core depression (SCL-CD6) scale: psychometric properties of a brief six item scale for the assessment of depression
.
Scand J Public Health
.
2014
;
42
(
1
):
82
88
.

44.

Au
N
,
Johnston
DW
.
Self-assessed health: what does it mean and what does it hide
?
Soc Sci Med
.
2014
;
121
:
21
28
.
doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.10.007

45.

Basner
M
,
Fomberstein
KM
,
Razavi
FM
et al.
American time use survey: sleep time and its relationship to waking activities
.
Sleep
.
2007
;
30
(
9
):
1085
1095
.

46.

Laird
NM
,
Ware
JH
.
Random-effects models for longitudinal data
.
Biometrics
.
1982
;
38
(
4
):
963
974
.

47.

Maltby
KF
,
Sanderson
CR
,
Lobb
EA
,
Phillips
JL
.
Sleep disturbances in caregivers of patients with advanced cancer: a systematic review
.
Palliat Support Care
.
2017
;
15
(
1
):
125
140
.

48.

Maher
J
,
Green
H.
Carers 2000
.
London
:
National Statistics, The Stationery Office
;
2002
.

49.

Wilcox
S
,
King
AC
.
Sleep complaints in older women who are family caregivers
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
.
1999
;
54
(
3
):
P189
P198
.

50.

Gibson
R
,
Gander
P
,
Alpass
F
,
Stephens
C
.
Effect of caregiving status on the sleep of older New Zealanders
.
Australas J Ageing
.
2015
;
34
(
3
):
155
159
.

51.

Kumari
M
,
Green
R
,
Nazroo
J
.
Sleep duration and sleep disturbance
. In:
Financial Circumstances, Health and Well-Being of the Older Population in England: The 2008 English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (Wave 4)
.
London
:
The Institute for Fiscal Studies
;
2010
:
178
226
.

52.

Simpson
C
,
Carter
P
.
The impact of living arrangements on dementia caregivers’ sleep quality
.
Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen
.
2015
;
30
(
4
):
352
359
.

53.

Pearlin
LI
,
Mullan
JT
,
Semple
SJ
,
Skaff
MM
.
Caregiving and the stress process: an overview of concepts and their measures
.
Gerontologist
.
1990
;
30
(
5
):
583
594
.

54.

DePasquale
N
,
Zarit
SH
,
Mogle
J
,
Moen
P
,
Hammer
LB
,
Almeida
DM
.
Double- and triple-duty caregiving men an examination of subjective stress and perceived schedule control
.
J Appl Gerontol
.
2016
.
doi:10.1177/0733464816641391. Epub ahead of print
.

55.

Burr
JA
,
Choi
NG
,
Mutchler
JE
,
Caro
FG
.
Caregiving and volunteering: are private and public helping behaviors linked
?
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
.
2005
;
60
(
5
):
S247
S256
.

56.

Creese
J
,
Bédard
M
,
Brazil
K
,
Chambers
L
.
Sleep disturbances in spousal caregivers of individuals with Alzheimer’s disease
.
Int Psychogeriatr
.
2008
;
20
(
1
):
149
161
.

57.

Washington
KT
,
Parker Oliver
D
,
Smith
JB
,
McCrae
CS
,
Balchandani
SM
,
Demiris
G
.
Sleep problems, anxiety, and global self-rated health among hospice family caregivers
.
Am J Hosp Palliat Med
.
2017
.
doi:10.1177/1049909117703643. Epub ahead of print
.

58.

von Känel
R
,
Mausbach
BT
,
Ancoli-Israel
S
et al.
Positive affect and sleep in spousal Alzheimer caregivers: a longitudinal study
.
Behav Sleep Med
.
2014
;
12
(
5
):
358
372
.

59.

McCrae
CS
,
Dzierzewski
JM
,
McNamara
JP
,
Vatthauer
KE
,
Roth
AJ
,
Rowe
MA
.
Changes in sleep predict changes in affect in older caregivers of individuals with alzheimer’s dementia: a multilevel model approach
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
.
2016
;
71
(
3
):
458
462
.

60.

Arber
S
,
Ginn
J
.
Gender differences in informal caring
.
Health Soc Care Community
.
1995
;
3
(
1
):
19
31
.

61.

Szebehely
M
,
Ulmanen
P
,
Sand
A-B.
Att Ge Omsorg Mitt I Livet: Hur Påverkar Det Arbete Och Försörjning? [Providing Informal Care in the Middle of Life: How Does It Affect Work and Livelihoods?]
.
Stockholm
:
Institutionen för socialt arbete
;
2014
.

62.

Corey
KL
,
McCurry
MK
.
When caregiving ends: the experiences of former family caregivers of people with dementia
.
The Gerontologist
.
2016
;
00
(
00
):
1
10
.
doi:10.1093/geront/gnw20

63.

Laird
NM
.
Missing data in longitudinal studies
.
Stat Med
.
1988
;
7
(
1-2
):
305
315
.

64.

Carrière
I
,
Bouyer
J
.
Choosing marginal or random-effects models for longitudinal binary responses: application to self-reported disability among older persons
.
BMC Med Res Methodol
.
2002
;
2
(
1
):
15
.

65.

Hu
FB
,
Goldberg
J
,
Hedeker
D
,
Flay
BR
,
Pentz
MA
.
Comparison of population-averaged and subject-specific approaches for analyzing repeated binary outcomes
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
1998
;
147
(
7
):
694
703
.

66.

Rostgaard
T
,
Szebehely
M
.
Changing policies, changing patterns of care: Danish and Swedish home care at the crossroads
.
Eur J Ageing
.
2012
;
9
(
2
):
101
109
.

67.

Plaisier
I
,
Broese van Groenou
MI
,
Keuzenkamp
S
.
Combining work and informal care: the importance of caring organisations
.
Hum Resour Manag J
.
2015
;
25
(
2
):
267
280
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Supplementary data

Comments

0 Comments
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.